Simulation and postsimulation debriefing have long been appreciated as linked steps in generating insights and clarifying lessons in experiential learning situations.1–4 In the experiential learning cycle, simulation can serve as an experience or experimentation period, and postsimulation debriefing is an opportunity for reflection, allowing learners to make a sense of the experience and determine how to apply lessons learned to future clinical performance.2,5 Yet, there are a variety of threats to learner engagement in simulation and debriefings, which can weaken the effectiveness of the experiential learning cycle. Learning can be impeded when (1) learners do not “buy in” to the simulation endeavor,6 (2) they find the fidelity of the simulation problematic, (3) they feel exposed by the simulation and debriefing in a way that threatens their professional identity,7 (4) they feel defensive discussing performance that falls short of a standard.8,9
So what can instructors do to help create a safe container, an environment where learners face professionally meaningful challenges and are held to high standards in a way that engages them but does not intimidate or humiliate them?
We suggest that establishing an environment where learners can enter a deep level of connection to their motivations, each other, and the instructors begins before the simulation starts. The notion of a thoughtful prebriefing, introduction, orientation, or other similarly entitled epoch occurring before a simulation has long been part of practice.10,11 However, the elements, rationale for each, and predicted effectiveness have not been blended into a set of promising practices, and we wish to do that here.
One crucial aspect of engagement in health care simulation and debriefings is risk taking in the service of learning. This focus on risk taking in the service of learning is guided by a diverse set of research findings that stress a willingness of the learner to go to their social and intellectual edges with a positive attitude.12 Manifestations of this engagement are what Edmondson13–15 calls learning-oriented behaviors: these include reflection on action, feedback seeking, speaking up about what one thinks, asking for help, testing ideas that might or might not be right, and reflecting on results. Engagement also includes what March16 calls experimentation and Elliot and Dweck17 call learning orientation, which both refer to the willingness to try and err at the edge of expertise or capacity, where knowledge and skills may or may not be sufficient to avoid mistakes. The edge of expertise is similar to the “zone of proximal development” where instructor assistance is needed to help the learner move to the next level.18 These studies suggest that participants willing to experiment and who hold a learning orientation can (1) tolerate practicing at the edge of their ability, within an unfamiliar and possibly confusing environment; (2) appreciate comprehensive feedback in the context of demanding professional standards; (3) willingly reflect on problems and skills that are new or challenging to them; (4) correct and repeat actions; (5) contemplate and learn from mistakes; and (6) tolerate not knowing the exact answers to complex questions.
Importantly, psychological safety may not completely mitigate feelings of interpersonal risk. Rather, it tends to create a setting where learners feel safe enough to embrace being uncomfortable. It creates a setting where learners can practice new or familiar skills without the burden of feeling that they will be shamed, humiliated, or belittled.
RATIONALE FOR A STRONG PRESIMULATION BRIEFING—THE ROLE OF PSYCHOLOGICAL SAFETY
The presimulation briefing (synonymous with the terms introduction, orientation, and prebriefing) for a simulation session or entire simulation course sets the tone for all that follows. Building on work in the field of organizational behavior that finds that the climate set by group leaders has a significant impact on group member engagement, we believe a well-crafted introduction in which instructors collaborate with learners to set goals and expectations can enhance participation and learning, minimize later complaints and disengagement, and reduce potential participant defensiveness and resentment during the simulation and debriefing.
Although learning is often seen primarily as a cognitive task, it has deep psychological and emotional foundations as well. The psychological foundations of learning from experience involve containing or reducing feelings of insecurity and threat while nurturing feelings of well-being and possibility.19–22 With origins in psychoanalytic disciplines, the metaphor of a safe container in which learners feel secure enough to be uncomfortable or trust that they will have help managing difficult feelings and anxiety has come to be recognized as an important feature of nurturing experiential learning.23,24 Furthermore, reducing threats to professional and social identity is increasingly recognized as the sine qua non of learning in groups.13
Our literature review suggests that psychological safety is a crucial concept in determining whether a safe container has been created. If one feels psychologically safe, then one feels that the current environment is conducive to interpersonal risk taking; learners feel that they will be viewed positively even if they make mistakes. Psychological safety has been demonstrated to be a precursor to learning-oriented behaviors such as asking questions, sharing one’s thinking, and asking for help.13,15 Psychological safety helps avert defensive behaviors triggered by feelings of personal threat such as obstructing and withdrawing; it can reduce elaborate false explanations known as “fancy footwork”25 and ego defenses such as mocking or denigrating the simulation activity.26 Psychologically safe simulation exercises are friendly to risk taking in the service of learning; people feel that it is acceptable, even desirable, to work at the edge of their expertise or capacity where mistakes are likely. The importance of this psychologically safe container is amplified by the fact that core professional skills closely associated with the construction of professional identity24,27,28 are in view with live observation and subsequent video. Paradoxically, creating a psychologically safe container does not mean completely avoiding the negative emotions associated with mistakes, which, in limited doses, can help motivate learning.29
Being observed by others usually increases physiologic activation. This activation can enhance performance via a number of mechanisms30 including social facilitation a process by which people perform better under scrutiny. The physiologic activation associated with being observed can also degrade performance when such scrutiny triggers evaluation apprehension or is viewed as a threat.31–33
Although psychological safety has been extensively studied as a predictor of learning in groups, few studies have looked at the variables that create psychological safety. Leader inclusiveness, behaviors such as inviting input and listening to and acknowledging subordinates’ ideas (or at least not shutting them down), has been posited as a precursor to psychological safety.14,15 There are, we hypothesize, practices such as these that are within an instructor’s control and might contribute to psychological safety.
METHODS FOR IDENTIFYING PROMISING PRACTICES
We have identified and structured practices useful in presimulation briefing through 3 inputs: (1) a synthesis of existing theory and research in fields closely related to simulation and debriefing; (2) from the exercise of developing an assessment of health care simulation briefing and debriefing34; and (3) the authors’ collective experience in conducting more than 6000 debriefings, hundreds of presimulation course briefings, as well as roughly 2000 instances of coaching other simulation instructors on the flow from prebriefing to simulation to debriefing.
