Journal Logo

Kidney Transplantation

Mediators of Socioeconomic Inequity in Living-donor Kidney Transplantation: Results From a UK Multicenter Case-Control Study

Bailey, Pippa K. PhD1,2; Caskey, Fergus J. MD1,2; MacNeill, Stephanie PhD1; Tomson, Charles R.V. DM3; Dor, Frank J.M.F. PhD4; Ben-Shlomo, Yoav PhD1

Author Information
doi: 10.1097/TXD.0000000000000986

Abstract

Living-donor kidney transplantation offers the best treatment in terms of life-expectancy and quality of life1,2 for most people with kidney failure. In the United Kingdom, there are no direct costs to an individual receiving a kidney transplant or donating a kidney, and potential donors are entitled to reimbursement from NHS England for loss of earnings, travel and carer costs.3 Despite this reimbursement system in the United Kingdom, socioeconomic deprivation is associated with reduced access to living-donor kidney transplantation4,5: individuals who have no educational qualifications are 45% less likely to receive a living-donor kidney transplant (LDKT) over a deceased-donor kidney transplant (DDKT) than those who have higher education qualifications.5 The same association has been demonstrated in the Netherlands,6 the United States,7 and Australia.8 A US Consensus Conference in 2014 on Best Practices in Live Kidney Donation concluded that the mechanisms behind these observed disparities must be understood to identify targets for intervention.9

This study is part of a mixed-methods program of research to understand why socioeconomically deprived people with kidney disease are less likely to receive an LDKT. We aimed to understand the reasons behind the observed socioeconomic inequity in access to living-donor kidney transplantation specifically, and not in access to transplantation in general.

Qualitative work identified that: (1) passivity, (2) disempowerment, and (3) perceiving a lack of social support were particularly important factors that prevented socioeconomically deprived individuals from accessing an LDKT.10 Qualitative interviews suggested that more socioeconomically deprived people with kidney failure are less involved in and less confident having discussion about their treatment, and they are less engaged in these discussion.10 This finding is consistent with research that has shown that more socioeconomically disadvantaged groups have lower levels of “patient activation,”11 a metric describing the “knowledge, skills, and confidence a person has in managing their own health and healthcare.”12 More socioeconomically deprived people also perceived a lack of social support and appeared to struggle to think of people who might be willing donors.10

We designed this questionnaire-based case-control study to further investigate and quantitatively evaluate our previous qualitative findings. We examined whether receipt of an LDKT was associated with: (1) an individual’s knowledge about living-donor kidney transplantation, (2) a person’s level of patient activation, and (3) the social support perceived by a person with kidney disease. We also investigated whether the above variables were associated with socioeconomic position within the control population of DDKTs. We then investigated the variables above as potential intermediaries in the causal pathway between socioeconomic deprivation and reduced odds of receiving an LDKT over a DDKT as these mediators might be more amenable to intervention than the socioeconomic position.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study Sites

The study was based at 14 hospitals in England and Northern Ireland: Belfast, Birmingham, Bristol, Cambridge, Epsom and St Helier, Guy’s and St Thomas’s, Imperial, Leicester, Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, Nottingham, Oxford, Sheffield, and St George’s.

Participants

We obtained from each hospital an anonymized list of all individuals who received kidney transplants between April 1, 2013 and March 31, 2017, stratified by LDKT and DDKT status. Individuals who were aged ≥18 years at the time of transplantation were eligible for participation. Individuals who lacked mental capacity according to the Mental Capacity Act 2005 were excluded. A person is defined as lacking capacity if they are unable to do 1 or more of the following: (1) understand information given to them, (2) retain that information long enough to be able to make a decision, (3) weigh up the information available to make the decision, and (4) communicate their decision. P.B. performed stratified random sampling using Stata 1513 to select on average 110 LDKTs and 110 DDKTs from each site, weighted by the number of transplants performed at each study site. Sex and 5-year age group strata matched sampling was used to try to ensure a similar sample distribution by age and sex. We calculated the study sample size using the patient activation variable.14 To detect a difference of 7 points between LDKT cases and DDKT controls (ie, 64 versus 57) at 90% power, 5% significance and a 1:1 ratio would require 85 subjects per group (170 total), and at least 850 if comparing across 5 socioeconomic strata/quintiles. A sample of 944 subjects would account for a predicted 10% missing data. This sample size allows detection of a far smaller difference (0.16 SD) for a dichotomous exposure or between 6% and 8% for a categorical outcome.14

