Secondary Logo

Journal Logo

Perceived Discrimination Predicts Longer Time to Be Accepted for Kidney Transplant

Myaskovsky, Larissa1,2,3,10; Almario Doebler, Donna4; Posluszny, Donna M.2; Dew, Mary Amanda3,4,5,6; Unruh, Mark2; Fried, Linda F.1,2,7; Switzer, Galen E.1,2,3; Kim, Sunghee8; Chang, Chung-Chou H.2,4; Ramkumar, Mohan2,7; Shapiro, Ron9

doi: 10.1097/TP.0b013e318241d0cd
Clinical and Translational Research
Free

Background. Although end-stage kidney disease in African Americans (AAs) is four times greater than in whites, AAs are less than one half as likely to undergo kidney transplantation (KT). This racial disparity has been found even after controlling for clinical factors such as comorbid conditions, dialysis vintage and type, and availability of potential living donors. Therefore, studying nonmedical factors is critical to understanding disparities in KT.

Methods. We conducted a longitudinal cohort study with 127 AA and white patients with end-stage kidney disease undergoing evaluation for KT (December 2006 to July 2007) to determine whether, after controlling for medical factors, differences in time to acceptance for transplant is explained by patients' cultural factors (e.g., perceived racism and discrimination, medical mistrust, religious objections to living donor KT), psychosocial characteristics (e.g., social support, anxiety, depression), or transplant knowledge. Participants completed two telephone interviews (shortly after initiation of transplant evaluation and after being accepted or found ineligible for transplant).

Results. Results indicated that AA patients reported higher levels of the cultural factors than did whites. We found no differences in comorbidity or availability of potential living donors. AAs took significantly longer to get accepted for transplant than did whites (hazard ratio=1.49, P=0.005). After adjustment for demographic, psychosocial, and cultural factors, the association of race with longer time for listing was no longer significant.

Conclusions. We suggest that interventions to address racial disparities in KT incorporate key nonmedical risk factors in patients.

1Center for Health Equity Research and Promotion, VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System, Pittsburgh, PA.

2Department of Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.

3Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.

4Department of Biostatistics, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.

5Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.

6Department of Epidemiology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.

7Renal Section, VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System, Pittsburgh, PA.

8Duke Clinical Research Institute, Duke University Medical Center, Pittsburgh, PA.

9Department of Surgery, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.

This work was supported by the Veterans Administration (VA) Health Services Research and Development Department (HSR&D) Merit Review Entry Program 04-409, VA HSR&D Merit Award IIR 06-220, VA Center for Health Equity Research and Promotion (CHERP) Pilot Fund LIP 72-023, VA Veterans Integrated Service Network (VISN) 4 Competitive Pilot Project Funds XVA 72-034, National Institutes of Diabetes, Digestive, and Kidney Diseases (NIDDK) R01DK081325, and the University of Pittsburgh Center for Social and Urban Research Manners Faculty Development Award XNV 72-085.

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

The contents do not represent the views of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the United States Government.

Address correspondence to: Larissa Myaskovsky, Ph.D., Center for Health Equity Research and Promotion, VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System, 7180 Highland Drive (151C-H), Pittsburgh, PA 15206. E-mail: larissa.myaskovsky@va.gov

L.M. participated in research design, writing of the manuscript, and data analysis. D.A.D. participated in writing of the manuscript and data analysis. D.M.P. participated in writing of the manuscript and data analysis. M.A.D. participated in research design and writing of the manuscript. M.U. participated in research design and writing of the manuscript. L.F.F. participated in research design and writing of the manuscript. G.E.S. participated in research design and writing of the manuscript. S.K. participated in writing of the manuscript and data analysis. C.-C.H.C. participated in data analysis and writing of the manuscript. M.R. participated in research design and writing of the manuscript. R.S. participated in the writing of the manuscript.

Received 15 July 2011. Revision requested 28 July 2011.

Accepted 11 November 2011.

Although end-stage kidney disease (ESKD) in African Americans (AAs) is four times greater than in whites, AAs are less than one half as likely to undergo kidney transplantation (KT), the optimal treatment for ESKD (14). AA race is associated with a longer time to completion of the medical evaluation for KT (5), lower rates of preemptive listing for transplant (6), and longer waiting times for KT (2, 3, 7, 8). Differences persist, despite the 2002 altered allocation system (9). These disparities have been found in KT even after controlling for clinical factors such as comorbid conditions, dialysis vintage and type, and availability of potential living donors (5, 6, 1012). Therefore, studying nonmedical factors is critical to understand disparities in KT.