Input From a Literature Review
Working on the premise that research findings and theory from domains closely related to simulation plus debriefing logically transfer, we identified and synthesized findings, constructs, and theory from aviation simulation, clinical learning and teaching, formative assessment, adult learning, experiential learning, organizational learning, deliberate practice, and the cognitive, emotional, and behavioral bases for mobilizing change in adults.1,5,10,13,25,35–38
Consistent with a nascent set of recommendations on how to conduct literature reviews to synthesize complex evidence, we used a systematic, but nonprotocolized literature review.39,40 We read and hand searched through references in articles related to debriefing, reflective practice, and learning in groups; we then asked 2 experts each in debriefing, psychological counseling, organizational learning, clinical and general education, and adult behavior change to provide 1 to 5 references that they thought relevant for creating a context for learning and change. From these sources, we were able to identify key words and search terms that we provided to medical and social science librarians at our university. The librarians helped us adapt these to different clinical and social science databases and find additional articles and books. We read these articles and, through citation tracking, pursued additional references that, in our judgment, seemed relevant. This process led to 78 articles we initially reviewed for this article (Appendix 1).
Structure From Developing a Behaviorally Anchored Rating Scale on Briefing and Debriefing
We also used the organizing structure provided by developing a behaviorally anchored rating scale on briefing and debriefing.34 Element 1 of the 6-element Debriefing Assessment for Simulation in Healthcare (DASH)41 assesses what instructors do or fail to do in a presimulation briefing to establish an engaging environment for learning (Table 1 and Appendix 2). In the following sections, we use the dimensions (subparts) of DASH Element 1 as the organizing rubric for the proposed practices.
TABLE 1: DASH Element 1 and Dimensions
Input From Our Own Experience
We have created, erred (sometimes significantly), and modified our presimulation briefings during a period of 20 years in the course of delivering hundreds of clinical crisis resource management courses for a variety of specialties, interprofessional teams, and levels of training from nursing and medical students through advanced practice professionals, primarily in the United States. In addition, we have observed the variance in the prebriefs of hundreds of simulation instructors-in-training from North America, Oceania, Europe, South America, Central America, and Asia. The prebriefing behaviors captured in Appendix 2—identified through research for the DASH behaviorally anchored rating scale—are emblematic of some of our own as well as other people’s errors and good practices; they reflect 2 ends of the variance in practice that we have observed in our instructor courses.
PROMISING PRACTICES FOR PRESIMULATION BRIEFING
Creating psychological safety is an abstract goal that instructors can move toward in collaboration with their learners through a set of discrete, concrete activities. The specific practices we have identified within a presimulation briefing are setting clear boundaries, expectations, and goals; establishing a fiction contract; attending to logistic details; and conveying respect for the learner and interest in their perspective.
Clarifying Objectives, Environment, Roles, Confidentiality, and Expectations
Educational and psychological research agree that when learners have a sense of control and clarity about what is expected of them and what to expect from those in authority—provided it is benign—they are more likely engage.10,42,43 Clarity about what is expected in a simulation and debriefing also increases learners’ ability to meet those expectations.10 Because the simulation etiquette, norms, and roles may be unfamiliar to learners, it is incumbent on simulation instructors to clarify them.
Although instructors may have a clear vision about the goals of the simulation and debriefing encounter, they may make the common and natural mistake of assuming that the learners see it the same way.44 Explicitly clarifying learning objectives, actively exploring learners’ objectives, explaining or demonstrating the properties of the simulators, explaining the process and timing of the debriefing or other postsimulation analysis activities, and creating shared agreements with learners regarding the role of instructors and learners are helpful in bridging this gap.10,43,45,46
Furthermore, to the extent that clearly stated goals are inspiring for the learners, they can trigger the positive affect shown to stimulate increased openness to new ideas.47,48 Creating the conditions for this positive affect and openness is a boon when simulations plus debriefings are designed to stimulate reflection and the integration of new knowledge, ideas, or perspectives.
Formative Versus Summative Assessment?
Absolute clarity about how and if performance during the session will be evaluated is vital to establishing a safe container for learning during debriefing. Learners may worry that mistakes will openly or surreptitiously be held against them. Formative assessment, often known as evaluation for learning, is the process of identifying the learner’s current assets or deficits with respect to specific learning objectives and helping learners remediate the deficits and leverage the assets.49–51 Summative assessment, also known as assessment of learning, is usually a higher-stakes evaluation of whether the learner has achieved expected milestones and may determine whether they advance in their program of learning.38 How and if performance in the simulation bears on the learner’s advancement in a training program, licensure process, or maintenance of certification are likely to influence the climate of the debriefing because many learners feel that summative or formal evaluation is a threat rather than an aid.49,52 Thus, trust can be built by being clear, consistent, and transparent about the sort of evaluation that will be taking place.
Confidentiality/Privacy?
The boundaries regarding who might observe or be informed about learner’s performance in the simulation and debriefing can impact simulation behavior and debriefing conversation. Whether the exercise takes place in situ or in the simulation laboratory, instructors can further define the parameters of the learning environment and build trust by informing learners whether visitors, researchers, colleagues, patients, preceptors, or students will or will not be privy to their performance. The principle is that maximizing transparency about what and with whom information about simulation performance will or will not be shared builds trust (not that confidentiality alone is the only way to build trust53).
Establishing a “Fiction Contract” With Participants
Engaging in a simulated learning environment poses a unique challenge, that is, acting as if things are real.6 To immerse themselves into a scenario, learners must often be willing to play an active role, pretending to take care of real patient in a simulated setting where their professional skills are on display. The skilled instructor, like a novelist or playwright, attempts to create a fictional environment engaging enough to draw people in.6,54 Rather than assuming participants will or must accept the simulated environment, Dieckmann et al6 have suggested that instructors must create an explicit and collaborative agreement with participants, in which both instructors and learners have commitments. The fiction contract is a form of psychological contract that describes what instructors and learners owe each other and should expect of each other to have a successful encounter.55
To create a fiction contract, the instructor typically offers to do what can reasonably be done to make the situation as real as possible but acknowledges the limitations (eg, mannequin patient’s skin color does not change or does not feel or look real; invasive procedures cannot be performed on standardized patients). The instructor seeks a voluntary commitment from the learner to do what he or she can to act as if everything is real6 and conveys that the quality of the learning experience depends, in part, on the learner’s willingness to participate as fully as possible.
Building on the work of Dieckmann et al,6 we propose a model of how the fiction contract, along with the other practices we describe, impacts learner engagement. In this model, the fiction contract moderates and influences learners’ willingness to engage despite perceived lapses in realism (Fig. 1). Dieckmann et al argued that health care simulations have 3 kinds of fidelity, where fidelity describes how accurately reality is represented and we have adapted their terminology.57 Physical fidelity is the degree to which the simulation elements are sensed as approximating visual, tactile, auditory, and olfactory reality. Conceptual fidelity is the degree to which the simulation proceeds in a causally plausible manner. When the patient’s physiologic, pharmacologic, or emotional responses make sense for a given intervention, this is conceptual fidelity. Emotional/experiential fidelity is the degree to which the simulation generates the feelings learners would expect in a similar real situation. How the simulation unfolds to develop realistic time pressure, stress, happiness, or relief would be a property of emotional/experiential fidelity.