Between October 2017 and November 2018, collaborators at study sites mailed paper questionnaires to participants. Questionnaires were accompanied by an invitation letter, a return postage paid envelope, and a patient information sheet outlining the potential risk and benefits of participating. Participants were advised that they may find the questionnaire-raised sensitive issues, and were offered an opportunity to discuss these further. We stated that taking part would not be of direct benefit, but that the information provided would help us to benefit other patients in the future. A website address was provided so that participants could complete the questionnaire online if preferred. Collaborators sent nonresponders a second questionnaire after 4–6 weeks. P.B. extracted anonymized data from returned paper questionnaires at the University of Bristol, and uploaded these onto a secure REDCap database.15

Questionnaire Content

We have previously reported questionnaire development and the findings of a single-center pilot study.14 As indicated in the introduction, original item generation was informed by themes arising from qualitative research10: (1) passivity, (2) disempowerment, and (3) perceiving a lack of social support. Written consent for the study was requested on the first page of the questionnaire.

Survey Tools

Social support was measured using the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List shortened version-12 items survey (ISEL-12).16-18 The ISEL-12 generates a total score (0–36) that describes overall perceived social support16; high scores indicate a greater level of perceived social support. The psychometric properties of the ISEL-12 have good validity and reliability, including in populations similar to our study population in terms of age, ethnicity and gender.17,18 Participants were asked to indicate the number of living relatives ≥18 years from a list (spouse/partner, parents, sisters/brothers, children, aunts/uncles, and first cousins) as a proxy for potential living-donor pool. Friends and colleagues were not included, as they contribute very small numbers to the donor pool: between 2006 and 2017 only 8% of the UK living donors were in this category (unpublished data provided by NHS Blood and Transplant to the first author P.B.). Participants were asked about the reasons why they thought their relatives could not donate (from a list of reasons including age, health, weight, location, financial cost, blood group, job, no one to care for them after donation, and free-text entry).

LDKT knowledge (scored 0–10) was measured using the living donation subscale of the Rotterdam Renal Replacement Knowledge Test survey19 which provides a validated and reliable measure of a patient’s knowledge of kidney disease and treatment options in clinic and research.20 The questions were in True/False format and higher scores indicate a greater knowledge of living-donor kidney transplantation.

An individual’s level of engagement in their health care was measured using Insignia Health’s 13-point patient activation measure (PAM).12 “Patient activation” is a behavioral concept that incorporates the themes emergent from the qualitative work of passivity, disempowerment, and limited knowledge. It is defined as “an individual’s knowledge, skill, and confidence for managing their health and healthcare.”12 The measure has good psychometric properties.21 Higher PAM scores indicate higher patient activation. The raw score (maximum 100) can be converted into 4 activation levels: (1) not believing activation important, (2) a lack of knowledge and confidence to take action, (3) beginning to take action, and (4) taking action.

Participant Demographics

We collected data on age, sex, marital status, ethnicity, religion, and individual-level socioeconomic data including education level and income. Ethnicity was coded using the UK’s Office for National Statistics 2011 census categories22: White; Asian/Asian British (Indian, Pakistani, Bangladeshi, and Chinese); Black/African/Caribbean/Black British; Mixed/Multiple (White and Black Caribbean, White and Black African, Any other Mixed/Multiple ethnic background); and Others (Arab, Any other ethnic group).

Statistical Analysis

We used multivariable logistic regression to look at the association of receipt of an LDKT (case) compared with DDKT (control) with a recipient’s socioeconomic position, perceived social support, patient activation, LDKT knowledge, and number of potential donors. We used 2 models: (1) unadjusted and (2) adjusted for potential confounders. We specified, a priori, potential confounders including sex, age, and ethnicity. For the model evaluating social support, the number of potential donors was included as a confounder. We used robust standard errors to account for clustering within renal centers. We tested for a priori interactions between the socioeconomic deprivation variables and age, sex, and ethnicity. We performed a complete case analysis and then undertook a sensitivity analysis using multiple imputation using chained equations to derive 40 imputed datasets per group, for the exposure variable and potential confounders and then combined using Rubin’s rules using the multiple imputation procedure in Stata 15.13

Mediation Analyses

We undertook mediation analyses to quantitatively evaluate LDKT knowledge, patient activation, and social support as potential intermediaries in the causal pathway between socioeconomic deprivation and reduced odds of receiving an LDKT over a DDKT. Directed acyclic graphs illustrating possible mediation models are provided in Figure 1A–F1C. We aimed to decompose the association between socioeconomic position (exposure) and living-donor kidney transplantation (outcome) into the direct effect of the exposure on the outcome measure (a) and the indirect mediated effect across each of the 3 proposed mediator variables (x, y and z).