Healthcare attitudes and perceived racism in the healthcare system play important roles in AAs' health behaviors and outcomes for other diseases (1318). Evidence indicates that cultural factors including medical mistrust, perceived racism and discrimination, religious beliefs, and family influence play a key role in decision making about medical interventions and that these issues are critical to understanding disparities in KT. In the context of evaluation for a kidney transplant, perceived discrimination in healthcare settings, medical mistrust, and perceived racism may affect patients' health behavior by causing them to disengage from the healthcare system and fail to proceed with necessary testing. Alternately, patients' religious beliefs and their beliefs about family responsibility may influence whether patients seek assistance—including asking for a living donor volunteer—from their family or friends. However, these factors have received little attention in patients with ESKD. In fact, our recent review (19) found that few studies have directly examined cultural factors that might explain race differences in receiving a transplant. The majority of these studies examined cultural factors and transplant outcomes retrospectively or cross-sectionally and could not determine whether such variables are predictive of outcomes (11, 20, 21).

The influence of patients' psychosocial characteristics (i.e., social support, anxiety/depression levels, locus of control, and self-image) and transplant-related knowledge have been found to influence decisions about transplantation and donation in a number of previous studies (10, 2129) and are included in our study. Finally, in addition to basic demographic characteristics (e.g., gender, age, income) that have been found to be associated with receipt of transplant (30, 31), we controlled for medical factors including, whether patients were on dialysis pretransplant, medical comorbidities, and whether patients had any potential living donors when they were being evaluated for transplant (3032).

Although previous research has demonstrated that AA patients are not referred to transplant at the same rate as white patients, and that they do not complete the necessary steps in the clinical process leading up to referral at the same rate as white patients (7, 8, 10, 33, 34), this study is one of the first to examine racial disparities once patients have been referred for transplant evaluation (29). This time point, however, is critical to study because patients who have been referred to transplant evaluation have supposedly surmounted the barriers that would have prevented them from being referred to transplant in the first place, but racial disparities in completing evaluation and being accepted for transplant continue.

The goals of our study are to determine whether AA and white patients with ESKD undergoing evaluation for transplantation: (1) differ across cultural factors, transplant knowledge, and psychosocial characteristics; (2) differ in the rate of being accepted for KT; and (3) whether potential differences in time to be accepted for transplant are explained by nonmedical factors after controlling for demographics and medical factors.

Back to Top | Article Outline

RESULTS

Race Differences on Predictor Variables

Table 1 displays race comparisons for all predictor variables. We found significant race differences for occupation and family income (P<0.05), but found no significant differences in gender, marital status, age, education, or type of insurance coverage. A significantly larger proportion of AA participants were on dialysis than white participants, that is, they were less likely to have been referred for preemptive transplantation. All AAs received hemodialysis, whereas 86.7% of whites received hemodialysis and 13.3% received peritoneal dialysis. There was no significant race difference in Charlson Comorbidity Index or in participants' report of the availability of potential donors at their first or second interview. We found significant race differences in perceived discrimination, perceived racism, medical mistrust, and religious objection to living donor kidney transplantation (LDKT), but not in family loyalty. There were no significant race differences in transplant knowledge or psychosocial characteristics except in external locus of control.

TABLE 1

TABLE 1

Back to Top | Article Outline

Time to Acceptance

Figure 1 shows the Kaplan-Meier survival analysis curves for comparisons of time to be accepted for a kidney transplant by race. The survival curves indicate that AAs took a significantly longer time than whites to be accepted for a kidney transplant. The gap between white and AA transplant candidates in time to be accepted for kidney transplant was at least 1.5 years (P<0.001).

FIGURE 1

FIGURE 1

Back to Top | Article Outline

Predictors of Time to Be Accepted for a Kidney Transplant

Before proceeding with multivariable analyses, variables were examined for multicollinearity. We found that education, occupation, income, and insurance level were significantly correlated (P<0.02). Therefore, we excluded educational level and insurance in the final model because they were related to occupation and income but were not found to be associated with race (Table 1).