FIGURE 1: A model of fidelity, realism, and educational engagement in simulation.
56For a given participant in a simulation exercise, the 3 kinds of fidelity combine to produce a perception of realism for that individual. One person may perceive a certain degree of realism, whereas another may experience a very different degree of realism, both with the same simulation fidelity. Thus, realism is a property of the learner’s perception rather than a property of the simulation.
Based on their subjective perception of realism, we propose that an individual’s willingness and ability to engage in the learning experience varies. Moreover, the willingness to engage is affected by the ambient psychological safety of the whole education encounter of which the fiction contract is an important part.
By making explicit faculty’s interdependence with learners on buying in to the simulation, the fiction contract also plays a vital role in mitigating the occasional shame or humiliation learners may feel if they do not perform well in front of others. By revealing their own vulnerability in setting the fiction contract, the instructor invites collaboration: for example, “I have done everything I can to make this as real as possible, but in the end, it is not reality; I have to depend on you. I ask you to do your best to act as if this is real so that the time we have together is used to our best advantage.” Without this, learners who feel they have not done well in the simulation tend to blame the simulation for lapses in perceived realism as a way to combat the identity threats7,58 they feel when they do not perform as well as they would like. They may feel that these “unfair” (unrealistic to them) qualities of the simulation prevented their performing better.
Conversely, we have found in our own simulations that if learners feel that the instructors are playing fair with respect to fidelity and realism, they are more likely to focus on the learning objectives at hand and to willingly reflect on their own practice.
Attending to Logistic Details
To help participants focus on the demands of the simulation exercise, they need to know what to expect logistically. Simulation instructors can easily focus on the content or technical aspects of the simulation environment and ignore or minimize the logistical details of the exercise. An important part of creating engagement is “student-centered design” where clinician learners know how the structure of the session interfaces with their other professional or educational commitments.59 Attending to logistics can prevent the dissatisfaction that results when care and comfort issues, also known as “hygiene factors,” are handled poorly.60 As trivial as these issues may seem, covering appropriate details such as the starting and stopping time of the session, breaks, how to handle pages, texting, e-mail, social media, telephone calls, transportation, refreshments, whether they will have enough time to get to their next class or shift, and so on prevents distraction and worry and helps learners focus on issues within the curriculum.61 Demonstrating sensitivity to the logistic constraints of participants’ other duties—by, for example, asking about them—sends a subtle message that the instructor is aware of and cares about the learner’s competing commitments.
Conveying a Commitment to Respecting Learners and Understanding Their Perspective
When instructors convey that they value the learner’s perspective, it can have a powerful impact on learning.62 Three related theories from experimental economics, organizational behavior, and cognitive anthropology are relevant to how instructors convey respect and interest in learners’ thinking. First, learners are not simply doers of correct or incorrect actions; they construct meaning about the world around them. Learners, like the rest of us, sample the stream of experience around them and make sense of it, constructing categories to label their reality.63 This “sense making”64 shapes how they perceive reality and, in turn, how they act. When instructors communicate—by inquiring into their perspective, for example—that they see learners as “meaning makers,” not simply doers of correct or incorrect actions, it reveals a deeper interest in the learner.62 When instructors show they value their students’ internal sense-making processes, learners will be more likely to give weight to the role of their own thoughts and emotional processes to improve future performance.62,63
Second, working on the premise that people are actively constructing a view of reality, experimental economics finds that people are generally “intendedly rational”; they are trying to accomplish a valued goal given their current perceptions and analysis of the situation.65–69 Learners, even when they make mistakes, usually fit this assumption. When instructors indicate that they realize the learner was working toward a goal as best he or she could in the moment given their current sense-making processes, level of knowledge, level of stress, and so on, they convey respect and interest in the learner’s perspective. For example, instructors could convey that mistakes are puzzles that will reveal valuable information about learners’ meaning-making process rather than a crime to be covered up or punished.4,62
Third, psychotherapeutic theory, although it has different goals from debriefing, also concerns itself with transforming thinking, skills, and attitudes. The role of positive regard70 for other people—assuming they are capable of competent action and self-transformation—infuses the debriefing with a positive psychological tone. Recent research on the transformative features of evoking positive emotion indicates that its presence in debriefing can help spur learning.71,72
CONCLUSIONS
Through a review and synthesis of relevant concepts from literatures with bearing on presimulation briefings, we have proposed and discussed a set of promising practices that make up a sound presimulation course briefing and provide examples of these practices in Appendix 2. We cannot be sure that any one of the practices we have proposed or all of them together will always enhance engagement, but they are supported conceptually by previous research and theory, our primary research on how to assess precourse briefings and debriefing, and our experience in health care simulation. We have found them to be useful in structuring our own presimulation briefings.
The promising practices we have proposed can be adjusted to match the demands of different simulation contexts and stable or changing participant composition. The presimulation briefing would be adjusted depending on whether it is for a once-a-year teamwork training for medical flight or retrieval medicine group, a twice-a-week simulation laboratory within a prelicensure nursing course, or a monthly residency training program on interprofessional collaboration. Different aspects of these practices could be covered at different degrees of depth; a briefing might run from 3 to 5 minutes for a one-hour session, to 45 to 60 minutes for a day-long training. For example, first-time exposure to simulation might involve a much longer discussion of principles of formative assessment, more details of what is expected from both the learner and the instructor with respect to the fiction contract, and a more deliberate description of the progression of the exercise than would be required for repeat learners. The presence of learners from different specialties and disciplines who might be unfamiliar to each other in an interprofessional simulation session might merit a longer discussion of confidentiality to be sure that everyone is confident that those principles will be upheld. A simulation course with learners who have experienced simulation with the instructor in the same setting many times before might warrant just a quick reminder of the elements of psychological safety, fiction contract, confidentiality, and behavior.
We believe that a strong presimulation briefing begins the process of creating a safe container for learning that allows learners to tolerate and welcome direct and critical feedback, create opportunities to “redo” a skill, work outside their comfort zone, accept and deal with surprises, change their current clinical practice, recast their current ways of thinking, and validate themselves as professionals.