FIGURE 1.
FIGURE 1.:
Mediation analyses models. A, Mediation model. B, This model assumes the multiple mediators are independent and that the mediated pathways are independent. C, This model allows mediator variables to affect other mediator variables. LDKT, living-donor kidney transplant.

Mediation analyses were performed in Stata 15 using the ldecomp command which applies the analytic approach by Buis,23 an extension of the decomposition method described by Eriksson et al24 for logit models. This method allows assessment of the effect of multiple mediators in 1 model and for the adjustment of confounders. The analyses were run allowing for interactions between the exposure variables and mediators.

Variables as Mediators

It is possible that 2 of the proposed mediator variables (LDKT knowledge and perceived social support) might change as a result of receiving a LDKT rather than because they act as mediators. In this scenario, in contrast to the directed acyclic graphs in Figure 1A–C, higher socioeconomic position would be associated with an increased likelihood of an LDKT over a DDKT, and the receipt of an LDKT itself would cause an increase in a participant’s LDKT knowledge and perceived social support (“reverse mediation”). However, this study is part of a mixed-methods program of research and follows on from previous qualitative research with people who had not received LDKT in which LDKT knowledge, perceived social support, and patient activation were identified as reasons for not receiving an LDKT. This qualitative study and subsequent quantitative questionnaire study together represent an exploratory sequential mixed methods design.25 In this approach, elaboration, enhancement, and quantification of the results of 1 method are sought with the results from the other method.26 The qualitative evidence is that these variables are mediators. In addition, before mediation analyses we investigated whether (1) LDKT knowledge, (2) patient activation, and (3) perceived social support were associated with socioeconomic position (exposure variable) within the control population of DDKTs. This demonstrated that socioeconomic position is associated with the mediators within the controls (SDC, http://links.lww.com/TXD/A247), which cannot be the result of receiving an LDKT. Higher socioeconomic position was associated with higher LDKT knowledge, greater levels of perceived social support, and greater patient activation within controls, who had not received the LDKT intervention (Tables S1–3, SDC, http://links.lww.com/TXD/A247). This provides quantitative evidence to support the first part of the mediation model in Figure 1A–C: that socioeconomic position is associated with the mediators before receipt of an LDKT.

While Human Leukocyte Antigen (HLA) sensitization is associated with access to transplantation in general, the Access to Transplantation and Transplant Outcome Measures (ATTOM) study found no evidence that HLA sensitization is related to likelihood of receiving an LDKT over a DDKT.5 There is evidence from the ATTOM study that Primary Renal Disease (PRD) can affect likelihood of LDKT over DDKT.5 When the incidence of specific diagnoses has been examined (eg, IgA nephropathy), associations with socioeconomic deprivation have been demonstrated.27,28 There is therefore some evidence to suggest that socioeconomic deprivation is associated with the development of specific PRDs, and that PRD can affect the likelihood of an LDKT. PRD is therefore another potential mediator (not measured in this study due to reliance on self-report in this questionnaire design), but it is not a confounder. HLA sensitization and PRD are therefore not confounders of the association between socioeconomic position (exposure) and living-donor kidney transplantation (outcome).

We received NHS Research Ethics Committee (Research Ethics Committee reference 17/LO/1602) and Health Research Authority approval. The clinical and research activities reported are consistent with the Principles of the Declaration of Istanbul as outlined in the “Declaration of Istanbul on Organ Trafficking and Transplant Tourism.”

RESULTS

One thousand two-hundred forty questionnaires were returned from 3103 patients (40% response). Three thousand one-hundred seventy-two individuals were sampled from anonymized lists of kidney transplant recipients from 14 hospitals, but 69 questionnaires were not received due to the sampled participant: (1) dying (death occurring or being recorded after sampling), (2) moving house, (3) not having mental capacity as judged by a relative on receipt of the questionnaire, or (4) being a hospital inpatient. LDKT recipients were more likely to respond than DDKT recipients (46% versus 34%) and women were more likely to respond than men (43% versus 37%) (Table S4, SDC, http://links.lww.com/TXD/A247). However, respondents did reflect the transplant population from which they were sampled (Table S5, SDC, http://links.lww.com/TXD/A247).