Table 2 shows the predictors of time to be accepted for a kidney transplant. Model 1 represents the effect of race without controlling for any of the other variables. It indicates that race was significantly associated with time to be accepted for kidney transplant. Race was found to have a time varying effect on acceptance for transplant. Specifically, we found that there was no difference between whites and AAs in time to acceptance by the first year of participant tracking, where most of the participants were accepted by this time. After the first year, whites took significantly less time than AAs to be accepted for a kidney transplant and that difference continued for the remainder of the time participants were tracked in the study.

TABLE 2

TABLE 2

Model 2 includes the other key variables and their independent effects on time to be accepted for transplant (while controlling for all other variables in the model). Those results indicate that race, while still having a time varying effect, was no longer significantly associated with time to acceptance. Instead, income, perceived discrimination, transplant knowledge, and religious objection to LDKT were independently and significantly associated with time to be accepted for kidney transplant (all P values<0.05). Those with a lower income took longer to get accepted for a kidney transplant than those with a higher income. Those who reported previously experiencing any discrimination in healthcare took significantly longer to be accepted for a transplant than those who reported not experiencing any discrimination. Those with more transplant knowledge took less time to be accepted for a kidney transplant than those with less transplant knowledge. Although there was no overall race difference in transplant knowledge, knowledge did predict race differences in time to be accepted for transplant. Of the 27 knowledge items, five showed significant race differences, including where a cadaveric kidney comes from, how long it takes for donors to return to their normal activities, whether dialysis patients live longer than transplant patients, whether donors are more likely to get kidney disease, and whether donors have to pay for testing and hospitalization related to donation. In all five items, AA participants had a higher error rate than white participants.

Finally, religious objection to LDKT was found to have a time varying effect on acceptance for transplant. Initially, participants with high religious objections to LDKT were less likely to be accepted for transplant than those with low objections; but, after 7 months, they were more likely to be accepted for kidney transplant. By 1 year, however, religious objection to LDKT no longer influenced being accepted for transplant.

Back to Top | Article Outline

DISCUSSION

This report demonstrated significant race differences on several cultural factors in patients as they begin the process of KT evaluation. AA patients reported experiencing more discrimination in healthcare, more perceptions of racism in healthcare, higher medical mistrust, and more religious objections to LDKT than did white patients. These results confirm previous research examining racial differences in experience with health care in other medical conditions (1318).

A critical finding of our work was that comorbidity, dialysis status, and availability of potential living donors were not associated with days to be accepted for transplant. This confirmed our hypothesis that medical factors alone could not explain the racial disparities. Also confirming national findings regarding racial disparities in KT (36, 34, 35), we found that AA ESKD patients took significantly longer to complete their medical work up before being presented to the transplant team and to be accepted for kidney transplants, than did white kidney disease patients. We believe that these disparities could be partially explained by the nonmedical predictors we examined.

It is of note that race ceased to be a significant predictor of being accepted for KT after controlling for cultural and psychosocial characteristics. We found that perceived discrimination in healthcare, less transplant knowledge, more religious objection to transplantation, and lower income explained the racial disparities observed in the time it took to be accepted for transplant. These results add to the existing research about racial disparities in transplantation which previously found significant effects of perceived discrimination (20), religious objection to transplantation (11, 21), and knowledge about transplantation (10, 25) in retrospective analyses of reasons for such disparities. Our prospective study validated these findings and found that these factors can account for the racial disparities. Thus, as in other healthcare domains, nonmedical factors may prove to be important considerations in efforts to reduce racial disparities in KT.