Creating and sustaining an engaging environment for learning relies on understanding and implementing the concept of a psychologically safe container.73,74 If well constructed, this container, like the nonreactive crucible used in chemistry experiments,75 allows instructors and learners to tolerate the “heat” of participating in simulations and debriefings to transform practice through experiential learning in a simulated environment.8,9 Based on the theory, research, and experience we synthesized for this article, we believe that 4 promising practices help learners participate actively in simulations, openly and rigorously analyze their performance in debriefing, and set the stage for improving clinical performance.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors thank Michaela Kolbe, Walter Eppich, and JWR’s peer review writing group, “Fulton 214” for their helpful comments on this article.
REFERENCES
1. Darling M, Parry C, Moore J. Learning in the thick of it.
Harv Bus Rev 2005; 83 (7): 84–92.
2. Baker AC, Jensen PJ, Kolb DA. In conversation: transforming experience into learning.
Simul Gaming 1997; 28: 6–12.
3. Lederman LC. Debriefing: toward a systematic assessment of theory and practice.
Simul Gaming 1992; 23 (2): 145–160.
4. Rudolph JW, Simon R, Dufresne RL, et al. There’s no such thing as a “non-judgmental” debriefing: a theory and method for debriefing with good judgment.
Simul Healthc 2006; 1 (1): 49–55.
5. Kolb DA.
Experiential Learning: Experience as the Source of Learning and Development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1984.
6. Dieckmann P, Gaba D, Rall M. Deepening the theoretical foundations of patient simulation as social practice.
Simul Healthc 2007; 2 (3): 183–193.
7. Williams M. Building genuine trust through interpersonal emotion management: a threat regulation model of trust and collaboration across boundaries.
Acad Manag Rev 2007; 32 (2): 595–621.
8. Mann K, van der Vleuten C, Eva K, et al. Tensions in informed self-assessment: how the desire for feedback and reticence to collect and use it can conflict.
Acad Med 2011; 86 (9): 1120–1127.
9. Sargeant J, Armson H, Chesluk B, et al. The processes and dimensions of informed self-assessment: a conceptual model.
Acad Med 2010; 85 (7): 1212–1220.
10. Dismukes RK, McDonnell LK, Jobe KK. Facilitating LOFT debriefings: instructor techniques and crew participation.
Int J Aviat Psychol 2000; 10: 35–57.
11. Howard S, Gaba D, Fish K, et al. Anesthesia crisis resource management training: teaching anesthesiologists to handle critical incidents.
Aviat Space Environ Med 1992; 63 (9): 763–770.
12. Skinner EA, Belmont MJ. Motivation in the classroom: reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year.
J Educ Psychol 1993; 85 (4): 572.
13. Edmondson A. Psychological safety and learning behavior in work teams.
Adm Sci Q 1999; 44: 350–383.
14. Edmondson A. Disrupted routines: team learning and new technology implementation in hospitals.
Adm Sci Q 2002; 46 (4): 685–716.
15. Edmondson AC. Speaking up in the operating room: how team leaders promote learning in interdisciplinary action teams.
J Manag Stud 2003; 40 (6): 1419–1452.
16. March JG. Exploration and exploitation in organizational learning.
Organ Sci 1991; 2 (1): 71–87.
17. Elliot AJ, Dweck CS.
Handbook of Competence and Motivation. New York, NY: Guillford; 2005.
18. Vygotsky L.
Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1978.
19. Bion WR. The psycho-analytic study of thinking. A theory of thinking.
Int J Psychoanal 1962; 43: 306–310.
20. Modell AH. “The Holding Environment” and the therapeutic action of psychoanalysis.
J Am Psychoanal Assoc 1976; 24 (2): 285–307.
21. Winnicott DW. Metapsychological and clinical aspects of regression within the psycho-analytical set-up.
Int J Psychoanal 1955; 36 (1): 16–26.
22. Bion WR.
Learning From Experience. 7th ed. London, England: Karnac; 2005 [1962].
23. French RB. The teacher as container of anxiety: psychoanalysis and the role of teacher.
J Manag Educ 1997; 21 (4): 483–495.
24. Gilmore S, Anderson V. Anxiety and experience-based learning in a professional standards context.
Manag Learn. 2012; 43 (1): 75–95.
25. Argyris C, Putnam R, Smith DM.
Action Science: Concepts, Methods and Skills for Research and Intervention. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 1985.
26. Argyris C.
Overcoming Organizational Defenses: Facilitating Organizational Learning. Wellesley, MA: Allyn and Bacon; 1990.
27. Pratt MG, Rockmann KW, Kaufmann JB. Constructing professional identity: the role of work and identity learning cycles in the customization of identity among medical residents.
Acad Manag J 2006; 49 (2): 235–262.
28. Ibarra H. Provisional selves: experimenting with image and identity in professional adaptation
Adm Sci Q 1999; 44 (4): 764–791.
29. Zhao N. Learning from errors: the role of context, emotion, and personality.
J Organ Behav 2011; 32 (3): 435–463.
30. Fisher S.
Stress and Strategy. London, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1986.
31. Blascovich J, Mendes WB, Hunter SB, et al. Social ‘facilitation’ as challenge and threat.
J Pers Soc Psychol 1999; 77 (1): 68–77.
32. Cottrell NB, Wack DL, Sekerak GJ, et al. Social facilitation of dominant responses by presence of others.
J Pers Soc Psychol 1968; 9: 245–250.
33. Zajonc RB. Social facilitation.
Science 1968; 149: 269–274.
34. Brett-Fleegler M, Rudolph J, Eppich W, et al. Debriefing assessment for simulation in healthcare: development and psychometric properties.
Simul Healthc 2012; 7 (5): 288–294.
35. Knowles MS, Holton EF, Swanson RA.
The Adult Learner: The Definitive Classic in Adult Education and Human Resource Development. 6th ed. Burlington, MA: Elsevier; 2005.
36. Watzlawick P, Weakland JH, Fisch R.
Change: Principles of Problem Formation and Problem Resolution. New York, NY: Horton; 1974.
37. Ende J. Feedback in clinical medical education.
JAMA 1983; 250 (6): 777–781.
38. Harlen W, James M. Assessment and learning: differences and relationship between formative and summative assessment.
Assess Educ 1997; 4 (3): 365–377.
39. Dixon-Woods M, Agarwal S, Jones D, et al. Synthesising qualitative and quantitative evidence: a review of possible methods.
J Health Serv Res Policy 2005; 10 (1): 45–53.
40. Greenhalgh T, Peacock R. Effectiveness and efficiency of search methods in systematic reviews of complex evidence: audit of primary sources.
BMJ 2005; 331 (7524): 1064–1065.
41. Brett-Fleegler M, Rudolph JW, Eppich WJ, et al. Debriefing Assessment for Simulation in Healthcare (DASH): assessment of the reliability of a debriefing instrument.
Simul Healthc 2009; 4 (4): 240–325.