Overall, the proportion of missing data was small (<10% for all variables except income for which it was 36%) (missing data). With respect to the income variable, no pattern of missingness was observed with ethnicity (chi2, P = 0.90) or transplant type (chi2, P = 0.20) but maybe with sex (chi2, P = 0.003) and age (chi2, P = 0.001). Women were more likely to have missing/blank data. There was the suggestion of a U-shaped curve with age, in that the oldest and youngest age groups were more likely to have missing/blank data (including “would rather not answer”).

No evidence of interaction between the socioeconomic deprivation measures and sex, age, ethnicity was found, nor between age and ethnicity, and age and sex. There was a suggestion of interaction between sex and ethnicity (likelihood ratio test, P = 0.009). In white people, 56%–59% of both DDKT and LDKT recipients were men, while in nonwhite people 70% of DDKT and 53% of LDKT recipients were men. However, the number of nonwhite people in the study was very small (n = 171 nonwhite participants compared with n = 1026 white participants).

Participant characteristics by case-control status are reported in Table 1T1. As expected, a greater proportion of recipients of DDKTs were from lower socioeconomic and nonwhite groups when compared to recipients of LDKTs.

TABLE 1.
TABLE 1.:
Participant characteristics by case-control statusa

Exposure Variables

As previously reported, people with higher socioeconomic position were more likely to receive a LDKT (Table 2T2):

TABLE 2.
TABLE 2.:
Logistic regression analysis: likelihood of receiving an LDKT over a DDKT findingsa
  • University education: University education versus no university education adjusted odds ratio (aOR) 1.48 [95% confidence interval (CI), 1.18-1.84], P = 0.001.
  • Income: aOR per £1000 increase in salary OR 1.14 (95% CI, 1.11-1.17), P < 0.001.

Receiving an LDKT over a DDKT was associated with higher levels of social support [aOR per +1 ISEL-12 score 1.05 (95% CI, 1.03-1.08), P < 0.001], higher levels of patient activation [aOR per +1 PAM level 1.35 (95% CI, 1.24-1.48), P < 0.001], and greater LDKT knowledge [aOR per +1 point R3K-T score 1.59 (95% CI, 1.49-1.69), P < 0.001] after adjusting for potential confounders (Table 2).

The number of potential donors available to an individual was associated with the likelihood of receiving an LDKT over a DDKT [aOR per +1 potential donor 1.03 (95% CI, 1.02-1.04), P < 0.001] (Table 2), but the effect appeared to be small.

Only 11% of participants (n = 141) reported that financial concerns for the donor were a reason that their relatives could not donate to them. There was weak evidence that financial concerns were associated with likelihood of having an LDKT (OR 0.64, 95% CI, 0.42-0.97, P = 0.03), but financial concerns were not associated with participant socioeconomic position (Table S6, SDC, http://links.lww.com/TXD/A247).

The associations did not significantly differ between the complete cases analysis and the analyses with missing variables imputed (Table S7, SDC,http://links.lww.com/TXD/A247).

Mediation Analyses

Using the method described by Buis,23 we estimated the following: (1) the direct effect of socioeconomic position on receipt of an LDKT and (2) the indirect effect mediated through the 3 mediators under investigation (social support, patient activation, and LDKT knowledge) (Table 3T3).

TABLE 3.
TABLE 3.:
Total, direct, and indirect effects of socioeconomic position on receipt of an LDKTa,b

The 3 mediator variables together mediate approximately 48.5% [(95% CI, 12.7-84.3), P = 0.008] of the association between university education and access to transplantation, and 46.0% [(95% CI, 28.7-63.4), P < 0.001] of the association between income and access to transplantation.

DISCUSSION

We have demonstrated that the modifiable factors of an individual’s perceived social support, level of patient activation, and LDKT knowledge are associated with both the socioeconomic position of people with kidney disease and the receipt of an LDKT over a DDKT. We have also demonstrated that these variables may mediate approximately 50% of the well-described association between the socioeconomic position and receipt of an LDKT. This understanding suggests interventions that target these factors could improve access to living-donor kidney transplantation for socioeconomically disadvantaged individuals in the United Kingdom.