These findings may help transplant teams identify patients who may be at risk for longer evaluation times. Once patients are identified as at risk, one of several approaches may be chosen. For example, transplant teams may target at-risk patients for interventions that would ensure timely completion of the evaluation process (e.g., fast-track transplant evaluation that would involve the coordination of all of their evaluation testing in one place over a period of a few days, rather having to coordinate and complete all of the testing on their own). Another clinical approach would include increasing contact with patients who are identified as at risk by transplant staff to ensure that patients follow through on necessary testing to complete the evaluation. Transplant staff may have to facilitate scheduling of appointments or contacting third-party clinicians to ensure patients' timely completion of necessary testing. Yet another approach would entail the use of transplant recipients as trained peer mentors or navigators, who would help current transplant candidates to navigate the system to complete the required testing and be available to answer questions and lend culturally appropriate support (36, 37). Finally, transplant teams could enhance their own cultural competency to become better educated about the reasons that patients may take longer to complete evaluation. For instance, some patients may purposely choose to take longer to complete the process because they may need more time to decide whether to go through with transplantation. In this case, the team's goals would be to respect the needs of the patient and their culture, providing extra help with coordination of appointments and follow-up, and answering patients' questions to address their transplant concerns. To this end, transplant teams need to be trained to be more aware of patients' cultural differences and to approach patients with respect and care when it comes to helping them complete the evaluation process. Cultural competency training is available online through The Provider's Guide to Quality and Culture (38). This online interactive tutorial provides training, assesses skills, and provides profiles of major US ethnic groups, and is a joint project of Management Sciences for Health, the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, the Health Resources and Services Administration, and the Bureau of Primary Health Care.

Some study limitations deserve mention. First, because this study was conducted in one region, our findings may lack generalizability. However, our sample was comparable with the National Organ Procurement and Transplantation Network and United States Renal Data System databases in basic demographics were equivalent (i.e., race, gender, and age) (39, 40). We also did not include measures of adherence pretransplant. Future work should assess this factor to control for its effect on acceptance for kidney transplant.

Despite these limitations, we found that perceived discrimination in healthcare can be as much of a risk factor as race, income, or low transplant knowledge. Directions for future work include replicating our study with a larger sample and in other geographic regions, tracking the influence of the identified significant factors on long-term graft and patient survival, and patient quality of life posttransplant, determining how these factors may predict patient adherence to the posttransplant immunosuppression and health behavior regimen, and testing the suggested clinical interventions to reduce disparities in KT.

Back to Top | Article Outline

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study Design

This longitudinal study was approved by the University of Pittsburgh and VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System Institutional Review Boards and all participants provided written informed consent to a master's level research assistant. Participants completed two semistructured telephone interviews. The first interview (1 hr) was completed shortly after participants' first transplant clinic appointment. The second interview (20 min) was completed shortly after they were accepted or found ineligible for kidney transplant.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Study Sample

Participants for the study were recruited from the University of Pittsburgh Starzl Transplant Institute kidney transplant clinic and the VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System renal clinic. Inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) referral for kidney transplant; (2) no previous history of KT; (3) English speaking; and (4) age 18 or older. A total of 168 patients met these criteria and 127 of those agreed to participate and had usable data at both time points in the study, yielding a 76% response rate. Participants were evaluated for transplant between December 2006 and July 2007.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Interview Procedures and Measures

Our study variables were assessed with standard measures administered by semistructured interview and medical record review. The interviews were conducted by a qualified staff of four bachelor- or master-level survey professionals from the Survey Research Program at the University of Pittsburgh Center for Social and Urban Research. This group is independent of the transplant team, thereby assuring patient confidentiality and no coercion for interview completion. All interview data were entered directly on the computer with the use of a computer-aided telephone interview system; therefore, no recordings of the interviews were made. The measures described later describe the full content of the interview. For a copy of the interview, please contact the first author.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Demographics

Demographics (e.g., race, gender, age) were obtained by self-report during the interview.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Medical Factors

Medical Factors including, receipt of dialysis, dialysis type, and Charlson Comorbidity Index (32) were abstracted from medical records. The number of potential living donors available for evaluation at participants' first and second interviews was determined by asking participants to indicate how many living relatives and friends they had between the ages of 18 and 70 years, the age range of adult living kidney donors (41).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Cultural Factors

We measured participants' perceived discrimination with an adapted version of the perceived discrimination in health care measure (seven items; Cronbach's α=0.90) (1314, 42). We measured perceived racism in health care with four items based on the work of LaVeist et al. (43, 44), which assess the belief that racism is common in healthcare in general (Cronbach's α=0.76). We assessed medical mistrust with 18 items adapted from LaVeist's Medical Mistrust Index (4345). This index assesses the degree to which participants believe their hospital to be trustworthy, competent, and acting in the best interest of the patient (Cronbach's α=0.87). We assessed religious objection to LDKT with a revised subscale of the Organ Donation Attitude Survey (46), a measure to assess religious beliefs related to LDKT (e.g., I have religious objections to living donor KT; Cronbach's α=0.71). Finally, we assessed family influence with the 16-item Bardis Familism scale, a measure of kinship rights and obligations (47) (Cronbach's α=0.81).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Transplant Knowledge