42. Miller S. Why having control reduces stress: if I can stop the roller coaster, I don’t want to get off. In: Garber J, Seligman M, eds.
Human Helplessness: Theory and Applications. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1980: 71–95.
43. Dismukes RK, Smith GM.
Facilitation and Debriefing in Aviation Training and Operations. Aldershot, United Kingdom: Ashgate; 2001.
44. Smith DM.
Divide or Conquer: How Great Teams Turn Conflict Into Strength. New York, NY: Portfolio Penguin Group; 2008.
45. Issenberg BS, McGaghie WM, Petrusa ER, et al. Features and uses of high-fidelity medical simulation that lead to effective learning: a BEME systematic review.
Med Teach 2005; 27 (1): 10–28.
46. McDonnell LK, Jobe KK, Dismukes RK.
Facilitating LOS Debriefings: A Training Manual. Ames Research Center Moffett Field, California 94035-1000: NASA; 1997. DOT/FAA/AR-97/6.
47. Boyatzis RE, Smith ML, Blaize N. Developing sustainable leaders through coaching and compassion.
Acad Manag Learn Educ 2006; 5 (1): 8–24.
48. Fredrickson BL. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology.
Am Psychol 2001; 56 (3): 218–226.
49. Black P, William D. Assessment and classroom learning.
Assess Educ 1998; 5 (1): 7–74.
50. Bloom B.
Handbook on Formative and Summative Evaluation of Student Learning. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Book Company; 1971.
51. Rudolph JW, Simon R, Raemer DB, Eppich W. Debriefing as formative assessment: closing performance gaps in medical education.
Acad Emerg Med 2008; 15 (11): 1110–1116.
52. Hattie J, Jaeger R. Assessment and classroom learning: a deductive approach.
Assess Educ 1998; 5 (1): 111–125.
53. National Institute for Learning Outcomes Assessment. The Transparency Framework. 2012. Available at:
http://www.learningoutcomeassessment.org/TransparencyFramework.htm. Accessed December 10, 2012.
54. Eco U.
Six Walks in the Fictional Woods. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1994.
55. Rousseau DM.
Psychological Contracts in Organizations: Understanding Written and Unwritten Agreements. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1995.
56. Nanji KC, Baca K, Raemer DB. The effect of an olfactory and visual cue on realism and engagement in a health care simulation experience.
Simul Healthc 2013; 8 (3): 143–147.
57. Rudolph JW, Simon R, Raemer DB. Which reality matters? Questions on the Road to high engagement in healthcare simulation.
Simul Healthc 2007; 2 (3): 161–163.
58. Staw BM, Sandelands LE, Dutton JE. Threat-rigidity effects in organizational behavior: a multilevel analysis.
Adm Sci Q 1981; 26: 501–524.
59. Bransford JD, Brown AL, Cocking RR.
How People Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience and School. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000.
60. Herzberg F. One more time: how do you motivate employees?
Harv Bus Rev 1987; 67 (6): 4–16.
61. Miner JB.
Organizational Behavior I: Essential Theories of Motivation and Leadership. Armonk, New York: M.E. Sharpe; 2005.
62. Kegan R, Lahey LL.
How the Way We Talk Can Change the Way We Work. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2001.
63. Spradley JP.
The Ethnographic Interview. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Javanovich College Publishers; 1979.
64. Weick KE, Sutcliffe K, Obstfeld D. Organizing and the process of sensemaking.
Organ Sci 2005; 16 (4): 409–421.
65. Morecroft JDW. Rationality in the analysis of behavioral simulation models.
Manag Sci 1985; 31 (7): 900–916.
66. Kahneman D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk.
Econometrica 1979; 47 (2): 263–291.
67. Kahneman D, Slovic P, Tversky A.
Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 1982.
68. Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D.
Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 2002.
69. Tversky A, Slovic P, Kahneman D. The causes of preference reversal.
Am Econ Rev 1990; 80 (1): 204–217.
70. Rogers CR. The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change.
J Consult Psychol 1957; 21 (2): 95–103.
71. Jack A, Boyatzis RE, Khawaja M, Passarelli A, Leckie R. Visioning in the brain: an fMRI study of inspirational coaching and mentoring.
Social Neuroscience 2013; 4 (8): 369–384.
72. Fredrickson BL, Branigana C. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires.
Cogn Emot 2003; 19 (3): 313–332.
73. Freshwater D, Robertson C.
Emotions and Needs. Buckingham, United Kingdom: Open University Press; 2002.
74. Winnicott D.
The Family and Individual Development. London, England: Tavistock Publishers; 1965.
75. Schnarch DM.
Constructing the Sexual Crucible: An Integration of Sexual and Marital Therapy. New York, NY: WW Norton & Co; 1991.
APPENDIX 1 BIBLIOGRAPHY OF LITERATURE REVIEW ARTICLES BY TOPIC AREA
Psychological Safety or Safe Container
- 1. Bion WR. The psycho-analytic study of thinking. A theory of thinking. Int J Psychoanal 1962;43:306–310.
- 2.Bion WR. Learning From Experience. 7th ed. London, England: Karnac; 2005 [1962].
- 3. Bowen M. Family Therapy in Clinical Practice. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson; 1994.
- 4. Edmondson A. Psychological safety and learning behavior in work teams. Adm Sci Q 1999;44:350–383.
- 5. Edmondson A. Disrupted routines: team learning and new technology implementation in hospitals. Adm Sci Q 2002;46(4):685–716.
- 6. Edmondson AC. Speaking up in the operating room: how team leaders promote learning in interdisciplinary action teams. J Manag Stud 2003;40(6):1419–1452.
- 7. Edmondson AE. Learning from mistakes is easier said than done: group and organizational influences on the detection and correction of human error. J Appl Behav Sci 1996;32(1):5–28.
- 8. Epstein RS. Keeping Boundaries: Maintaining Safety and Integrity in the Psychotherapeutic Process. 1st ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc; 1994.
- 9. French RB. The teacher as container of anxiety: psychoanalysis and the role of teacher. J Manag Educ 1997;21(4):483–495.
- 10. Modell AH. “The holding environment” and the therapeutic action of psychoanalysis. J Am Psychoanal Assoc 1976;24(2):285–307.
- 11. Schnarch DM. Constructing the Sexual Crucible: An Integration of Sexual and Marital Therapy. New York, NY: WW Norton & Co; 1991.
- 12. Staw BM, Sandelands LE, Dutton JE. Threat-rigidity effects in organizational behavior: a multilevel analysis. Adm Sci Q 1981:26:501–524.
- 13. Williams M. Building genuine trust through interpersonal emotion management: a threat regulation model of trust and collaboration across boundaries. Acad Manag Rev 2007;32(2):595–621.
- 14. Winnicott D. The family and Individual Development. London, England: Tavistock Publishers; 1965.
- 15. Winnicott DW. Metapsychological and clinical aspects of regression within the psycho-analytical set-up. Int J Psychoanal 1955;36(1):16–26.
- 16. Zhao N. Learning from errors: the role of context, emotion, and personality. J Organ Behav 2011;32(3):435–463.
Clarifying Objectives, Environment, Roles, Confidentiality, and Expectations
- 17. Black P, William D. Assessment and classroom learning. Assess Educ 1998: 5(1):7–74.
- 18. Bloom B. Handbook on Formative and Summative Evaluation of Student Learning. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Book Company; 1971.
- 19. Boyatzis RE, Smith ML, Blaize N. Developing sustainable leaders through coaching and compassion. Acad Manag Learn Educ 2006;5(1):8–24.
- 20. Bransford JD, Brown AL, Cocking RR. How people Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience and School. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000.
- 21. Dismukes RK, McDonnell LK, Jobe KK. Facilitating LOFT debriefings: instructor techniques and crew participation. Int J Aviat Psychol 2000;10:35–57.
- 22. Dismukes RK, Smith GM. Facilitation and Debriefing in Aviation Training and Operations. Aldershot, United Kingdom: Ashgate; 2001.
- 23. Dismukes RK, Jobe KK, McDonnell LK. LOFT debriefings: an analysis of instructor techniques and crew participation. Moffett Field, CA: NASA Ames Research Center US National Aeronautics and Space Administration (NASA); 1997.
- 24. McDonnell LK, Jobe KK, Dismukes RK. Facilitating LOS Debriefings: A Training Manual: NASA;1997. DOT/FAA/AR-97/6.
- 25. Fredrickson BL. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. Am Psychol 2001;56(3):218–226.
- 26. Fredrickson BL, Branigana C. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cogn Emot 2003;19(3):313–332.
- 27. Harlen W, James M. Assessment and learning: differences and relationships between formative and summative assessment. Assess Educ 1997;4(3):365–377.
- 28. Hattie J, Jaeger R. Assessment and classroom learning: a deductive approach. Assess Educ 1998;5(1):111–125.
- 29. Issenberg BS, McGaghie WM, Petrusa ER, Gordon DL, Scalese RJ. Features and uses of high-fidelity medical simulation that lead to effective learning: a BEME systematic review. Med Teach 2005;27(1):10–28.
- 30. McDonnell LK, Jobe KK, Dismukes RK. Facilitating LOS Debriefings: A Training Manual. NASA; 1997.
- 31. Miller S. Why having control reduces stress: if I can stop the roller coaster, I don’t want to get off. In: Garber J, Seligman M, eds. Human Helplessness: Theory and Applications. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1980:71–95.
- 32. National Institute for Learning Outcomes Assessment. The Transparency Framework, Vol. 2012. Available at: http://www.learningoutcomeassessment.org/TransparencyFramework.htm. Accessed December 10, 2012.
- 33. Rudolph JW, Simon R, Raemer DB, Eppich W. Debriefing as formative assessment: closing performance gaps in medical education. Acad Emerg Med 2008;15(11):1110–1116.
- 34. Smith DM. Divide or Conquer: How Great Teams Turn Conflict Into Strength. New York, NY: Portfolio Penguin Group; 2008.
Establishing a Fiction Contract
- 35. Dieckmann P, Gaba D, Rall M. Deepening the theoretical foundations of patient simulation as social practice. Simul Healthc 2007;2(3):183–193.
- 36. Eco U. Six Walks in the Fictional Woods. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1994.
- 37. Kagin SL, Lusebrink VB. The expressive therapies continuum. Art Psychother 1978;5:171–180.
- 38. Kirlik A. Brunswikian theory and method as a foundation for simulation-based research on clinical judgment. Simul Healthc 2010;5(5):255–259.
- 39. Nanji KC, Baca K, Raemer DB. The effect of an olfactory and visual cue on realism and engagement in a health care simulation experience. Simul Healthc 2013;8(3):143–147.
- 40. Rousseau DM. Psychological Contracts in Organizations: Understanding Written and Unwritten Agreements. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1995.
- 41. Rousseau DM. The contracts of individuals and organizations. In: Staw LLCaBM, ed. Research in Organizational Behavior. Vol 15. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press; 1993:1–43.
Attends to Logistic Details
- 42. Bransford JD, Brown AL, Cocking RR. How People Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience and School. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000.
- 43. Herzberg F. One more time: how do you motivate employees? Harv Bus Rev 1987;67(6):4–16.
Conveying a Commitment to Respecting Learners and Understanding Their Perspective
- 44. Alexander PA, Murphy PK. The research base for APA’s learner-centered psychological principles. In: Lambert NM, McCombs BL, eds. How Students Learn: Reforming Schools Through Learner-Centered Education. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1998:25–60.
- 45. Boyatzis RE, Jack T, Cesaro R, Khawaja M, Passarelli A. Coaching with compassion: an fMRI study of coaching to the positive and negative emotional attractor. Acad Manag Proc 2010.
- 46. Fredrickson BL, Branigana C. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cogn Emot 2003;19(3):313–332.
- 47. Friedman VJ, Lipshitz R. Teaching people to shift cognitive gears: overcoming resistance on the road to model II. J Appl Behav Sci 1992;28(1):118–136.
- 48. Friedman VJ, Rogers T. Action science: linking causal theory and meaning making in action research. In: Reason P, Bradbury H, eds. Handbook of Action Research: Participative Inquiry and Practice. London, England: Sage Publications Inc; 2008:252–265.
- 49. Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D. Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 2002.
- 50. Kahneman D, Slovic P, Tversky A. Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 1982.
- 51. Kahneman D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 1979;47(2):263–291.
- 52. Neisser U. Cognition and Reality: Principles and Implications of Cognitive Psychology. San Francisco, CA: W.H. Freeman and Company; 1976.
- 53. Kegan R, Lahey L. How the Way We Talk Can Change the Way We Work. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2001.
- 54. Louis MR, Sutton RI. Switching cognitive gears: from habits of mind to active thinking. Hum Relat 1991;44(1):55–76.
- 55. Morecroft JDW. Rationality in the analysis of behavioral simulation models. Manag Sci 1985;31(7):900–916.
- 56. Rogers CR. The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change. J Consult Psychol 1957;21(2):95–103.
- 57. Spradley JP. The Ethnographic Interview. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Javanovich College Publishers; 1979.
- 58. Torbert WR. Learning From Experience: Toward Consciousness. New York, NY: Columbia University Press; 1972.
- 59. Torbert WR. The Power of Balance: Transforming Self, Society, and Scientific Inquiry. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1991.
- 60. Torbert WR, Associates. Action Inquiry: The Secret of Timely and Transforming Leadership. San Francisco, CA: Berret-Koehler Publishers; 2004.
- 61. Schön D. The Reflective Practitioner. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1983.
- 62. Schön D. Educating the Reflective Practitioner: Toward a New Design for Teaching and Learning in the Professions. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 1987.
- 63. Tversky A, Slovic P, Kahneman D. The causes of preference reversal. Am Econ Rev 1990;80(1):204–217.
- 64. Weick KE, Sutcliffe K, Obstfeld D. Organizing and the process of sensemaking. Organ Sci 2005;16(4):409–421.
Debriefing
- 65. Arora S, Ahmed M, Paige J, et al. Objective structured assessment of debriefing (OSAD): bringing science to the art of debriefing in surgery. Ann Surg 2012;256(6):982–988.
- 66. Baker AC, Jensen PJ, Kolb DA. In conversation: transforming experience into learning. Simul Gaming 1997;28:6–12.
- 67. Cheng A, Rodgers D, Van der Jagt E, Eppich W, O’Donnell J. Evolution the pediatric advanced life support course: enhanced learning with anew debriefing tool and a web-based module for PALS instructors. Pediatr Crit Care Med 2012;13(5):589–595.
- 68. Darling M, Parry C, Moore J. Learning in the thick of it. Harv Bus Rev 2005;83(7):84–92.
- 69. Dieckmann P, Molin Friis S, Lippert A, Ostergaard D. The art and science of debriefing in simulation: ideal and practice. Med Teach 2009;31(7):e287–e294.
- 70. Dismukes RK, McDonnell LK, Jobe KK. Facilitating LOFT debriefings: instructor techniques and crew participation. Int J Aviat Psychol 2000;10:35–57.
- 71. Fanning RM, Gaba DM. The role of debriefing in simulation-based learning. Simul Healthc 2007;2(2):115–125.
- 72. Lederman LC. Debriefing: toward a systematic assessment of theory and practice. Simul Gaming 1992;23(2):145–160.
- 73. Morrison JE, Meliza LL. Foundations of the After Action Review Process: US Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Science; 1999.
- 74. Raemer DB. Assessing and improving debriefing: a workshop using the Debriefing Assessment for Simulation in Healthcare (DASH). Clin Simul Nurs 2009;5(3):S12–S12.
- 75. Rudolph JW, Simon R, Dufresne RL, Raemer DB. There’s no such thing as a “non-judgmental” debriefing: a theory and method for debriefing with good judgment. Simul Healthc 2006 2006;1(1):49–55.
- 76. Savoldelli GL, Naik VN, Park J, Joo HS, Chow R, Hamstra SJ. Value of debriefing during simulated crisis management: oral versus video-assisted oral feedback. Anesthesiology 2006;105(2):279–285.
- 77. Savoldelli GL, Naik VN, Park J, et al. The value of debriefing in simulation-based education: oral versus video-assisted feedback. Simul Healthc 2006;1(2).
- 78. Welke TM, LeBlanc VR, Savoldelli GL, et al. Personalized oral debriefing versus standardized multimedia instruction after patient crisis simulation. Anesth Analg 2009;109(1):183–189.
From the Debriefing Assessment for Simulation in Healthcare (DASH) Rater’s Handbook: Simon R, Raemer D, Rudolph J. Rater’s handbook for the Debriefing Assessment for Simulation in Healthcare (DASH) Rater Version. 2009;http://www.harvardmedsim.org/debriefing-assesment-simulation-healthcare.php. Accessed March 14, 2014.
Development and psychometric properties of the DASH are described in: Brett Fleegler M, Rudolph J, Eppich W, Monuteux M, Fleegler E, Cheng, A, Simon R. Debriefing Assessment for Simulation in Healthcare: Development and Psychometric Properties. Simul Healthc. Oct 2012;7(5):288–294.
Copyright 2014 Center for Medical Simulation, Boston, MA, USA. All Rights Reserved.
APPENDIX 2 ELEMENT 1
Establishes an engaging learning environment
How well the debriefer or instructor introduces the simulation learning experience can set the tone for all that follows. Before any simulation begins, the instructor helps participants be clear about what is expected of them, and helps them understand the benefits and limits of the simulated clinical setting. The instructor informs trainees whether and how the case, event, or procedure will later be debriefed (i.e., discussed and analyzed) and whether the simulation will be recorded. The effective instructor makes plain that the focus is on learning, not on “catching” people in a mistake, and seeks to create an environment where participants feel safe, even inspired to share their goals, thought, or feelings about the upcoming simulation and debriefing.
Element 1 Dimensions
Clarifies course objectives, environment, roles, and expectations.
Simulation-based courses flow better and participants engage more when they understand 1) The goals and objectives: What do the instructors intend learners to get out of the session? What do learners hope to get out of the session? 2) Learner and instructor roles; 3) The simulation environment– what can they expect from the simulators and actors/embedded simulated people? Who will be observing? 4) Expectations: What level of performance is expected or encouraged? To what extent is their performance confidential? How will recordings of their performance be handled? Will there be research conducted during the session?; To what extent is summative and/or formative assessment involved in this course?
Positive, effective behaviors for this dimension include:
- Introducing oneself and inviting others to introduce themselves.
- Sharing and inviting others to share information about their personal qualifications and experience, background, and interest in and goals for the course.
- Presenting the session overview and learning objectives.
- Addressing confidentiality explicitly. Examples: “Your preceptors will (or will not) be getting a report of your performance here;” “What you say and do here stays here—can we all agree to this?”
- Explaining assessment: E.g. “There is no formal evaluation in this course. We will provide you feedback on what we observe only to assist you in developing your skills.” Or “Your ability to follow the central venous line (CVL) insertion protocol will be assessed using the hospital checklist and this will determine whether you will be allowed to insert a CVL on the unit.”
- Introducing and/or describing the simulators, ancillary equipment, location of supplies, role of actors/embedded simulated people, etc.
- Discussing the origins of the cases, procedures, or events to be simulated and why certain cases or tasks have been included in the session (e.g., they are part of a transition to practice curriculum; they are actual cases that had bad outcomes; they are high consequence, low frequency events amenable to practice, etc.).
- Explicitly stating expectations for participants’ roles in the simulation course and subsequent debriefing. Example: “Act as yourself in the simulations. Take care of the patients using the skills you have. Don’t pretend to be a different specialty or level of training.”