Social support and social networks are recognized as important social determinants of health.29 Lack of social support has been associated with adverse health behavior and poor health outcomes, especially among individuals from areas of high socioeconomic deprivation30,31: this study demonstrates this association among people with kidney disease.

While the perception of a lack of social support may reflect the accurate perception of a true lack of social support resulting from less strong social ties, it might indicate a misperception of the social support that is truly available to an individual, and therefore may be modifiable. A perceived lack of social support may deter individuals from engaging with their social network regarding possible living kidney donation, but recent research from the United States has also suggested that transplant providers exclude between 10% and 22% of transplant candidates from transplantation due to an assessment that they lack social support.32 Whether this judgment among healthcare professionals in the United Kingdom explains part of our observed association between social support and reduced access to an LDKT requires investigation.

A higher socioeconomic position was associated with a greater level of LDKT knowledge, which was associated with a greater odd of having an LDKT. However, in addition to information and knowledge, individuals require the confidence and skill to use this information, which is captured in the PAM.21 A recent cross-sectional survey from the United States found that how an individual perceived their transplant knowledge, and their confidence in this knowledge, was more important than their actual knowledge in pursuing an LDKT,33 which indicates that the added element of confidence is important in receiving an LDKT. Further evidence to support this comes from a scoping review of interventions to increase numbers of LDKTs34 which found that interventions of patient education alone did not result in an increase in the number of LDKTs.34 Increasing transplant knowledge must also be accompanied by increasing confidence and skill in using this knowledge to increase uptake of LDKTs.

Transplant recipients have been reported to have higher levels of activation compared to people on dialysis,35 but to our knowledge, this is the first time LDKTs recipients have been shown to have higher levels of patient activation than DDKT recipients. Uncertainties remain as to how best to increase an individual’s patient activation, and to what extent changes in patient activation result in improved clinical outcomes. Several studies have reported increases in patient activation in response to an intervention: these include tailored coaching,36 1-to-1 sessions and group discussion,37 information sheets plus counseling,38 and support preparing questions for a consultation.39 Some studies have failed to show any change in PAM as a result of the intervention,37 and only a small number of studies have demonstrated that increases in PAM change patient behavior or objective clinical outcomes, such as blood pressure.36

Improving equity in living-donor kidney transplantation has been highlighted as a UK and international research priority by patients and clinicians.40,41 In this study, we have provided evidence of mediators of the socioeconomic inequity in living-donor kidney transplantation, and thus targets to intervention. A recent scoping review identified an important gap in the literature for evidence-based strategies to increase LDKTs.34 The only intervention that has been shown to be effective in randomized control trials is a home-based patient and family education approach.42,43 Developed and trialed among disadvantaged populations in the United States42 and the Netherlands.43 Kidney patients and invited family members are visited at home by health workers who provide them with information on transplantation and donation, crucially engage the social network, and facilitate conversations about living kidney donation. The studies have reported a >20% increase in the number of LDKTs in the intervention versus control group.

Other home-based educational interventions are currently being evaluated in clinical trials, including the “Explore Transplant at Home” intervention in the United States.44 Other promising interventions have not yet been formally evaluated in a clinical trial: the use of patient advocates has been evaluated in a small single-center US observational study.45 In this intervention, a friend/relative/volunteer is trained as an advocate: someone willing to speak to other friends and family about LDKTs and donation on the patient’s behalf, with resources available to share with the patient’s social network. Although the study in the United States concentrated on advocating for patients in interactions with potential donors, this could be extended to advocate for patients in interactions with clinicians to help provide a “work-around” solution to low levels of patient activation.