Transplant knowledge was assessed with items adapted from the KT Knowledge Survey (48) and the Kidney Transplant Questionnaire (25, 49). This measure included 27 multiple choice and true-false items (Cronbach's α=0.71) that assessed participants understanding about kidney disease and transplantation (e.g., “A person can live with only one working kidney”).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Psychosocial Characteristics

Social support was measured with the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (50) (Cronbach's α=0.85). Emotional distress was assessed with the anxiety and depression subscales of the Brief Symptom Inventory (51) (Cronbach's α was 0.87 and 0.88, respectively). Self-image was assessed with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (52) and the Sense of Mastery Scale (53). The Self-Esteem Scale assesses patients' feelings of self-worth and self-respect (Cronbach's α=0.85), and the Sense of Mastery Scale assesses the degree to which participants feel they have personal control over the things that happened to them (Cronbach's α=0.76). Locus of control was assessed with the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control scale, Form C (54). We grouped locus of control into internal (Cronbach's α=0.68) versus external locus of control (Cronbach's α=0.79).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Outcome Variables

We conducted medical record review to determine the total number of days from first presenting to transplant clinic to being accepted or found ineligible for transplant.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Statistical Analysis

All data were examined for statistical assumptions. Because of non-normal distributions, age, education, occupation, income, cultural factors, transplant knowledge, and psychosocial characteristics were dichotomized as high or low using a median cutoff (except perceived discrimination was categorized as any/none). Locus of control was normally distributed.

Race differences were examined using chi-square tests or Fisher's exact test for categorical variables, and t tests or Wilcoxon rank sum tests for continuous variables. Kaplan-Meier survival curves were constructed to depict race differences in time to be accepted for transplant. Log-rank or Peto-Peto tests were used to assess the difference between these survival functions.

To determine which variables would be in the final model predicting time to be accepted for transplant, univariate analyses of each variable of interest, and the outcome were examined. Any variable associated with the outcome at a P less than 0.10 significance level was included in the multivariable model. This procedure ensured that only the variables that had the strongest association with time to be accepted for transplant would be included in the final model and would limit type 1 error. The proportionality assumption was evaluated before fitting the models. We considered nine predictors. Given our total sample size, we maintained an appropriate respondent-to-variable ratio within the recommended range of 10:1 to 15:1 (55).

To identify a model predicting time to be accepted for kidney transplant, the proportional hazards assumption was first assessed at P less than 0.05 significance level using Gray's test for each potential predictor (56). Because some of the predictor variables violated the proportional hazard assumption, we used Gray's time-varying coefficients survival regression model for the multivariable analyses (57). Two models were run for each outcome. Model 1 examined the between group differences in time to be accepted for transplant. Model 2 examined racial differences after controlling for demographics, cultural factors, transplant knowledge, and psychosocial characteristics.

