Journal Logo

ORIGINAL ARTICLES

Well-Being and Arthritis Incidence: The Role of Inflammatory Mechanisms. Findings From the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing

Okely, Judith A. MSc; Weiss, Alexander PhD; Gale, Catharine R. PhD

Author Information
doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000480

Abstract

Erratum

In the article published in volume 79 of Psychosomatic Medicine on pages 742–748, the sample size was misstated in several places. The correct number of participants included in the sample is 5266. In the Abstract, Methods section, and Figure S1 (Supplemental Digital Content), it was incorrectly reported as 5622. In the Methods section and Figure S1, the number of excluded participants who had missing covariate data at wave 1 should be 2404, not 2048. Additionally, in Table S1, some participant characteristic numbers were incorrectly reported. Figure S1and Table S1 have been corrected in the Supplemental Digital Content file at http://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A395 .

Psychosomatic Medicine. 80(3):333, April 2018.

INTRODUCTION

After the finding that well-being is predictive of health outcomes such as disease risk and longevity (1–4), a number of studies have explored the possibility that well-being directly affects biological processes relevant to disease risk (5). Here, we focus on the link between well-being and inflammatory processes. Several cross-sectional studies have documented an association between high well-being or optimism and lower levels of inflammatory markers including interleukin 6, C-reactive protein (CRP) fibrinogen and homocysteine (6–11). These associations are not fully accounted for by differences in demographic factors, depressive symptoms, or health behaviors—suggesting that well-being may directly affect inflammatory systems, potentially, via prefrontal and limbic system pathways (5).

The link between well-being and inflammation may be clinically significant because elevated markers of inflammation in older adults are associated with a higher risk of disease and disability (12). However, the extent to which the association between well-being and inflammatory processes accounts for the relationship between well-being and disease risk remains to be explored. In the current study, we tested whether the association between high well-being and lower levels of inflammation translates into a reduced risk of one particular disease—arthritis.

We chose to examine the link between well-being and risk of arthritis for two reasons. Firstly, inflammation is implicated in the etiology and progression of rheumatoid and osteoarthritis (13–16). Thus, down regulation of inflammatory processes associated with high well-being could result in a reduced disease risk. Secondly, in a previous study, we found evidence of an association between well-being and arthritis risk (17). Using data from the Survey of Health Ageing and Retirement in Europe, we found that higher well-being was associated with a reduced risk of arthritis for a 9-year follow-up period. This association remained significant although attenuated after adjusting for demographic variables, depressive symptoms, comorbidities, and health behaviors—suggesting that additional (potentially psychobiological) mechanisms underlie the association between well-being and arthritis risk. Here, we tested this hypothesis; specifically, we examined whether the association between well-being and arthritis risk is partly mediated by the effect of well-being on biomarkers of inflammation.

The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) is a representative sample of men and women aged 50 years or older living in England. This data set includes measures of two inflammatory biomarkers that have previously been related to arthritis onset or progression: CRP (14,18) and fibrinogen (19). The ELSA data set also includes a measure of well-being (control, autonomy, self-realization and pleasure 19 [CASP-19]). Research into the association between well-being and health has been informed by three distinct measures of well-being: evaluative well-being (life satisfaction), hedonic well-being (feelings of joy or happiness), and eudemonic well-being (sense of purpose in life) (20). The CASP-19 is designed to assess hedonic and eudemonic well-being; higher CASP-19 scores have previously been associated with lower levels of CRP and fibrinogen in women (9), and an abridged (12-item) version of the CASP has been found to predict arthritis risk (17).

Our aim was to test whether levels of CRP or fibrinogen mediated the association between well-being and incident arthritis. The ELSA data set currently consists of six waves of data collection. We predicted that the association between well-being at wave 1 and incident arthritis (for the follow-up period) would be mediated by biomarker concentrations at wave 2. In addition, we predicted that change in well-being for the 6 waves would be associated with arthritis risk and that this association would be mediated by change in biomarker levels.

METHODS

Study Sample

ELSA participants are 50 years or older and were initially recruited from the Health Survey for England database in 1998, 1999, and 2001. At wave 1 (2002–2003), 11,391 core participants were recruited; since then, participants have been interviewed biennially. Refreshment samples drawn from the Health Survey for England were added at waves 3 and 4 to maintain the representation of people aged 50 to 75 years. Currently, there are six waves of data available (2002–2012). In addition to the main interview, blood samples were taken in waves 2, 4, and 6 during a separate nurse visit. Ethical approval for all ELSA waves was provided by the London Multicentre Research and Ethics Committee. All participants gave written informed consent (21).

A total of 5622 participants were included in our sample. Participants were excluded if they reported a history of arthritis or did not know whether their conditions had been diagnosed with arthritis at wave 1 (n = 3721) (we excluded these participants so that the well-being measure preceded arthritis diagnosis). We also excluded participants if they had missing covariate data at wave 1 (n = 2048). See Figure S1 (Supplemental Digital Content 1, http://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A395) for a summary of how we derived our sample. See Table S1 (Supplemental Digital Content 1, http://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A395) for a comparison of covariates among included and excluded participants. Compared with excluded participants, participants included in our sample were younger, reported fewer depressive symptoms, were wealthier, were more likely to be female, had a higher body mass index (BMI), were more physically active, drank more frequently, were more likely to have a partner, had more years of education, were less likely to report a history of diabetes, cardiovascular disease (CVD), and more likely to report a history of hypertension.

We did not exclude participants with missing CASP-19 or CRP data. Mplus uses all available data to estimate the model using full information maximum likelihood. This approach to handling missing data is recommended over listwise deletion, pairwise deletion, and similar response pattern imputation (3).

Well-Being

Well-being was assessed at each wave with the CASP-19 quality of life questionnaire (22). The CASP-19 is designed to measure well-being across the subdomains of control, autonomy, self-realization, and pleasure. Participants respond to 19 questions on a four-point Likert scale (scored 0–3). Possible scores range from 0 to 57 with higher scores indicating higher well-being. For the study sample, the internal consistency reliability at wave 1 was high (α = 0.86).

Inflammatory Biomarkers

Participants who were not taking anticoagulant drugs and did not have clotting or bleeding disorders were invited to provide a blood sample. Fasting samples (no food or drink except water for the past 5 hours) were taken where possible (44% of the blood samples taken at wave 2 were fasting samples). Samples were assayed for high-sensitivity CRP and fibrinogen at the Royal Victoria Infirmary, Newcastle-upon-Tyne, United Kingdom. CRP concentration was measured in milligram/liter (reference range ≤ 3 mg/L (23)) and fibrinogen was measured in grams per liter (reference range = 1.45–3.48 g/L (24)). Because of its skewed distribution, we log transformed the CRP measure.

Incident Arthritis

At wave 1, participants were asked whether a doctor had ever told them that they had “arthritis or rheumatism.” Participants reported the month and year of their diagnosis. In subsequent waves, participants were asked to report whether they had been diagnosed with arthritis or rheumatism since their last interview. If a new diagnosis was reported, participants reported the month and year of diagnosis.

Covariates

We chose to adjust for factors that could account for the association between well-being and arthritis risk. These covariates were the following: age, sex, depressive symptoms, socioeconomic status, level of education, relationship status, health behaviors (physical activity, alcohol consumption, and smoking status), and BMI. These factors have previously been linked with well-being (20,25–28), arthritis risk (29–33), and CRP levels (34–36). We additionally adjusted for prevalent hypertension, diabetes, and CVD at wave 1 because these conditions commonly co-occur with arthritis (37) and have been linked to lower well-being (38).

The eight-item version of Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale was used to assess depressive symptoms (39). Socioeconomic status was indexed by total household wealth, which has been identified as the most accurate indicator of long-term socioeconomic circumstances in ELSA (40). Education was categorized on the basis of the highest reported level of qualification: less than O level or equivalent, O level or equivalent, A level or equivalent, higher than A level but below degree, and degree level (the US equivalent qualifications are the high school diploma for O level and 1 year of study at college or university with a B average for A level). Relationship status was dichotomized as having (coded 1) or not having (coded 0) a partner. Participants reported the frequency with which they engaged in vigorous, moderate, and mild exercise. Response options were “more than once a week,” “once a week,” “one to three times a month,” and “hardly ever or never.” As previously reported (41), responses to physical activity questions were recorded as either once a week (or more) or less than once a week. We then created the following four categories: physical inactivity, mild but not moderate/vigorous activity at least once a week, moderate but not vigorous physical activity at least once a week, and vigorous physical activity at least once a week. Frequency of alcohol consumption was recorded. Response options were the following: twice a day or more, daily or almost daily, once or twice a week, once or twice a month, special occasions only, and not at all. Participants reported their smoking status as either nonsmoker, ex-smoker, or current smoker. BMI was derived from height and weight measures taken during the nurse visit at wave 0, which took place between 1998, 1999, and 2001 (there was no BMI measure at wave 1).

To summarize, we used well-being measures from waves 1 to 6, CRP and fibrinogen measures from waves 2, 4 and 6, a BMI measure from wave 0, and all other covariate measures from wave 1.

Analysis

We ran preliminary analysis to establish whether log-CRP or fibrinogen levels at wave 2 were associated with arthritis risk in our sample. Each biomarker was entered separately into a Cox proportional hazards model, which was additionally adjusted for age and sex. Only CRP was a significant predictor of arthritis risk (p = .001). Consequently, we only tested for mediation using CRP.

To examine the association between CASP-19 or CRP with arthritis risk, we ran a Cox proportional hazards model predicting arthritis risk that included age, sex, and latent variables representing well-being and CRP initial status (intercepts) at wave 1 and wave 2, respectively, and amount of change (slopes) in well-being and CRP for the follow-up period (42). We used unstandardized CRP and well-being scores in line with the recommendation by Seltzer et al, (43). Unstandardized parameter estimates are in the units of the original scale. The CASP-19 slopes were defined so that slopes represented the predicted amount of change in CASP-19 score every 2 years (between waves). Slopes ranged from −4.65 to 1.86 (M [SD] = −0.69 [0.57]). CRP slopes represented the predicted amount of change in log-CRP concentration every 4 years (between waves 2, 4, and 6). These slopes ranged from −0.47 to 0.32 (M [SD] = −0.03 [0.05]).

We ran mediation analysis testing two possible mediation pathways. Specifically, we tested whether the association between well-being at wave 1 and arthritis risk was mediated by CRP concentrations at wave 2 and whether the association between change in well-being and arthritis risk was mediated by change in levels of CRP. Mediation analyses were conducted in Mplus Version 7.4 (44) using a maximum likelihood robust estimator and Monte Carlo integration. We tested for mediation using a structural equation modeling approach (45). This allowed us to estimate the direct effect of CASP-19 (intercept or slope) on arthritis risk and the indirect or mediated effect of CASP-19 (intercept or slope) on arthritis risk through CRP (intercept or slope). Mplus uses the Delta method (46) to calculate indirect effects and provides standard errors, confidence intervals (CIs), and significance tests.

We repeated these analysis additionally adjusting for wealth, education, relationship status, depressive symptoms, health behaviors, BMI, and comorbidities.

To test for the effect of reverse causation (specifically, undiagnosed arthritis at baseline affecting well-being and diagnosis of arthritis predating first CRP measurement), we re-ran the analysis excluding participants diagnosed with arthritis at wave 2.

To test whether we would find similar results for a shorter period, we repeated the analysis using only data collected between waves 1 and 4. Because there were only two measures of CRP within this period, we could not estimate CRP slope in this analysis. Instead, we created a measure of residual change in CRP between waves 2 and 4. This was achieved by regressing CRP at wave 4 on CRP at wave 2 with adjustment for follow-up duration, the standardized residuals from this analysis were used as the residual change measure (47). We then ran the Cox proportional hazards model outlined above replacing CRP slope with the residual change measure. The sample size (n = 2071) for this analysis was smaller because we excluded participants with missing CRP (at waves 2 or 4).

Coefficients of log-transformed dependent variables were back transformed using the formula (eβ1 − 1) × 100 and interpreted as the average percentage change in the dependent variable according to a unit increase in the independent variable. Log-transformed independent variables were back transformed using the formula β1 × ln(1.01) and interpreted as the amount of change in the dependent variable according to a 1% increase in the independent variable (48).

RESULTS

There were 1090 incident cases of arthritis between waves 2 and 6. Table 1 shows the number of new diagnoses reported at each wave as well as the mean CASP-19 score (at waves 1–6) and CRP concentration (milligram per liter) (at waves 2, 4, and 6).

TABLE 1
TABLE 1:
Incident Cases of Arthritis, Mean CASP-19 Score, and Median CRP Concentration at Each Wave

Table 2 shows baseline characteristics of the sample (n = 5266) according to well-being tertile. People with high well-being tended to be younger, wealthier, have a partner, were more likely to be female, more educated, more physically active, consumed more alcohol, and had lower depressive symptom scores. People with high well-being were also less likely to be overweight, smoke, or report a history of diabetes, hypertension, or CVD.

TABLE 2
TABLE 2:
Baseline Characteristics Stratified According to Tertiles of CASP-19 Scores (Lowest, Middle, and Highest Subjective Well-Being)

In preliminary Cox models adjusted for age and sex, fibrinogen was not associated with arthritis risk (for a unit increase in fibrinogen the hazard ratio [HR] was 1.05, 95% CI = 0.94 to 1.16, p = .39); however, higher levels of log-CRP were significantly associated with increased risk (HR = 1.14, 95% CI = 1.08 to 1.23, p < .001). Difference in median CRP concentration between participants that developed arthritis (median = 2.00 mg/L) and those that did not (median = 1.70 mg/L) was significant (p < .001) and similar in magnitude to the differences reported by Karlson et al. (13) and Nielen (15).

In the age- and sex-adjusted model (Fig. 1), the path from CASP-19 at wave 1 to CRP at wave 2 was significant. A unit increase in CASP-19 score at wave 1 was associated with an average of 2% (95% CI = 2% to 1%, p < .001) decrease in CRP concentration at wave 2. The path from CASP-19 slope to CRP slope was also significant with a unit increase in CASP-19 slope associated with an average of 6% (95% CI = 9% to 5%, p < .001) decrease in CRP slope. CASP-19 at wave 1 and CRP at wave 2 were significant predictors of arthritis risk. A 1-point increase in CASP-19 score was associated with a 3% decrease in arthritis risk (HR = 0.97, 95% CI = 0.96 to 0.98, p < .001). A 1% increase in CRP concentration at wave 2 was associated with an average of 0.002% (HR = 1.002, 95% CI = 1.001 to 0.002, p < .001) increase in arthritis risk. CASP-19 slope was also a significant predictor of arthritis risk; a unit increase in CASP-19 slope was associated with a 20% decrease in arthritis risk (HR = 0.80, 95% CI = 0.74 to 0.91, p < .001). CRP slope was not a significant predictor of arthritis risk. Mediation analysis revealed that the indirect effect of CASP-19 intercept on arthritis risk via CRP intercept was significant with 1 unit increase in CASP-19 associated with a 0.004% reduction in arthritis risk (p < .001). However, the indirect effect of CASP-19 slope via the CRP slope was not significant. The results of this model (including fit indices) are displayed in Table S2 (Supplemental Digital Content 1, http://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A395).

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1:
Path model adjusted for age and sex. Numbers in parentheses are exponentiated path coefficients (HRs). aCoefficients have been transformed to represent percentage change in CRP intercept or slope according to a unit increase in CASP intercept or slope. bCoefficients have been transformed to represent increase in arthritis risk according to a 1% increase in CRP intercept or slope. *p < .05, **p < .001, ns = p ≥ .05.

Estimates in the model that also adjusted for wealth, education, relationship status, depressive symptoms, health behaviors, BMI, and comorbidities were similar to those in the age and sex-adjusted model. However, the association between CRP at wave 2 and arthritis risk, which was attenuated (HR = 1.001, 95% CI = 1.000 to 1.002, p = .016). The association between CASP-19 slope and arthritis risk was also attenuated (HR = 0.82, 95% CI = 0.69 to 0.96). The indirect effect of CASP-19 intercept on arthritis risk via CRP intercept remained significant with a unit increase in CASP associated with a 0.002% (p = .020) reduction in arthritis risk (Fig. 2). The results of this model (including fit indices) are displayed in Table S3 (Supplemental Digital Content 1, http://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A395).

FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2:
Path model additionally adjusted for comorbidities, demographic, and health behavior variables. Numbers in parentheses are exponentiated path coefficients (HRs). aCoefficients have been transformed to represent percentage change in CRP intercept or slope according to a unit increase in CASP intercept or slope. bCoefficients have been transformed to represent increase in arthritis risk according to a 1% increase in CRP intercept or slope. *p < .05, **p < .001, ns = p ≥ .05.

Additional factors significantly associated with a higher arthritis risk in the fully adjusted model, included the following: being female, being diagnosed with hypertension, having a higher depressive symptom score, and having a higher BMI.

We re-ran the age- and sex-adjusted model excluding participants diagnosed with arthritis at wave 2. Results were similar to those in the original analysis; a unit increase in CASP-19 intercept was associated with a 2% (p < .001) reduction in arthritis risk via the direct pathway and 0.003% reduction in arthritis risk via the indirect pathway (via CRP intercept) (p = .004).

In analysis using data from waves 1 to 4 only, direct pathways from CASP-19 and CRP intercepts to arthritis risk were attenuated but remained significant as did the association between CASP-19 and CRP intercept. However, the indirect pathway from CASP-19 intercept (via CRP intercept) to arthritis risk was no longer significant (p = .057). The indirect pathway from CASP-19 slope (via change in CRP) to arthritis risk was also not significant (p = .188).

DISCUSSION

High well-being is associated with a reduced risk of developing arthritis. Our aim was to test whether CRP or fibrinogen mediated this association. Only CRP was associated with arthritis risk. Our analysis revealed that the association between well-being at wave 1 and arthritis risk for a 10-year period was partially mediated by CRP concentration at wave 2. However, it should be noted that CRP concentration accounted for only 12% of this risk association. Although change in well-being for the follow up period was associated with arthritis risk, this association was not mediated by change in CRP.

Our estimated effect size for the association between well-being and arthritis risk was similar to that found in our previous study (17), although, in contrast to this previous study, we did not detect a stronger association at younger ages.

The significant pathway between well-being at wave 1, CRP concentration at wave 2, and arthritis risk suggests that inflammatory processes are implicated in the link between well-being and arthritis risk. Although this mediation effect was modest, it supports the idea that well-being can affect disease risk via biological pathways.

Our estimate for the association between well-being and CRP is comparable with the association reported in a cross-sectional study in which an SD increase in quality of life score was associated with a 9.42% reduction in CRP concentration (6). However, as in this cross-sectional study, the direction of effect between well-being and CRP concentration is unclear. It is possible that reduction in CRP is a downstream consequence of high well-being on prefrontal and limbic system processes (49). Alternatively, it is also possible that inflammatory processes could affect well-being because they have been linked to insomnia, fatigue, hostility, and depression (50,51). It is perhaps most likely that well-being and CRP are reciprocally related; additional intervention studies could help quantify the extent to which well-being can affect CRP concentration or vice versa.

The mediation effect in our study was small due to the fact that CRP concentration at wave 2 was only weakly related to arthritis risk. Deane et al. (52) suggest that single inflammatory markers do not provide a reliable indicator of arthritis risk. This limitation may account for the weak association between CRP and arthritis risk as well as the insignificant association between fibrinogen and arthritis risk in our sample. A more accurate prediction of risk can be achieved by combining measures of multiple arthritis-related biomarkers including levels of autoantibodies and cytokines/chemokines (52). A model of the association between well-being and arthritis risk including these measures may reveal a stronger mediation effect than the one observed here.

We found that well-being and CRP slopes were significantly and inversely related—such that an increase in well-being between waves was associated with a decrease in CRP concentration between waves. The significant relationship between well-being and CRP trajectories could result from mechanisms similar to those outlined earlier. That is, change in well-being could cause a change in CRP concentration via psychobiological pathways, or change in physical symptoms (e.g., chronic pain or disability) associated with levels of inflammation could affect an individual's sense of well-being.

Change in well-being was associated with arthritis risk; however, change in CRP did not mediate this association. This is because change in CRP was not related to arthritis risk. Further work is needed to establish the timing between change in CRP concentration and the onset of arthritic symptoms (53). However, there is some indication that elevation in CRP concentration can precede the onset of symptoms by up to 20 years (14). It is possible that the 8-year follow-up period (waves 2–6) in our study was too short to capture changes in CRP concentration relevant to arthritis risk. In addition, participants who left the study before wave 6 had significantly higher levels of CRP at wave 2 than participants who remained in the study. This pattern of attrition could have resulted in an underestimation of the association between CRP or CRP change and arthritis risk in our study as participants who left may have had a higher risk of arthritis.

Analysis excluding participants diagnosed with arthritis at wave 2 yielded similar results—indicating that our findings are unlikely to reflect the effect of reverse causation, that is, undiagnosed arthritis affecting reports of well-being at wave 1. Results from analysis using a shorter follow-up period (4 years) were less consistent with our original findings. The indirect effect of well-being intercept (via CRP at wave 2) on arthritis risk was no longer significant. It is possible that this insignificant result was due to the lower power of this analysis (the number of incident cases was 421 compared with 1090 in our original analysis).

Our findings should be interpreted with caution because this study had some important limitations. Excluding a significant proportion of participants from our sample (due to missing data at wave 1) may have introduced a source of selection bias. Participants excluded from our sample differed to those included on a number of covariate variables (see Table S1, Supplemental Digital Content 1, http://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A395). In addition, arthritis incidence was ascertained using self-report. Although access to medical records would have been preferable, there is evidence that self-report of arthritis diagnosis is consistent with clinically derived measures (54). Finally, we were unable to distinguish between cases of rheumatoid arthritis and osteoarthritis. It is likely that the mechanisms underlying the association between well-being and rheumatoid or osteoarthritis are qualitatively different because these conditions involve distinct pathophysiological processes. Our study also had several strengths. The sample size was large, and we were able to control for many possible confounds.

In summary, our results indicate that CRP concentration mediates the association between well-being and arthritis risk (after taking demographic and health behavior differences into account). Although the magnitude of this mediating effect was small, we believe our findings have theoretical implications. Specifically, they provide a proof of principle that biological processes can partially mediate the link between well-being and disease. CRP concentration represents a small component of a dynamic and interactive biological system. A combination of multiple measures of biological function would enable researchers to assess the clinical significance of the pathway between well-being, psychobiological processes and disease risk (55). We hope that our findings will help motivate this line of investigation.

Source of Funding and Conflicts of Interest: Funding from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council and Medical Research Council is gratefully acknowledged. J.O. and C.G. are members of the University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, part of the cross council Lifelong Health and Wellbeing Initiative (MR/K026992/1). The authors report no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

1. Diener E, Chan MY. Happy people live longer: subjective well-being contributes to health and longevity. Appl Psychol Health Well-Being 2011;3:1–43.
2. Feller S, Teucher B, Kaaks R, Boeing H, Vigl M. Life satisfaction and risk of chronic diseases in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC)-Germany Study. PLoS One 2013;8:e73462.
3. Steptoe A, Wardle J. Enjoying life and living longer. Arch Intern Med 2012;172:273–5.
4. Howell RT, Kern ML, Lyubomirsky S. Health benefits: meta-analytically determining the impact of well-being on objective health outcomes. Health Psychol Rev 2007;1:83–136.
5. Dockray S, Steptoe A. Positive affect and psychobiological processes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010;35:69–75.
6. Marteinsdottir I, Ernerudh J, Jonasson L, Kristenson M, Garvin P. Psychological resources are independently associated with markers of inflammation in a middle-aged community sample. Int J Behav Med 2016;23:611–20.
7. Roy B, Diez-Roux AV, Seeman T, Ranjit N, Shea S, Cushman M. Association of optimism and pessimism with inflammation and hemostasis in the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA). Psychosom Med 2010;72:134–40.
8. Sin NL, Graham-Engeland JE, Almeida DM. Daily positive events and inflammation: findings from the National Study of Daily Experiences. Brain Behav Immun 2015;43:130–8.
9. Steptoe A, Demakakos P, de Oliveira C, Wardle J. Distinctive biological correlates of positive psychological well-being in older men and women. Psychosom Med 2012;74:501–8.
10. Steptoe A, O'Donnell K, Badrick E, Kumari M, Marmot M. Neuroendocrine and inflammatory factors associated with positive affect in healthy men and women: The Whitehall II Study. Am J Epidemiol 2008;167:96–102.
11. Hamer M, Chida Y. Life satisfaction and inflammatory biomarkers: The 2008 Scottish Health Survey1. Jpn Psychol Res 2011;53:133–9.
12. Singh T, Newman AB. Inflammatory markers in population studies of aging. Ageing Res Rev 2011;10:319–29.
13. Karlson EW, Chibnik LB, Tworoger SS, Lee I-M, Buring JE, Shadick NA, Manson JE, Costenbader KH. Biomarkers of inflammation and development of rheumatoid arthritis in women from two prospective cohort studies. Arthritis Rheum 2009;60:641–52.
14. Masi AT, Aldag JC, Sipes J. Do elevated levels of serum C-reactive protein predict rheumatoid arthritis in men: correlations with pre-RA status and baseline positive rheumatoid factors. J Rheumatol 2001;28:2359–61.
15. Nielen MM. Preclinical rheumatoid arthritis [Internet]. Vrije Universiteit; 2009. Available at: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Mark_Nielen/publication/8378814_Increased_levels_of_Creactive_protein_in_serum_from_blood_donors_before_the_onset_of_rheumatoid_arthritis/links/54002d290cf24c81027dcb1b.pdf. Accessed September 9, 2016.
16. Sokolove J, Lepus CM. Role of inflammation in the pathogenesis of osteoarthritis: latest findings and interpretations. Ther Adv Musculoskelet Dis 2013;5:77–94.
17. Okely JA, Cooper C, Gale CR. Wellbeing and arthritis incidence: the survey of health, ageing and retirement in Europe. Ann Behav Med 2016;50:419–26.
18. Nielen MM, Van Schaardenburg D, Reesink HW, Twisk JW, Van De Stadt RJ, Van Der Horst-Bruinsma IE, De Gast T, Habibuw MR, Vandenbroucke JP, Dijkmans BA. Increased levels of C-reactive protein in serum from blood donors before the onset of rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 2004;50:2423–7.
19. Arvidson NG, Larsson A, Larsen A. Disease activity in rheumatoid arthritis: fibrinogen is superior to the erythrocyte sedimentation rate. Scand J Clin Lab Invest 2002;62:315–9.
20. Steptoe A, Deaton A, Stone AA. Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet 2015;385:640–8.
21. Steptoe A, Breeze E, Banks J, Nazroo J. Cohort profile: the English longitudinal study of ageing. Int J Epidemiol 2013;42:1640–8.
22. Hyde M, Wiggins RD, Higgs P, Blane DB. A measure of quality of life in early old age: the theory, development and properties of a needs satisfaction model (CASP-19). Aging Ment Health 2003;7:186–94.
23. Kushner I, Rzewnicki D, Samols D. What does minor elevation of c-reactive protein signify? Am J Med 2006;119:166.e17–28.
24. Oswald MW, Hunt HH, Lazarchick J. Normal range of plasma fibrinogen. Am J Med Technol 1983;49:57–9.
25. Brett CE, Gow AJ, Corley J, Pattie A, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Psychosocial factors and health as determinants of quality of life in community-dwelling older adults. Qual Life Res 2012;21:505–16.
26. Hanmer J, Lawrence WF, Anderson JP, Kaplan RM, Fryback DG. Report of nationally representative values for the noninstitutionalized US adult population for 7 health-related quality-of-life scores. Med Decis Making 2006;26:391–400.
27. Pinquart M, Sörensen S. Influences of socioeconomic status, social network, and competence on subjective well-being in later life: a meta-analysis. Psychol Aging 2000;15:187–224.
28. Pressman SD, Cohen S. Does positive affect influence health? Psychol Bull 2005;131:925–71.
29. Bengtsson C, Nordmark B, Klareskog L, Lundberg I, Alfredsson L. Socioeconomic status and the risk of developing rheumatoid arthritis: results from the Swedish EIRA study. Ann Rheum Dis 2005;64:1588–94.
30. Buckwalter JA, Lappin DR. The disproportionate impact of chronic arthralgia and arthritis among women. Clin Orthop Relat Res 2000:159–68.
31. Felson DT, Zhang Y, Hannan MT, Naimark A, Weissman B, Aliabadi P, Levy D. Risk factors for incident radiographic knee osteoarthritis in the elderly: the Framingham Study. Arthritis Rheum 1997;40:728–33.
32. Felson DT, Lawrence RC, Dieppe PA, Hirsch R, Helmick CG, Jordan JM, Kington RS, Lane NE, Nevitt MC, Zhang Y, Sowers M. Osteoarthritis: new insights. Part 1: the disease and its risk factors. Ann Intern Med 2000;133:635–46.
33. Harris ML, Loxton D, Sibbritt DW, Byles JE. The influence of perceived stress on the onset of arthritis in women: findings from the Australian Longitudinal Study on women's health. Ann Behav Med 2013;46:9–18.
34. Brummett BH, Babyak MA, Singh A, Jiang R, Williams RB, Harris KM, Siegler IC. Socioeconomic indices as independent correlates of C-reactive protein in the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Psychosom Med 2013;75:882–93.
35. Wium-Andersen MK, Ørsted DD, Nielsen SF, Nordestgaard BG. Elevated C-reactive protein levels, psychological distress, and depression in 73, 131 individuals. JAMA Psychiatry 2013;70:176–84.
36. Zhang X, Shu X-O, Signorello LB, Hargreaves MK, Cai Q, Linton MF, Fazio S, Zheng W, Blot WJ. Correlates of high serum C-reactive protein levels in a socioeconomically disadvantaged population. Dis Markers 2008;24:351–9.
37. Nüesch E, Dieppe P, Reichenbach S, Williams S, Iff S, Jüni P. All cause and disease specific mortality in patients with knee or hip osteoarthritis: population based cohort study. Bmj [Internet] 2011 [cited 2015 Jul 30];342. Available at: http://www.bmj.com/content/342/bmj.d1165. Accessed July 30, 2015.
38. Wikman A, Wardle J, Steptoe A. Quality of life and affective well-being in middle-aged and older people with chronic medical illnesses: a cross-sectional population based study. PLoS One 2011;6:e18952.
39. Steffick DE. Documentation of affective functioning measures in the Health and Retirement Study. Ann Arbor MI HRS Health Work Group 2000. Available at: http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/dr-005.pdf.
40. Banks J, Karlsen S, Oldfield Z. Socio-economic position. In: Health, wealth and lifestyles of the older population in England, London: Institute of Fiscal Studies; 2003;71–125.
41. Hamer M, de Oliveira C, Demakakos P. Non-exercise physical activity and survival: English longitudinal study of ageing. Am J Prev Med 2014;47:452–60.
42. Duncan TE, Duncan SC. An introduction to latent growth curve modeling. Behav Ther 2004;35:333–63.
43. Seltzer MH, Frank KA, Bryk AS. The metric matters: the sensitivity of conclusions about growth in student achievement to choice of metric. Educ Eval Policy Anal 1994;16:41–9.
44. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User's Guide, 7th ed. Los Angeles: CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998.
45. Lockhart G, MacKinnon DP, Ohlrich V. Mediation analysis in psychosomatic medicine research. Psychosom Med 2011;73:29–43.
46. MacKinnon DP. Introduction to statistical mediation analysis [Internet]. Routledge; 2008 . Available at: https://books.google.co.uk/books?hl=en&lr=&id=vHUVYKfrdtYC&oi=fnd&pg=PP2&dq=Introduction+to+Statistical+Mediation+Analysis&ots=QB6VI3lhNs&sig=qaRSRjamaj8x4Eg7V137MPvWNmM. Accessed June 16, 2016.
47. Twisk JW. Applied longitudinal data analysis for epidemiology: a practical guide [Internet]. Cambridge University Press; 2013. Available at: https://books.google.co.uk/books?hl=en&lr=&id=DRSAfYDnZqUC&oi=fnd&pg=PR13&dq=Applied+Longitudinal+Data+Analysis+for+Epidemiology:+A+Practical+Guide&ots=UP4FTcusDX&sig=fIubGJ4FF5eZKtCB6aZtLSK2Mx0. Accessed January 26, 2017.
48. Vittinghoff E, Glidden DV, Shiboski SC, McCulloch CE. Regression methods in biostatistics: linear, logistic, survival, and repeated measures models [Internet]. Springer Science & Business Media; 2011. Available at: https://books.google.co.uk/books?hl=en&lr=&id=fXC1l1JeqLoC&oi=fnd&pg=PR7&dq=Regression+Methods+in+Biostatistics:+Linear,+Logistic,+Survival,+and+Repeated+Measures+Models+(Statistics+for+Biology+and+Health&ots=xJo2K494OU&sig=pbNO7QXK6IqEDigDMnZD9qVHygg. Accessed August 31, 2016.
49. Ryff CD, Singer BH, Dienberg Love G. Positive health: connecting well-being with biology. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2004;359:1383–94.
50. Raison CL, Capuron L, Miller AH. Cytokines sing the blues: inflammation and the pathogenesis of depression. Trends Immunol 2006;27:24–31.
51. Suarez EC, Lewis JG, Krishnan RR, Young KH. Enhanced expression of cytokines and chemokines by blood monocytes to in vitro lipopolysaccharide stimulation are associated with hostility and severity of depressive symptoms in healthy women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004;29:1119–28.
52. Deane KD, O'Donnell CI, Hueber W, Majka DS, Lazar AA, Derber LA, Gilliland WR, Edison JD, Norris JM, Robinson WH, Holers VM. The number of elevated cytokines and chemokines in preclinical seropositive rheumatoid arthritis predicts time to diagnosis in an age-dependent manner. Arthritis Rheum 2010;62:3161–72.
53. van Steenbergen HW, Huizinga TW, van der Helm-van Mil AH. The preclinical phase of rheumatoid arthritis: what is acknowledged and what needs to be assessed? Arthritis Rheum 2013;65:2219–32.
54. March LM, Schwarz JM, Carfrae BH, Bagge E. Clinical validation of self-reported osteoarthritis. Osteoarthritis Cartilage 1998;6:87–93.
55. Kubzansky LD, Boehm JK, Segerstrom SC. positive psychological functioning and the biology of health. Soc Personal Psychol Compass 2015;9:645–60.
Keywords:

arthritis; inflammation; longitudinal study; mediation; well-being; BMI = body mass index; CASP = control, autonomy, self-realization and pleasure; CI = confidence interval; CRP = C-reactive protein; CVD = cardiovascular disease; ELSA = English Longitudinal Study of Ageing; HR = hazard ratio; IQR = interquartile range

Supplemental Digital Content

Copyright © 2017 The Author(s). Published by Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. on behalf of the American Psychosomatic Society.