This was a large, multicenter questionnaire-based case-control study. To our knowledge, this is the first study to look at social support, patient activation, and LDKT knowledge as mediators of the well-described socioeconomic inequity in living-donor kidney transplantation. The questionnaire used validated measures, no single-item measures were used, and before use the questionnaires were evaluated in cognitive interviews.14 The study used individual-level socioeconomic measures, and the proportion of missing data was small. However, this study has some limitations. First, although our response rate was reasonable for an unincentivized postal survey, and compared to the response rate of other postal surveys in the United Kingdom46,47 and the 47% response to a survey sent to Dutch and Swedish transplant recipients,48 there is a risk of self-selection bias. Recipients of LDKTs were more likely to respond to the questionnaire, and individuals with greater patient activation, social support, LDKT knowledge, and higher socioeconomic position might be expected to be more likely to participate.14 This could introduce an artifactual association or affect the strength of a true association due to collider bias. However, this bias is only an issue if there is evidence of an interaction between exposure and outcome affecting response (or nonresponse)49 which is not likely here. In addition, we also compared our findings to those from the ATTOM study (which had 72% participation), and found the same effect sizes between socioeconomic position and likelihood of an LDKT (Table S8, SDC, http://links.lww.com/TXD/A247) providing further evidence, our sample is fairly representative of the total population of such patients. Second, 13.8% of participants were from Black, Asian and Minority Ethnic (BAME) groups: this is not a surprising finding as in the United Kingdom between 2013 and 2017 BAME individuals comprised 27% of DDKT recipients and 17% of LDKT kidney transplant recipients,50 but study findings might not be transferrable to BAME groups. Third, the case-control design means it is impossible to prove causal associations between the variables explored and access to LDKTs. While reverse causation between living-donor kidney transplantation (over deceased-donor kidney transplantation) and socioeconomic position is not likely, recipients of LDKTs may have greater LDKT knowledge and perception of social support as a result of having an LDKT (“reverse mediation”). We do not believe that our findings are due to this effect for the following reasons. There is a priori evidence that the investigated variables do act as mediators (see above). Second, the observed socioeconomic differences observed in the control group would if anything be reduced if this was a secondary effect of receiving an LDKT, that is, lower socioeconomic recipients of an LDKT are more likely to increase their knowledge, for example, and hence this would actually attenuate the mediation effect we have observed rather than enhance it. For example, if the mediators were not associated with socioeconomic status, then adjustment would have no effect on the crude association between socioeconomic position and LDKT status. Finally, the strongest support for our findings comes from an intervention study from the Netherlands which demonstrated that increased knowledge and communication with one’s social network in disadvantaged renal patients increased numbers of LDKTs. This experimental evidence highlights the impact of altering these mediators and cannot be a consequence of receiving an LDKT.43 We were unable to measure PRD, which, as noted above, may be another mediator that explains part of the unexplained association between socioeconomic position and living-donor kidney transplantation.

A lack of LDKT knowledge, perceived social support, and low levels of patient activation are associated with a reduced likelihood of having an LDKT. In addition, they appear to be potentially modifiable mediators of the socioeconomic inequity in access to LDKTs. Future interventions that target these factors may improve access to living-donor kidney transplantation for disadvantaged individuals in the United Kingdom.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors would like to thank all the study participants, and the participating center collaborators who facilitated the study (Miss Sarah Heap, Dr Mysore Phanish, Dr Shafi Malik, Dr Aisling Courtney, Dr Adnan Sharif, Dr Nicholas Torpey, Dr Refik Gökmen, Dr Michael Picton, Dr Linda Bisset, Dr Edward Sharples, and Dr Simon Curran).