Back to Top | Article Outline

REFERENCES

1. Alexander CG, Sehgal AR. Barriers to cadaveric renal transplantation among blacks, women and the poor. J Am Med Assoc 1998; 280: 1148.
2. Epstein AM, Ayanian JZ, Keogh JH, et al.. Racial disparities in access to renal transplantation clinically appropriate or due to under use or over use? N Engl J Med 2000; 343: 1537.
3. Young CJ, Gaston RS. African Americans and renal transplantation: Disproportionate need, limited access, and impaired outcomes. Am J Med Sci 2002; 323: 94.
4. Young CJ, Kew C. Health disparities in transplantation: Focus on the complexity and challenge of renal transplantation in African Americans. Med Clin North Am 2005; 89: 1003.
5. Weng FL, Joffe MM, Feldman HI, et al.. Rates of completion of the medical evaluation for renal transplantation. Am J Kidney Dis 2005; 46: 734.
6. Keith D, Ashby VB, Port FK, et al.. Insurance type and minority status associated with large disparities in prelisting dialysis among candidates for kidney transplantation. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol 2008; 3: 463.
7. Alexander C, Sehgal AR. Barriers to cadaveric renal transplantation among blacks, women and the poor. J Am Med Assoc 1998; 280: 1148.
8. Tankersley MR, Gaston RS, Curtis JJ, et al.. The living donor process in kidney transplantation: Influence of race and comorbidity. Transplant Proc 1997; 29: 3722.
9. UNOS. OPTN/UNOS Board Revises Kidney Policy to Boost Minority Transplants. 2002. Available at: www.unos.org. sAccessed October 27, 2011.
10. Ayanian JZ, Cleary PD, Keogh JH, et al.. Physician's beliefs about racial differences in referral for renal transplantation. Am J Kidney Dis 2004; 43: 350.
11. Ozminkowski RJ, White AJ, Hassol A, et al.. Minimizing race disparity regarding receipt of a cadaver kidney transplant. Am J Kidney Dis 1997; 30: 749.
12. Weng FL, Mange KC. A comparison of persons who present for preemptive and nonpreemptive kidney transplantation. Am J Kidney Dis 2003; 42: 1050.
13. Bird ST, Bogart LM. Perceived race-based and socioeconomic status (SES)-based discrimination in interactions with health care providers. Ethn Dis 2001; 11: 554.
14. Bird ST, Bogart LM. Birth control conspiracy beliefs, perceived discrimination, and contraception among African Americans: An exploratory study. J Health Psychol 2003; 8: 263.
15. Bird ST, Bogart LM, Delahanty DL. Health-related correlates of perceived discrimination in HIV care. AIDS Patient Care STDS 2004; 18: 19.
16. Krieger N. Discrimination and health. In: Berkman L, Kawachi I, eds. Social epidemiology. Oxford, Oxford University Press 2000, p 36.
17. Landrine H, Klonoff EA. The schedule of racist events: A measure of racial discrimination and a study of its negative physical and mental health consequences. J Black Psychol 1996; 22: 114.
18. Paradies Y. A systematic review of empirical research on self-reported racism and health. Int J Epidemiol 2006; 35: 888.
19. Myaskovsky L, Switzer GE, Crowley-Matoka M, et al.. Psychosocial factors associated with ethnic differences in transplantation. Curr Opin Organ Transplant 2007; 12: 182.
20. Klassen AC, Hall AG, Saksvig B, et al.. Relationship between patients perceptions of disadvantage and discrimination and listing for kidney transplantation. Am J Public Health 2002; 92: 811.
21. Holley JL, McCauley C, Doherty B, et al.. Patients' views in the choice of renal transplant. Kidney Int 1996; 49: 494.
22. Schweitzer EJ, Yoon S, Hart J, et al.. Increased living donor volunteer rates with a formal recipient family education program. Am J Kidney Dis 1997; 29: 739.
23. Rodrigue JR, Cornell D, Lin J, et al.. Increasing live donor kidney transplantation: A randomized controlled trial of a home-based educational intervention. Am J Transplant 2007; 7: 394.
24. Rodrigue JR, Cornell DL, Kaplan B, et al.. A randomized trial of a home-based educational approach to increase live donor kidney transplantation: Effects in blacks and whites. Transplantation 2008; 51: 663.
25. Waterman AD, Barrett AC, Stanley SL. Optimal transplant education for recipients to increase pursuit of living donation. Prog Transplant 2008; 18: 55.
26. Dew MA, Myaskovsky L, Switzer GE, et al.. Profiles and predictors of the course of psychological distress across four years after heart transplantation. Psychol Med 2005; 35: 1215.
27. Myaskovsky L, Dew MA, Switzer GE, et al.. Avoidant coping with health problems is related to poorer quality of life among lung transplant candidates. Prog Transplant 2003; 13: 183.