- Requesting that participants engage in debriefing discussions and attempt to be self-reflective. “One of the hard things about this course is reflecting on the thought processes behind what you do in the sim. I can help you with reflective process. We can see what you do, but not what you are thinking. Understanding the relationship between your thinking and performance is one of the most interesting things about this course. I hope you will engage with me in that process.”
- Stating etiquette rules for the simulation and debriefing: e.g. to be respectful, curious about others’ thoughts and actions.
- Explicitly encouraging people to speak up and allowing for respectful disagreement. “I may say something you disagree with or have a different perspective on. I welcome hearing different perspectives; so please speak up.”
- Explicitly describing the instructor’s role: examples: to facilitate discussion; commenting on performance based on expertise or observing similar events or simulations; acting as a resource on own area of expertise (e.g., New practice guidelines, procedural steps, PALS, ACLS; CRM; teamwork; clinical, breaking bad news, human factors); and ensuring that the training objectives are met.
- Negative, ineffective behaviors include:
- Starting the session without introducing oneself or others or explaining why introductions are not needed.
- Not mentioning objectives, roles, or expectations of the session or describing them in a too-vague or misleading way
- Not explicitly addressing matters of confidentiality such as who will be informed of participants’ performance while in the simulation.
- Not explaining whether the goal of feedback is to improve performance (formative assessment); or that evaluation has consequences for the learners’ advancement (summative assessment). E.g. Student: “Does this matter for our grades?” Instructor: “We’ll see.”
- Being vague or misleading in describing the simulation or debriefing.
- Ignoring or leaving no time or opportunity for student statements or questions about goals and expectation of the session.
- Making statements or using body language that appears to belittle the learners’ goals, questions or concerns about the session.
- Using language that implies that poor performance in the simulation will be held against the learner.
Establishes a “fiction contract” with participants.
The fiction contract is a joint agreement that debriefers and students create. In it, the instructor acknowledges that the simulation cannot be exactly like real life but agrees to make the simulation as real as possible within resource and technology constraints. The instructor invites learners to do their best to act as if everything were real but acknowledges that s/he is dependent on the learners’ participation. Conveying this interdependence is a way to build agreement on how the learning process will go and is part of an effective learning contract. It is a better approach than unilaterally decreeing that the learners shall “suspend disbelief.”
Positive, effective behaviors for this dimension include:
- Explaining that the instructor and participants have to collaborate to create an engaging simulation and learning environment
- Stating that the instructor‘s obligation is to do everything to make the simulation as real as possible within resource and technology constraints.
- Asking the participants to do their part to act, as best they can, as if the simulation were real, acknowledging that a participant will likely act differently in the simulation environment than in the real clinical environment.
- Stating a fair and balanced assessment of simulator strengths and weaknesses.
An example that includes some of the above behaviors is: . E.g. “We have done our best to make the simulations as real as possible, but when all is said and done it is a simulation and not exactly the same as real patient context. So I’m asking you to meet me half way and do your best to act as if it is real. I know you will likely not exactly the same as you would if it were a real patient, but we will still have lots of interesting things to discuss.”
- Modeling the practice of engaging with the simulated environment as if were real by talking to or taking care of a simulated patient during an orientation.
- Negative or ineffective behaviors include:
- Trivializing the challenges students face in “buying in” to the realism of the simulation.
- Stating or assuming that trainees should and will act the same way they would in the real clinical setting.
- Insinuating or stating that it’s the student’s fault if the simulation doesn’t seem real to them. “We judge you level of commitment by your ability to suspend disbelief.”
Attends to logistical details.
Although it may seem secondary, informing participants about logistical details and providing a physically comfortable environment helps them focus on learning.
Positive, effective behaviors for this dimension include:
- Making sure that the learning space or conference room is clean. When available, chairs, tables, white board, video, simulation devices, or other educational materials are orderly, clean, and ready when the participants arrive.
- Briefing participants on where the simulation will take place and how long it may last.
- Letting participants know about the availability of food and drink, transportation or logistical considerations, locations of bathrooms, etc.
- Informing participants about when and where the simulated case, procedure, or event is likely to be debriefed.
- Inquiring or stating provisions to make accommodations for allergies (e.g. latex) or physical disabilities. Offering the opportunity to speak privately with instructors about these issues.
Negative or ineffective behaviors include:
- Not orienting participants to course logistics and the physical environment.
- Ignoring or making light of trainees’ concerns about timing, location, or physical needs.
- Failing to address individuals’ potential challenges related to their particular physical circumstances (e.g., do they use a wheelchair or other device, do they have a latex allergy etc.)
Conveys a commitment to respecting learners and understanding their perspective.
Participants often worry that simulations are designed to expose their weaknesses or to humiliate them. To counter these notions, instructors should offer clear alternative interpretations. One alternative is to highlight the difference in stress and cognitive load inside versus outside the simulation; it is easy to see what needs to done when one is outside the simulation; much harder inside. A second alternative is for the instructor to convey that they assume the trainee has good intentions and are trying to do their best but will likely make mistakes along the way – which is perfectly all right because this is a good place to talk about improving our practice.
Positive or effective behaviors in this dimension include:
- Stating that he or she understands that trainees are trying to accomplish something positive, even when they make mistakes. Could include a comment like, “Mistakes are puzzles to be solved, not crimes to be punished.”
- Expressing a commitment to hold generous inferences about learners such as “We believe participants in our courses are intelligent, capable, and are trying to do their best to learn and improve.”
- Stating that learners’ goals and interests are important in the learning process: “Your goals and interests are important. What are some of the things you would like to get out of the session today?”
- Expressing interest in thought and emotional processes: “An important feature of simulation is that it allows us all to reflect on the thought processes that drive our practice. Though I can see what you do, I can’t know what you are thinking or feeling. I’ll do my best to share my thinking and I am also very interested in yours.”
- Normalizing and clarifying the difference in perspective inside versus outside of the simulation: “Research on cognitive load and stress tells us that it is often much more challenging to be in the simulation; on the outside things may seem obvious and the pathway clear, but in the sim it can be very challenging.”
Negative, ineffective behaviors include:
- Teasing, belittling, or ignoring participants’ expressions of anxiety.
- Threatening to expose inadequate knowledge, values, or skill
- Stating or implying that poor performance by trainees in the simulation is indicative of poor actual skills or will be held against them.
- Making demeaning comments about a student’s competence.
- Using a mean tone of voice and message to undermine a student’s aspiration to be a capable health care provider. “You really aren’t cut out for this profession, are you?”