REFERENCES

1. Terasaki PI, Cecka JM, Gjertson DW, et al.High survival rates of kidney transplants from spousal and living unrelated donors. N Engl J Med. 1995;333:333–336.
2. Laupacis A, Keown P, Pus N, et al.A study of the quality of life and cost-utility of renal transplantation. Kidney Int. 1996;50:235–242.
3. NHS England Specialised Services Clinical Reference Group for Renal Services. Clinical commissioning policy: reimbursement of expenses for living donors. 2017. Available at https://www.england.nhs.uk/wp-content/uploads/2018/08/comm-pol-reimbursement-expenses-living-donors-v2.pdf. Accessed January 17, 2020.
4. Udayaraj U, Ben-Shlomo Y, Roderick P, et al.Social deprivation, ethnicity, and uptake of living kidney donor transplantation in the United Kingdom. Transplantation. 2012;93:610–616.
5. Wu DA, Robb ML, Watson CJE, et al.Barriers to living donor kidney transplantation in the United Kingdom: a national observational study. Nephrol Dial Transplant. 2017;32:890–900.
6. Roodnat JI, Laging M, Massey EK, et al.Accumulation of unfavorable clinical and socioeconomic factors precludes living donor kidney transplantation. Transplantation. 2012;93:518–523.
7. Axelrod DA, Dzebisashvili N, Schnitzler MA, et al.The interplay of socioeconomic status, distance to center, and interdonor service area travel on kidney transplant access and outcomes. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2010;5:2276–2288.
8. Grace BS, Clayton PA, Cass A, et al.Transplantation rates for living- but not deceased-donor kidneys vary with socioeconomic status in Australia. Kidney Int. 2013;83:138–145.
9. Rodrigue JR, Kazley AS, Mandelbrot DA, et al.American Society of TransplantationAmerican Society of TransplantationLiving donor kidney transplantation: overcoming disparities in live kidney donation in the US–recommendations from a consensus conference. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2015;10:1687–1695.
10. Bailey PK, Ben-Shlomo Y, Tomson CR, et al.Socioeconomic deprivation and barriers to live-donor kidney transplantation: a qualitative study of deceased-donor kidney transplant recipients. BMJ Open. 2016;6:e010605.
11. Lubetkin EI, Lu WH, Gold MR.Levels and correlates of patient activation in health center settings: building strategies for improving health outcomes. J Health Care Poor Underserved. 2010;21:796–808.
12. Hibbard JH, Mahoney ER, Stockard J, et al.Development and testing of a short form of the patient activation measure. Health Serv Res. 2005;406 Pt 11918–1930.
13. Stata StatisticalSoftware: Release 152017.College Station, TXStataCorp LLC
14. Bailey PK, Tomson CR, Ben-Shlomo Y.What factors explain the association between socioeconomic deprivation and reduced likelihood of live-donor kidney transplantation? A questionnaire-based pilot case-control study. BMJ Open. 2016;6:e012132.
15. Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, et al.Research electronic data capture (REDCap)–a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009;42:377–381.
16. Cohen S, Mermelstein R, Kamarck T, et al.Sarason IG, Sarason BRMeasuring the functional components of social support. Social Support: Theory, Research and Application. 1985The Hague, The NetherlandsMartinus Nijhoff73–94.
17. Merz EL, Roesch SC, Malcarne VL, et al.Validation of interpersonal support evaluation list-12 (ISEL-12) scores among English- and Spanish-speaking Hispanics/Latinos from the HCHS/SOL Sociocultural Ancillary Study. Psychol Assess. 2014;26:384–394.
18. Cohen SBasic psychometrics for the ISEL-12. 2008. Available at https://www.cmu.edu/dietrich/psychology/stress-immunity-disease-lab/scales/.doc/psychometrics-for-the-isel-12-item-scale.doc.
19. Timmerman L, Ismail SY, Luchtenburg AE, et al.Exploring knowledge about dialysis, transplantation, and living donation among patients and their living kidney donors. Int J Behav Med. 2015;22:580–589.
20. Ismail SY, Timmerman L, Timman R, et al.A psychometric analysis of the Rotterdam Renal Replacement Knowledge-Test (R3K-T) using item response theory. Transpl Int. 2013;26:1164–1172.
21. Hibbard JH, Stockard J, Mahoney ER, et al.Development of the patient activation measure (PAM): conceptualizing and measuring activation in patients and consumers. Health Serv Res. 2004;394 Pt 11005–1026.
22. Office for National Statistics. 2011 Census analysis: ethnicity and religion of the non-UK born population in England and Wales: 2011. 2015. Available at https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/culturalidentity/ethnicity/articles/2011censusanalysisethnicityandreligionofthenonukbornpopulationinenglandandwales/2015-06-18. Accessed January 17, 2020.
23. Buis ML.Direct and indirect effects in a logit model. Stata J. 2010;10:11–29.
24. Erikson R, Goldthorpe JH, Jackson M, et al.On class differentials in educational attainment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:9730–9733.
25. Creswell JW, Plano Clark VL. Designing and Conducting Mixed Methods Research2011.2nd edLos Angeles, CASAGE Publications