28. Lunsford SL, Simpson KS, Chavin K, et al.. Racial differences in coping with the need for kidney transplantation and willingness to ask for live organ donation. Am J Kidney Dis 2006; 47: 324.
29. Gordon EJ. Patients' decisions for treatment of end-stage renal disease and their implications for access to transplantation. Social Sci Med 2001; 53: 971.
30. Andreoni KA, Brayman KL, Guidinger MK, et al.. Kidney and pancreas transplantation in the United States, 1996–2005. Am J Transplant 2007; 7(5 Part 2): 1359.
31. Hariharan S, McBride MA, Cherikh WS, et al.. Post-transplant renal function in the first year predicts long-term kidney transplant survival. Kidney Int 2002; 62: 311.
32. Jassal SV, Schaubel DE, Fenton SSA. Baseline comorbidity in kidney transplant recipients: A comparison of comorbidity indices. Transplantation 2005; 46: 136.
33. Alexander GC, Sehgal AR. Why hemodialysis patients fail to complete the transplantation process. Am J Kidney Dis 2001; 37: 321.
34. Agodoa L, Eggers P. Racial and ethnic disparities in end-stage kidney failure—Survival paradoxes in African-Americans. Semin Dialysis 2007; 20: 577.
35. Young CJ, Gaston RS. Renal transplantation in Black Americans. N Engl J Med 2000; 343: 1545.
36. Whitley E, Valverde P, Wells K, et al.. Establishing common cost measures to evaluate the economic value of patient navigation programs. Cancer 2011; 117(15 suppl): 3616.
37. Natale-Pereira A, Enard KR, Nevarez L, et al.. The role of patient navigators in eliminating health disparities. Cancer 2011; 117(15 suppl): 3541.
38. Management Sciences for Health. The provider's guide to quality and culture. Available at: http://erc.msh.org/qualityandculture. Accessed October 27, 2011.
39. Organ Procurement and Transplantation Network. 2011. Available at: http://optn.transplant.hrsa.gov/data/. Accessed June 13, 2011.
40. United States Renal Data System. USRDS 2010 Annual data report: Atlas of chronic kidney disease and end-stage renal disease in the United States. Bethesda, MD, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases 2010.
41. Delmonico F, Council of the Transplantation Society. A report of the Amsterdam forum on the care of the live kidney donor: Data and medical guidelines. Transplantation 2005; 79(6 suppl): S53.
42. Williams DR. Race and health: Basic questions, emerging directions. Ann Epidemiol 1997; 7: 322.
43. Boulware LE, Ratner LE, Cooper LA, et al.. Understanding disparities in donor behavior: Race and gender differences in willingness to donate blood and cadaveric organs. Med Care 2002; 40: 85.
44. LaVeist TA, Nickerson KJ, Bowie JV. Attitudes about racism, medical mistrust, and satisfaction with care among African-American and white cardiac patients. Med Care Res Rev 2000; 57(suppl 1): 146.
45. LaVeist TA, Isaac LA, Williams KP. Mistrust of healthcare organizations is associated with underutilization of health services. Health Services Res 2009; 44: 2093.
46. Rumsey S, Hurford DP, Cole AK. Influence of knowledge and religiousness on attitudes toward organ donation. Transplant Proc 2003; 35: 2845.
47. Bardis PD. A familism scale. J Marriage Family 1959; 21: 340.
48. Murray LR, Conrad NE, Bayley E. Perceptions of kidney transplant by persons with end stage renal disease. ANNA J 1999; 26: 479.
49. Waterman AD, Stanley SL, Covelli T, et al.. Living donation decision making: Recipients' concerns and educational needs. Prog Transplant 2006; 16: 17.
50. Cohen S, Zdaniuk B. ISEL 12 Psychometric properties. 2006. Available at: http://www.psy.cmu.edu/∼scohen/. Accessed October 27, 2011.
51. Derogatis L, Spencer P. The Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI): Administration, scoring, and procedures manual. Clinical psychometric research. Baltimore, MD 1975.
52. Rosenberg M. Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ, Princeton University Press 1965.
53. Pearlin L, Schooler C. The structure of coping. J Health Soc Behav 1978; 19: 2.
54. Wallston KA, Stein MJ, Smith CA. Form C of the MHLC scales: A condition-specific measure of locus of control. J Personal Assess 1994; 63: 534.
55. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics [ed. 5th] Boston, MA, Allyn and Bacon 2007.
56. Gray RJ. A class of K-sample tests for comparing the cumulative incidence of a competing risk. Ann Statistics 1988; 16: 1141.
57. Gray RJ. Flexible methods for analyzing survival data using splines, with applications to breast cancer prognosis. J Am Statistical Assoc 1992; 87: 942.
Keywords:

Kidney transplantation; Disparities; Discrimination

© 2012 by Lippincott Williams & Wilkins