26. Greene JC, Caracelli VJ, Graham WF.Toward a conceptual framework for mixed-method evaluation designs. Educ Eval Policy Anal1989;11:255–274.
27. Ward MM.Socioeconomic status and the incidence of ESRD. Am J Kidney Dis. 2008;51:563–572.
28. McQuarrie EP, Mackinnon B, McNeice V, et al.The incidence of biopsy-proven IgA nephropathy is associated with multiple socioeconomic deprivation. Kidney Int. 2014;85:198–203.
29. Berkman L, Glass T.Berkman LF, Kawachi ISocial integration, social networks, social support, and health. Social Epidemiology2000New York, NYOxford University Press158–162.
30. Weyers S, Dragano N, Möbus S, et al.Poor social relations and adverse health behaviour: stronger associations in low socioeconomic groups? Int J Public Health. 2010;55:17–23.
31. Stringhini S, Berkman L, Dugravot A, et al.Socioeconomic status, structural and functional measures of social support, and mortality: the British Whitehall II cohort study, 1985-2009. Am J Epidemiol. 2012;175:1275–1283.
32. Ladin K, Emerson J, Berry K, et al.Excluding patients from transplant due to social support: results from a national survey of transplant providers. Am J Transplant. 2019;19:193–203.
33. Gupta N, Salter ML, Garonzik-Wang JM, et al.Actual and perceived knowledge of kidney transplantation and the pursuit of a live donor. Transplantation. 2014;98:969–973.
34. Barnieh L, Collister D, Manns B, et al.A scoping review for strategies to increase living kidney donation. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2017;12:1518–1527.
35. Gair R, Wong E, Stannard C, et al.FP363 Associations between patient activation, characteristics and outcomes. Nephrol Dial Transplant2018;33Suppl 1i153–i154.
36. Hibbard JH, Greene J, Tusler M.Improving the outcomes of disease management by tailoring care to the patient’s level of activation. Am J Manag Care. 2009;15:353–360.
37. Hibbard JH, Mahoney ER, Stock R, et al.Do increases in patient activation result in improved self-management behaviors? Health Serv Res. 2007;42:1443–1463.
38. Wallace AS, Seligman HK, Davis TC, et al.Literacy-appropriate educational materials and brief counseling improve diabetes self-management. Patient Educ Couns. 2009;75:328–333.
39. Deen D, Lu WH, Rothstein D, et al.Asking questions: the effect of a brief intervention in community health centers on patient activation. Patient Educ Couns. 2011;84:257–260.
40. Lentine KL, Kasiske BL, Levey AS, et al.KDIGO clinical practice guideline on the evaluation and care of living kidney donors. Transplantation. 2017;1018S Suppl 1S1–S109.
41. Karet Frankl F, Coward R, Gallagher H, et alUK Renal Research Strategy. UK Kidney Research Consortium (UKKRC). 2016. Available at https://www.kidneyresearchuk.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/KR9501-UKRRS-Booklet-V8-WEB.pdf. Accessed January 17, 2020.
42. Rodrigue JR, Cornell DL, Lin JK, et al.Increasing live donor kidney transplantation: a randomized controlled trial of a home-based educational intervention. Am J Transplant. 2007;7:394–401.
43. Ismail SY, Luchtenburg AE, Timman R, et al.Home-based family intervention increases knowledge, communication and living donation rates: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Transplant. 2014;14:1862–1869.
44. Waterman AD, Anderson C, Alem A, et al.A randomized controlled trial of explore transplant at home to improve transplant knowledge and decision-making for CKD 3-5 patients at Kaiser Permanente Southern California. BMC Nephrol. 2019;20:78.
45. Garonzik-Wang JM, Berger JC, Ros RL, et al.Live donor champion: finding live kidney donors by separating the advocate from the patient. Transplantation. 2012;93:1147–1150.
46. Robb KA, Gatting L, Wardle J.What impact do questionnaire length and monetary incentives have on mailed health psychology survey response? Br J Health Psychol. 2017;22:671–685.
47. Harrison S, Henderson J, Alderdice F, et al.Methods to increase response rates to a population-based maternity survey: a comparison of two pilot studies. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2019;19:65.
48. Slaats D, Lennerling A, Pronk MC, et al.Donor and recipient perspectives on anonymity in kidney donation from live donors: a multicenter survey study. Am J Kidney Dis. 2018;71:52–64.
49. Bartlett JW, Harel O, Carpenter JR.Asymptotically unbiased estimation of exposure odds ratios in complete records logistic regression. Am J Epidemiol. 2015;182:730–736.
50. NHS Blood and Transplant. Organ donation and transplantation data for black, Asian and minority ethnic (BAME) communities. Report for 2017/2018 (1 April 2013 – 31 March 2018). Available at https://nhsbtdbe.blob.core.windows.net/umbraco-assets-corp/12048/bame-organ-donation-and-transplantation-data-2017-18.pdf. Accessed January 17, 2020.

Supplemental Digital Content

Copyright © 2020 The Author(s). Transplantation Direct. Published by Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc.