INTRODUCTION
The period from birth to 12 months or infancy is a crucial period in development as it marks the onset of rapid growth in the physical, social, and neurocognitive domains.1 These domains dynamically interact with each other such that a developmental change in any one domain causes a “cumulative and cascading” effect in other domains.2 For instance, when infants learn to sit independently, they use their hands more frequently for communicative gestures with caregivers, supporting their language development.3 Likewise, the onset of independent locomotion allows infants to take control of play in terms of toy choices, supporting more mature social interactions with caregivers.4 Emerging evidence supports that pediatric therapeutic interventions (physical, occupational, and speech therapy) starting from birth can support development in multiple domains, ranging from feeding and neurobehavioral domains in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU)5 to motor and cognitive domains in infancy.6,7 However, the collective evidence for effective early intervention techniques, especially in children younger than 2 years, is heterogeneous and limited at best.8
The need for evidence-informed interventions in infancy calls for a more robust approach that explores complementary fields of development to identify theoretical models, measurement, and intervention approaches.9,10 The developmental psychology literature is abundant with task-specific paradigms that examine learning and memory in infants, one of them being the “mobile paradigm.” In this paradigm, the infant's leg is tethered to an overhead mobile, such that when the infant kicks, the mobile moves. The original paradigm is based on the principle of operant conditioning, which suggests that behaviors that are reinforced are repeated, the reinforcer being the mobile movement.11 However, a recent review proposed embodied cognition as a mechanism responsible for learning in contingency paradigms.12 They stated that learning a contingency is not merely a result of reinforcement but rather occurs when the infant identifies the relationship between their self-initiated actions and its effect on the environment.12 This form of contingency learning is dependent on variability in practice,13 age,14 and environmental constraints.15 In the last 2 decades, the mobile paradigm has been adapted to develop new contingency paradigms to evaluate motor-learning abilities13 in both term and preterm infants and to study motor adaptations to reinforcement.14–16
Although these paradigms are typically used for evaluation purposes, they include critical elements required for effective early intervention.17,18 First, these paradigms are inherently designed to be task-specific and require practice through trial and error to learn and retain the contingency. Such practice mimics typical motor development, during which infants repeatedly perform variable movements until they select the most successful strategy for function.19 This could be specifically beneficial in infants with or at risk for delays since they lack the movement variability necessary for learning functional skills.20 Second, contingency paradigms provide opportunities for a more demanding practice while maintaining active infant participation. This is crucial as conventional early intervention services are not designed to be intensive.21 Moreover, repeated practice not only strengthens the developing musculature but also optimizes neuroplasticity.18 Finally, these paradigms can easily be adapted to home environment and may provide opportunities for caregiver involvement in therapy. Caregiver-delivered interventions are proven to be more effective in very young infants born preterm.5
Thus, the strong theoretical background, ease of application, and potential to improve developmental outcomes make contingency paradigms a promising new intervention strategy for infants. To the best of our knowledge, there is no review assimilating the studies and summarizing the effectiveness of contingency paradigms as an intervention, particularly as it relates to physical therapy and infant development. Thus, the aim of this systematic review was to quantify the efficacy of interventions based on contingency paradigms in infancy on feeding, motor, and cognitive outcomes. These specific outcome domains were selected as they form the primary focus of physical therapy practice for neonates and infants and capture therapy outcomes from birth to infancy.22 The contingency paradigms for this review included “a contingency learning component, active infant movement and a practice through trial and error to achieve a clearly defined outcome.” The interventions are abbreviated as CPBI, that is, contingency paradigm–based interventions throughout this article.
METHODS
Protocol and Registration
This systematic review was prospectively registered with PROSPERO and can be accessed at (https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/; identifier CRD42020163140). Review reporting is in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses statement.23
Eligibility Criteria
Full-text articles in English, published, or in press before our search, that is, January 2021; Population: Term or preterm infants with a mean age of 12 months and younger and no restriction on medical diagnosis; Intervention: Interventions incorporating task-specific activities based on contingency learning principles as either the primary component or one of the key components. Comparator: Noncontingency learning–based paradigms, usual care, or no treatment; Outcomes: feeding, motor, and cognitive outcomes. Outcomes had to be assessed pre- and postintervention (ranging from last day of intervention to 24 months post). The outcome had to measure “transfer of learning,” that is, application of the acquired skill to a new situation. For instance, an infant is trained to reach using a teether toy during therapy session and then uses this learned new skill to reach for a different toy during play; Studies: Randomized controlled trials (RCTs), non-RCTs (NRCTs), quasi–randomized controlled trials (QRCTs), multiarm pre-/postdesigns, and randomized crossover trials. Articles were excluded if mean age was more than 12 months at the time of intervention; interventions principles were based on other forms of associative learning such as classical conditioning; studies using principles of associative learning for evaluative or assessment purpose only; studies reporting only physiological outcomes (example: oxygen saturation, weight gain, heart rate, electroencephalography); abstracts, reviews, or conceptual papers; and study designs with no control group (single arm pre-/post, case series, or case study).
Search Strategy
A comprehensive literature search of articles from the earliest record to January 2021 was performed using the following electronic databases: PubMed, Cochrane, CINAHL, Web of Science, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, PsycINFO, and ERIC. The final search terms for PubMed database were as follows: infan*, “Association Learning“[Mesh]”, Conditioning, Operant”[Mesh], “Token Economy”[Mesh], “Reinforcement, Psychology”[Mesh], “contingent reinforcement”, “contingency learning”, “associative learning”, “learn*”, “motor learning.” These search terms were adapted according to the other database's command language, and no date or language restrictions were applied. Filters for age (infants or 0-12 months), population, and article type were applied to refine the search when necessary. Secondary search was performed using relevant references of the included articles or by searching for included interventions by name and author. Additional gray literature was searched using the following resources: Google Scholar, PsycEXTRA, and Proquest dissertation and thesis (see Supplemental Digital Content 1, available at: https://links.lww.com/PPT/A350).
Study Selection
Search results were imported in a Web-based systematic review management software called “Covidence” (https://www.covidence.org). After duplicates were removed, 2 reviewers independently performed the title and abstract screening based on the defined exclusion criteria. If the abstract was not excluded, full text of the selected article was retrieved and the screening process was repeated reviewing the eligibility criteria. If there was a lack of agreement between the 2 reviewers on the inclusion of any of the articles, the third reviewer was consulted for the final decision.
Data Extraction
Two independent reviewers extracted the following data from the included articles: study setting, sponsorship source, corresponding author details, study design, eligibility criteria, participant characteristics, intervention groups, outcomes used, statistical results, and study conclusion. The “Template for Intervention Description and Replication” (TIDieR) checklist was used to extract additional intervention-specific information as follows: provider, setting, dosage, fidelity, and compliance assessment.24 If no agreement was reached, the third reviewer was consulted.
Risk of Bias and Quality of Evidence Assessment in Individual Studies
The risk of bias (ROB) for all the NRCTs or QRCTs was assessed using the ROBINS-I (“Risk of Bias in Non-randomized Studies of Interventions”). This tool comprises a 3-stage assessment and 7 specific domains. Each study was given an overall ROB judgment as having no information, low, moderate, serious, and critical ROB.25 For the RCTs, ROB was assessed using Cochrane ROB 2.0 and it has 5 domains. The overall judgment for ROB can be low, high, or show some concerns.26 Quality of evidence for each study was identified using the Sackett's level of evidence for group research designs (see Supplemental Digital Content 2, available at: https://links.lww.com/PPT/A351).27
Data Synthesis
Because of the heterogeneity in CPBI and measured outcomes, a meta-analysis could not be completed. A narrative synthesis of results is presented, and the effectiveness of interventions is reported using P values at a level of significance of ≤.05.
RESULTS
Study Selection
The final search located 6025 studies. After duplicate removal, 4523 studies remained and were assessed for title and abstract screening and 4378 studies were excluded. The full text of 145 studies was examined and 125 studies were excluded as they did not meet the eligibility criteria. A total of 20 studies were identified for inclusion in the review.28–47 Of the 20 studies, 6 studies shared a common data set32,33,37,38,43,44 and were combined to give a total of 17 unique studies (Figure).
Fig.: PRISMA flowchart of article selection. This figure is available in color online (
www.pedpt.com).
Characteristics of Included Studies
A summary of individual study characteristics is shown in Table 1. The TIDieR checklist for each study is shown in Supplemental Digital Content 3, available at: https://links.lww.com/PPT/A352. Description of outcome measures for each study is shown in Table 2 and results for intervention effectiveness are summarized in Table 3.
TABLE 1 -
Characteristics of Included Studies
|
|
|
Participant Details |
|
|
Study Details |
|
|
Intervention Details |
Study |
Design |
Sample Size |
Gestational Age, wk |
Age at Treatment Onset |
Intervention Group |
Control Group |
Contingency learning–based interventions to improve feeding outcomes |
Standley28 (2003) |
RCT |
32 |
24-36 (M = 31.8) Extremely to very preterm |
36.1-wk AA |
Pacifier activated lullaby for NNS |
Usual care NNS |
Standley et al29 (2010) |
RCT |
68 |
M = 29.2 Very preterm |
32- to 36-wk AA |
Pacifier-activated lullaby for NNS |
Usual care NNS |
Chorna et al30 (2014) |
RCT |
94 |
28-32 (M = 30) Very preterm |
34.5-wk AA |
Pacifier-activated mother's voice for NNS-3 trials and pacifier-activated mother's voice for NNS-1 trial |
Usual care NNS |
Contingency learning interventions to improve motor and cognitive outcomes |
Needham et al31 (2002) |
NRCT |
32 |
>37 Full term |
3-4 mo |
Closed sticky mittens training |
No training |
Libertus and Needham32,33 (2010, 2011) |
NRCT |
58 |
>37 Full term |
2-3 mo |
Closed sticky mittens training |
Nonsticky mittens observation training No treatment 3-mo-olds No treatment 5-mo-olds |
Libertus et al34 (2016) Follow-up to Libertus and Needham32 (2010) |
NRCT |
40 (N = 25 were a subset of Libertus and Needham32 [2010]) |
>37 Full term |
2-3 mo |
Closed sticky mittens training |
Nonsticky mittens observation training Control (no training) |
Libertus and Needham35 (2014) |
NRCT |
72 (N = 36 were a subset of Libertus and Needham32 [2010]) |
>37 Full term |
3 mo |
Closed sticky mittens training |
Nonsticky mittens observation training Encouragement experience Movement experience |
Rakison and Krogh36 (2012) |
RCT |
40 |
>37 Full term |
4.5 mo |
Closed sticky mittens training |
Nonsticky mittens movement training |
Gerson and Woodward37,38 (2014) |
NRCT |
60 |
>37 Full term |
3.5 mo |
Closed sticky mittens training |
Nonsticky mittens observation training |
Williams et al39 (2015) |
QRCT |
37 |
>37 Full term |
3 mo |
Open sticky mittens |
Nonsticky mittens movement training Control (no training) |
Wiesen et al40 (2016) |
RCT |
32 |
>37 Full term |
3 mo |
Closed sticky mittens training |
Nonsticky mittens observation training |
Needham et al41 (2017) |
RCT |
38 |
>37 Full term |
4-5 mo |
Closed sticky mittens training |
Nonsticky mittens observation training |
Nascimento et al42 (2019) |
RCT |
24 |
34-36 mo (M = 36.08) Late preterm |
4- to 4.5-mo AA |
Open sticky mitten training |
Social training |
Heathcock et al43 (2008); Heathcock and Galloway44 (2009) |
RCT |
26 |
28-32 (M = 31) Very preterm |
2-moAA |
Contingent toys + Movement training |
Social training |
Needham et al45 (2014) |
NRCT |
38 |
>37 Full term |
3 mo |
Contingent toy training |
Noncontingent passive observation |
Campbell et al46 (2015) |
RCT |
13 |
23-32 (M = 27 wk) Extremely preterm with periventricular brain injury |
2- to 4-mo AA |
Contingent toy training |
Control (no training) |
Williams and Corbetta47 (2016) |
RCT |
35 |
>37 Full term |
3 mo |
Contingent toy training |
Noncontingent toy movement training Control (no training) |
Abbreviations: AA, adjusted age; M, mean; NNS, nonnutritive sucking; NRCT, non–randomized controlled trials; QRCT, quasi–randomized controlled trial; RCT, randomized controlled trials.
TABLE 2 -
Description of Intervention Outcomes
Study |
Outcome Name |
Outcome Domain |
Measurement Criteria |
Purpose |
Scoring Method |
Psychometrics |
Standley28 (2003) |
Oral feeding rate |
Feeding |
Volume ingested through nipple feeding divided by minutes of feeding |
Measures nutritive sucking abilities |
Recorded from nurse's notes |
None reported |
Standley et al29 (2010) |
Number of oral feeding days before discharge |
Feeding |
Number of nipple feeding days from the last day of nasogastric/oral-gastric feed to discharge date |
Measures nutritive sucking abilities |
Recorded from nurse's notes |
None reported |
Length of gavage feeding days |
Number of days from birth to the last day of nasogastric/oral-gastric feed |
Measures days to transition to oral feeding. Indicates neural maturation |
Chorna et al30 (2014) |
Oral feeding rate |
Feeding |
Volume of nipple nutrition intake in milliliters divided by time in minutes for consumption |
Measures nutritive sucking abilities |
Recorded from nurse's notes |
None reported |
Oral feeding volume |
Amount of nipple nutrition measured in milliliters per kilogram per day |
Oral feeding frequency |
Amount of nipple nutrition measured by the number of nipple feedings per day |
Days to oral feeding |
Number of days to transition from gavage feeding to nipple feeding |
Measures days to transition to oral feeding. Indicates neural maturation |
Needham et al31 (2002) |
Intentional swats |
Motor |
Number of times the infant attempted making manual contact with the toy while maintaining visual contact |
Measures “intentionality” of swatting and early reaching abilities |
Video coding using a joystick |
One-third of sample recorded for interrater reliability: percent agreement was 87%-99% (mean = 93%) |
Visual attention to toy |
Visual cognition |
Percentage of time the infant visually explored the toy divided by total trial duration |
Measures engagement in exploration |
Multimodal exploration |
Motor and visual cognition |
Adding the time infant spent mouthing and looking at the time and time spent switching between oral and visual exploration |
Switching between 2 modalities requires more advanced cognitive and motor skills. Measures intersensory coordination |
Libertus and Needham32,33 (2010, 2011) |
Reaching |
Motor |
Proportion of time infant spent reaching toward the toy while looking at it, when the toy was within its reach divided by total trial duration |
Measures early reaching abilities |
Frame-by-frame video coding using StopFrameCoding software (Version 0.9) |
Random sample was coded for interrater reliability: correlation (r = 0.90) Coding method was also correlated with previously used coding methods (r = 0.75-0.90) |
|
Grasping |
Motor |
Proportion of time infant spent touching the toy and bringing at least one corner of the toy off the testing table divided by total trial duration |
Measures early object manipulation abilities |
|
Visual attention to toy |
Visual cognition |
Proportion of time infant spent looking at the toy when it was placed beyond its reach divided by total trial duration |
Measures engagement in exploration |
|
Visual attention to experimenter |
Visual cognition |
Proportion of time infant spent looking at the experimenter when it was placed beyond its reach divided by total trial duration |
Measures engagement in exploration and attention to task |
|
Shift in visual attention |
Visual cognition |
Proportion of time infant spent looking back and forth from the toy when it was placed in its hands divided by total trial duration |
Measures the ability to identify relationship between object and environment |
|
Face preference |
Social cognition |
Difference in proportion of time infant spent looking at the photograph of a face (neutral expression of a White female) versus a toy (commercially available) |
Measures infant's ability to identify itself as “intentional agent” and to discriminate social beings from inanimate objects |
Remote eye tracker (Tobii 1750) |
Not reported |
|
Face orienting |
Social cognition |
Face preference during the 1000-2000 ms time window following stimulus presentation |
Libertus et al34 (2016) Follow-up to Libertus and Needham32 (2010) |
Grasping |
Motor |
Duration of time the infant made a toy contact resulting in lifting at least 1 corner of the toy off the table, lifting internal parts of the toy, or by having fingers clearly curled around the object |
Measures the effect of early reaching practice on later complex manual skills |
Frame-by-frame video coding using StopFrameCoding software (Version 0.9) |
30% of videos coded for interrater reliability: correlation analysis (r = 0.97) |
|
Toy rotations |
Motor |
Duration of time the infant touched the toy or its parts in such a way that the object turned around its own axis. |
Measures the effect of early reaching practice on later complex manual skills |
|
Visual attention to toy |
Visual cognition |
Duration of time the infant spent looking at the toy |
Measures engagement in exploration |
|
Distraction |
Visual cognition |
Duration of time the infant spent looking somewhere else |
Measures engagement in exploration and attention to task |
Libertus and Needham35 (2014) |
Reaching and grasping combined |
Motor |
Duration of time the infant moved its arm toward the toy resulting in contact with the toy or partial/complete lift of the object off the table |
Measures early object manipulation abilities |
Frame-by-frame video coding using StopFrameCoding software (Version 0.9) |
38% of videos coded for interrater reliability: correlation analysis (r = 0.88) |
|
Visual attention to toy |
Motor |
Frequency of the infant's looking episodes at the toy when the toy was placed in its hand |
Motor, manual exploration |
|
Face preference |
Social cognition |
Difference in proportion of time infant spent looking at the photograph of a face (neutral expression of a White female) versus a toy (commercially available) |
Measures infant's ability to identify itself as “intentional agent” and to discriminate social beings from inanimate objects |
Remote eye tracker (Tobii 1750) |
Not reported |
Rakison and Krogh36 (2012) |
Visual attention/looking at habituation experiment |
Causal cognition |
Duration of looking time at an animated event on a computer screen which resembled the contingent training (Training involved touching green ball with red mitten
Test event displayed green ball being hit by a red ball) |
Measures infant's ability to identify the goal structure in other's actions. Indicates change in information-processing abilities after self-produced actions |
Video scoring from a live feed |
Random videos coded for interrater reliability: correlation analysis (r > 0.97) |
|
Looking duration to switch in direction of action goal |
Duration of looking time at an animated event on a computer screen which resembled the contingent training but with change in direction (Red ball being hit by green ball) |
Measures infant's ability to discriminate the reversal in agent-recipient roles in other's actions |
|
Looking duration to switch in causality of action goal |
Duration of looking time at an animated event on a computer screen which was displayed in same direction as training but there was no contingency/causality (Green ball moved towards red ball but did not hit it) |
Measures infant's ability to discriminate noncausality in other's actions |
Gerson and Woodward37,38 (2014) |
Looking duration to change in action goal structure |
Causal cognition |
Duration of looking time at a staged event with a switch in action goal compared with a familiarization trial (Familiarization: Experimenter showed infant 2 toys, ie, bear and ball and touched the ball with mitten) (Test: Experimenter showed infant 2 toys, ie, bear and ball and touched the bear with mitten) |
Measures infant's ability to discriminate goal structure in other's actions. Indicates change in information-processing abilities after self-produced actions |
Video coding using the Java Habituation software (version 1.0.0) |
100% double coding for interrater reliability: correlation analysis (r ≥ 0.94) |
Williams et al39 (2015) |
Total intentional toy contacts |
Motor and visual cognition |
Frequency of times the infant made hand toy contact while looking at it either prior or during the contact. |
Measures “intentionality” during early reaching |
Video coding using the Observer XT (Noldus Information Technology BV, Wageningen, The Netherlands) |
30% of videos coded for interrater reliability: correlation analysis (r = 0.92) |
|
Total toy contacts |
Motor |
Frequency of times the infant made hand toy contact without consideration to the visual attention/looking |
Measures early reaching abilities |
|
Visual attention to toy |
Visual cognition |
Duration of time the infant spent looking at the toy |
Measures engagement in exploration |
20% of videos coded for interrater reliability. Cohen κ values (0.70-0.95), average = 0.84 |
|
Peak velocity of reaching |
Motor |
Average peak velocity of reaching was calculated by dividing the values of the velocity peaks by the total number of velocity peaks in the time series |
Measures the kinematics of reaching, ie, speed of hand while approaching the target. Slower speed indicates better control |
Collected using Mini Flock of Birds motion analysis system (Ascension Technology Corp, Burlington, Vermont) and processed using MATLAB |
Not reported |
Wiesen et al40 (2016) |
Toy touches |
Motor |
Duration of time the infant made manual contact with the toy |
Measures early reaching abilities |
Frame-by-frame video coding using StopFrameCoding software (Version 0.9) |
Random sample recoded for interrater reliability: Looking (ICC = 0.91), touching (ICC = 0.96), reaching (ICC = 0.85), bimanual (ICC = 0.90), and grasping (ICC = 0.99) |
|
Reaching |
Duration of time the infant made a movement of the hand toward the toy |
|
Grasping |
Duration of time the infant's finger encircled or gripped the toy |
Measures early object manipulation abilities |
|
Bimanual exploration |
Duration of time the infant touched the toy with both hands |
|
Visual attention to toy |
Visual cognition |
Duration of time the infant's gaze was directed toward the toy |
Measures engagement in exploration |
Needham et al41 (2017) |
Total toy touches |
Motor |
Duration of time the infant spent with any of the hands or mouth in contact with the toy |
Measures early object manipulation abilities |
Frame-by-frame video coding using StopFrameCoding software (Version 0.9) |
20% of videos were coded for interrater reliability: looking (ICC = 0.96), touching (ICC = 0.99) |
|
Visual attention to toy |
Visual cognition |
Duration of time the infant spent in visual contact with the toy |
Measures engagement in exploration |
|
|
Nascimento et al42 (2019) |
Total number of reaches |
Motor |
Number of times the infant contacted the toy with one or both hands, regardless of grasping |
Measures early reaching abilities |
Video coding using the ReSpeedr software |
20% of videos were coded for interrater |
|
|
|
|
|
|
reliability: Cohen's κ = .86 (95% CI ± 0.01) |
|
Total number of proximal hand adjustment, ie, bimanual reaches |
|
Number of times the infant moved both hands simultaneously toward the toy for at least 50% of the trajectory, resulting in a touch |
Measures motor adaptations during early reaching |
|
|
|
Total number and type of distal hand adjustment while reaching |
|
Number of times the infant reached for a toy with hand closed versus hand open |
|
|
|
|
Grasping |
|
Number of times the infant held/grabbed the toy with fingers using one or both hands after reaching |
Measures early object manipulation abilities |
|
|
Heathcock et al43 (2008); Heathcock and Galloway44 (2009) |
Average hand-toy contacts |
Motor |
Average number of times the infant contacted the toy per assessment |
Measures early reaching abilities |
Video coding using custom program |
20% of videos were coded for interrater reliability. Percent agreement was >90% |
|
Hand-toy contact duration |
Proportion of time the infant contacted the toy for across all testing trials per assessment |
Measures early object manipulation abilities |
|
Hand-toy contact type |
Proportion of time the infant contacted the toy with dorsal aspect of hand versus ventral aspect and open hand versus closed hand, across all testing trials per assessment |
Measures complexity of object manipulation. Contact with ventral surface and open hand represents a more functional grasp |
|
Foot-toy contact frequency |
Number of times the infant contacted the toy with any part of its foot or toes |
Measures early feet-reaching behaviors |
|
Foot-toy contact duration |
The average amount of time infants spent touching the toy per foot-toy contact |
Needham et al45 (2014) |
Manual toy contact |
Motor |
Duration of time the infant spent touching the toy with hand or fingers when it was placed in its hand |
Measures early object manipulation abilities |
Frame-by-frame video coding using StopFrameCoding software (Version 0.9) |
26% of videos coded for interrater reliability: Percent agreement was maintained at 94.28% |
|
Bimanual exploration |
Duration of time the infant spent contacting the toy with both hands simultaneously, when it was placed in its hand |
Measures early object manipulation abilities |
|
Reaching |
Duration of time the infant moved its hand and arm in the direction of the toy. Contact with toy was not necessary |
Measures early reaching abilities |
|
Visual attention to toy |
Visual cognition |
Duration of time the infant spent looking at the toy when it was placed out of its reach |
Measures engagement in exploration |
|
Visual attention to experimenter |
Duration of time the infant made contact with experimenter's body or face |
Measures engagement in exploration and attention to task |
Campbell et al46 (2015) |
Average leg movement frequency |
Motor |
Frequency of times the hip was observed to move more than about 15° in either direction from the resting position in the slings of approximately 90° of hip and knee flexion |
|
Video coding |
Random videos coded for interrater reliability: percent agreement was 89% |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Interlimb coordination patterns |
Coded in 3 ways (Reference leg = Right): 1. Ipsilateral leg movement 2. Alternate (left leg extending within 1 s of right hip flexion) 3. Synchronous (both hips flexing within 1 s of each other) |
|
|
|
Williams and Corbetta47 (2016) |
Visually attended toy contacts |
Motor and visual cognition |
Frequency of times the infant made hand-toy contact while looking at it either prior or during the contact |
Measures “intentionality” during early reaching |
Video coding using the with the ObserverXT9 (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands) |
20% of videos were coded for interrater reliability. Percent agreement was 91% |
|
Total toy contacts |
Motor |
Frequency of times the infant made hand-toy contact without consideration to the visual attention/looking |
Measures early reaching abilities |
|
Visual attention to toy |
Visual cognition |
Duration of time the infant spent looking at the toy |
Measures engagement in exploration |
20% of videos were coded for interrater reliability: percent agreement was 85% |
|
Mean peak velocity of reaching |
Motor |
Average peak velocity of reaching was calculated by dividing the values of the velocity peaks by the total number of velocity peaks in the time series |
Measures the kinematics of reaching, ie, speed of hand while approaching the target. Slower speed indicates better control |
Collected using Mini Flock of Birds motion analysis system (Ascension Technology Corp, Burlington, Vermont) and processed using MATLAB |
Not reported |
Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; ICC, intraclass correlation coefficient (measure of reliability).
TABLE 3 -
Results for Effectiveness of Included Studies
Study |
Feeding Outcomes |
Motor and Cognitive Outcomes |
Favors Intervention Group (P)a |
Favors Control Group/No Difference Between Groups (P)a |
Standley28 (2003) |
Oral feeding rate |
|
<.05 |
|
Standley et al29 (2010) |
Number of oral feeding days before discharge Reduction in length of gavage feeding days |
|
<.05 (at 34-wk adjusted age and with 3 sessions of intervention) |
>.05 <.05 (at 32-wk adjusted age) |
Chorna et al 30 (2014) |
Oral feeding rate and frequency Oral feeding volume Time/days to oral feeding |
|
<.001 .001 .04 Average 7 d faster |
|
Needham et al31 (2002) |
|
Intentional swats Visual attention to toy Multimodal exploration (visual + oral) |
<.05 <.001 <.05 |
|
Libertus and Needham32,33 (2010, 2011) |
|
Reaching and grasping Visual attention to toy Visual attention to experimenter Face preference Face orienting |
.009 .011 .03 .022 .004 |
|
Libertus et al34 (2016) Follow-up to Libertus and Needham32 (2010) |
|
12-mo follow-up to Libertus and Needham32 (2010) Toy grasping Toy rotations Visual attention to toy Distraction |
.012 .004 .004 <.001 |
|
Libertus and Needham35 (2014) |
|
Reaching and grasping Visual attention to toy Face preference |
.033 .019 |
>.121 |
Rakison and Krogh36 (2012) |
|
Looking duration to switch in direction of action goal Looking duration to switch in causality of action goal |
<.05 |
>.05 |
Gerson and Woodward37,38 (2014) |
|
Looking duration to change in action goal structure |
.021 .034 |
|
Williams et al39 (2015) |
|
Total toy contacts Total intentional toy contacts Peak velocity of reaching Visual attention to toy |
.010 |
>.05 .047 <.0001 |
Wiesen et al40 (2016) |
|
Postintervention Toy touching and reaching Visual attention to toy 2-mo follow-up Toy touching Grasping Reaching Bimanual exploration Visual attention to toy |
.025 .004 .005 <.001 |
>.05 >.05 .011 |
Needham et al41 (2017) |
|
Toy touching Visual attention to toy |
.008 |
.077 |
Nascimento et al42 (2019) |
|
Postintervention Total number of reaches Number of bimanual reaches Open vs closed hand reaches Grasping Retention test (4 min later) Total number of reaches Number of bimanual reaches Open vs closed hand reaches Grasping |
<.01
.01
<.01 |
>.63 >.05 .07
>.30 >.05 |
Heathcock et al43 (2008)
Heathcock and Galloway44 (2009) |
|
Average hand-toy contacts Hand-toy contact duration Number of open hand-toy contacts Number of ventral toy contacts Foot-toy contact frequency Hand-toy contact duration |
<.01
<.05
<.01 <.002
.000 <.045 |
|
Needham et al45 (2014) |
|
Manual toy-contact Bimanual exploration Reaching toward toy Visual attention to toy Visual attention to experimenter |
.025 |
.798 .723
.957 .897 |
Campbell et al46 (2015) |
|
Average leg movement frequency Interlimb coordination patterns, ie, more synchronous hip flexion movements |
|
.121
.932 |
Williams and Corbetta47 (2016) |
|
Total toy contacts Visually attended toy contacts Mean peak velocity of reaching Visual attention to toy |
.044 <.05 |
.011 <.05 |
aP ≤ .05 indicates significant results.
ROB and Quality Assessment Within Studies
Of the 17 studies selected, 10 were RCTs28–30,36,40–43,46,47 and 7 were QRCTs.31–35,37,39,45 Based on Sackett's grading, all the RCTs had level II evidence (sample size <100) and QRCTs had level III evidence. According to Cochrane ROB-2 for RCTs, 6 RCTs had threats,28,40–43,47 and 4 RCTs had a low ROB (Table 4).29,30,46 Based on ROBINS-I, 3 QRCTs had serious ROB,31,35,45 3 had moderate ROB,32,37,39 and 1 had a low ROB (Table 5).36
TABLE 4 -
Cochrane Risk of Bias (ROB-2) for Randomized Controlled Trials
Study |
Domain 1: Randomization Process |
Domain 2: Deviation From Interventions |
Domain 3: Missing Outcome Data |
Domain 4: Measurement of Outcome |
Domain 5: Selection of Reported Result |
Overall Risk of Bias |
Standley28 (2003) |
Some concerns |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Some concerns |
Some concerns |
Standley29 et al (2010) |
Some concerns |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Chorna et al30 (2014) |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Rakison and Krogh36 (2012) |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Wiesen et al40 (2016) |
Some concerns |
Some concerns |
Low |
Some concerns |
Low |
Some concerns |
Needham et al41 (2017) |
Low |
Some concerns |
Low |
Some concerns |
Low |
Some concerns |
Nascimento et al42 (2019) |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Some concerns |
Some concerns |
Heathcock et al43 (2008) |
Low |
Some concerns |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Some concerns |
Heathcock and Galloway44 (2009) |
Low |
Some concerns |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Some concerns |
Campbell et al46 (2015) |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Williams and Corbetta47 (2016) |
Some concerns |
Low |
Low |
Some concerns |
Low |
Some concerns |
TABLE 5 -
ROBINS-I Risk of Bias Assessment for Nonrandomized Controlled Trials
|
Before Intervention |
At Intervention |
After Intervention |
|
Study |
Confounding |
Selection of Participant |
Classification of Intervention |
Deviation From Intended Intervention |
Missing Data |
Measurement of Outcome |
Selection of Reported Result |
Overall ROB |
Needham et al31 (2002) |
Serious |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Moderate |
Low |
Moderate |
Serious |
Libertus and Needham32 (2010) |
Moderate |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Moderate |
Low |
Moderate |
Moderate |
Libertus and Needham33 (2011) |
Moderate |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Moderate |
Moderate |
Moderate |
Moderate |
Libertus et al34 (2016) |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Libertus and Needham35 (2014) |
Serious |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Serious |
Low |
Serious |
Gerson and Woodward37 (2014) |
Moderate |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Moderate |
Low |
Moderate |
Gerson and Woodward38 (2014) |
Moderate |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Moderate |
Low |
Moderate |
Williams et al39 (2015) |
Moderate |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Serious |
Low |
Moderate |
Moderate |
Needham et al45 (2014) |
Serious |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Low |
Serious |
Serious |
Abbreviation: ROB, risk of bias.
CPBI for Feeding Outcomes
Three studies used CPBI to improve feeding outcomes28–30 and comprised a total of 194 preterm infants between 32 and 36 weeks of adjusted age (AA) at treatment onset. The CPBI used in all 3 studies was pacifier-activated lullaby (PAL), with 1 study specifically recording the lullabies in the mother's voice.30 The PAL interventions positively reinforce the preterm infant's nonnutritive sucking (NNS) by associating the suck with a lullaby whenever the infant's suck reaches a threshold pressure. The control groups received usual care for NNS. The PAL intervention was provided once per day, each session lasting for 15 to 20 minutes. Frequency varied from 1 to 5 days. The PAL intervention was provided 30 to 60 minutes before the scheduled feeding time (see Supplemental Digital Content 3, available at: https://links.lww.com/PPT/A352).
Outcomes were assessed at baseline and postintervention. Primary outcomes measured nutritive sucking, that is, number of oral feeding days before discharge, oral feeding rate, volume, and frequency. Secondary outcomes included number of gavage feeding days and days to transition to oral feeding (Table 2).
The PAL intervention significantly improved oral feeding rate (P < .05),28,30 frequency (P < .001),30 and volume (P = .001)30 in preterm infants compared with usual care. No differences between groups were reported for number of oral feeding days before discharge.29 For the secondary outcomes, PAL trials significantly reduced gavage feeding days (P < .05) when initiated at 34 weeks of AA.29 This reduction was significantly greater with 3 intervention sessions when compared with 1 session (P < .05). The PAL intervention provided at 32 weeks of AA had a negative outcome, that is, it lengthened the gavage feeding days significantly (P < .05). Finally, PAL intervention reported a faster transition to oral feeding (P = .04), that is, an average of 7 days earlier than the control group.30
CPBI for Motor and Cognitive Outcomes
Fourteen studies used contingency paradigms as either the primary or one of the key components of intervention to improve motor and cognitive outcomes in infants.31,32,34–37,39–43,46,47 Eleven studies included term born infants31,32,34–37,39–41,45,47 (n = 421) and 3 studies included preterm infants42,43,46 (n = 63) resulting in a total of 484 infants across the 14 studies. Age at treatment onset had a range of 2 to 5 months.
The CPBI comprised either sticky mittens/socks or contingent toys. In sticky mittens training, infants are fitted with Velcro mittens and the corresponding end of the Velcro is attached to light-weight toys. The toys stick to the mitten when infants try to contact them either accidently or on purpose providing positive reinforcement for reaching. Sticky socks are similar and are used to encourage lower extremity movements in infants. Contingent toys in this review refer to toys that provide reinforcement in response to the infant's active movement and include an overhead mobile attached via ribbon to infant's arms or legs, modified toys that made a sound, light up, or oscillated on touch, and socks with attached auditory reinforcements. Control groups included noncontingent interventions or no training (see Supplemental Digital Content 3, available at: https://links.lww.com/PPT/A352).
Outcomes were assessed at baseline and immediately postintervention in 12 studies31,32,35–37,39,41–43,46,47 and at 2 months40 and 12 months34 postintervention in 2 studies. Motor outcomes included (i) manual exploration behaviors such as touching, swatting, grasping, bimanual explores, and object rotations, and (ii) feet exploration behaviors such as kicking or foot-toy contacts. Cognitive outcomes included measures of early cognition such as (i) visual cognition, that is, visual attention to the toy, people, and environment, and shifts in visual attention, (ii) social cognition, that is, face preference, and (iii) causal cognition, that is, action goal understanding (Table 2).
Eight studies used a closed sticky mitten training protocol31,32,34–37,40,41 in term infants. Effectiveness of closed sticky mittens on motor outcomes was measured in 6 studies,31,32,34,40,41,48 and 4 of the 6 studies reported improvements in intentional swatting, reaching, and grasping (P < .05) in prereaching term infants.31,32,35,41 An increase in bimanual exploration (P = .005), grasping (P = .004, P = .012), and object rotations (P = .004) was observed at 2-month40 and 12-month34 follow-up. Closed sticky mittens improved manual motor outcomes only when training was provided for minimum 10 sessions (total 100 minutes of training).
Eight studies measured the effectiveness of closed sticky mittens on 1or more cognitive outcomes.31,32,34–37,40,41 Six of the 8 studies measured visual cognition31,32,34,35,40,41 and 5 of the 6 studies reported an increase in visual attention and gaze shifts (P < .05) immediately postintervention31,32,41 or at a follow-up in term infants.34,40 Two of the 8 studies measured social cognition33,35 and reported increased face preference in infants in the intervention group (P = .02 and P = .019). Closed sticky mittens improved visual and social cognition only when training was provided for minimum 10 sessions (total 100 minutes of training). Finally, 2 of the 8 studies measured causal cognition36,37 and reported an increase in action-goal understanding in term infants in the intervention group after a single 3-minute training session (P < .05).
One study used an open sticky mitten training protocol in term infants39 and reported that the control group (nonsticky mittens) improved significantly in reaching and visual attention compared with the open sticky mitten group (P = .047). Finally, 1 study42 used an individualized open sticky mitten training in late preterm infants and reported an immediate increase in reaching and bimanual exploration in the sticky mitten group following a single 4-minute session; however, this effect was transient and not retained.
Four studies used contingent toys43–47 and 3 measured manual exploration.43,45,47 Two of 3 studies reported improvements in intentional toy contacts in the contingent toy group in term and preterm infants, respectively (P = .04, P < .01).43,47 In the study including preterm infants, intensive dosage of intervention was provided (total 600 minutes of training), and it included additional movement training components.43 The remaining one study that did not report change in manual exploration included a single session of 9-minute training.45
Two studies measured the effectiveness of contingent toy training on feet exploration in preterm infants,44,46 with 1 study specifically including preterm infants with periventricular brain injury (PBI).46 The study including preterm infants without PBI reported improvements in feet-toy contacts in very preterm infants.44 The study including preterm infants with PBI reported no additional benefits of contingent toy training. However, a longitudinal follow-up revealed that higher number of infants (diagnosed as typically developing at 12 months) in the contingent toy group were walking independently at 12 months of age.46 Two studies measured visual attention toward the toy and reported no effect of contingent toy training on visual cognition.45,47
DISCUSSION
This systematic review identified 17 unique controlled trials using CPBI for improving feeding, motor, and cognitive outcomes in infants.28–32,34–37,39,40–43,45–47 Our review suggests that interventions based on contingency paradigms have potential for application to physical therapy practice, starting from NICU to early intervention in infancy. In the following sections, we will highlight the clinical implications for these findings based on each intervention, that is, PAL, sticky mittens, and contingent toys and its target outcome (feeding, motor, and/or cognitive).
Pacifier-Activated Lullaby
This CPBI was primarily applied in the NICU setting for infants born extremely to very preterm to improve feeding outcomes. Our results support that PAL intervention can reinforce NNS, improve nutritive sucking, and reduce number of gavage feeding days for extremely to very preterm infants.28–30 This finding is important as nutritive sucking can be a challenge for very preterm infants due to underdeveloped oral-motor skills, poor sucking endurance, and inability to coordinate motor and autonomic systems.49 The NNS experience during gavage feeding is crucial for facilitation of functional feeding. Moreover, ability to perform nutritive sucking with no cardiorespiratory comprise is one of the criteria for discharge to home for preterm infants.49 Although contingency learning for feeding has been demonstrated in infants at as early as 32 weeks of gestational age, most of the studies refer to training-specific learning.50 Our results support that reinforcement provided by CPBI during NNS can transfer to improved performance during functional feeding. We identified 3 important parameters that need to be considered for implementing CPBI for feeding in very preterm infants: (i) age of initiation, (ii) salience of reinforcer, and (iii) dosage of training. Most optimal feeding outcomes are achieved when CPBI are initiated at or after 34 weeks of AA.29 This could be because coordinated suck-swallow function starts establishing around 34 to 36 weeks of gestational age in infants.49 Moreover, preterm infants younger than 34 weeks may not tolerate CPBI due to poor readiness to oral feeding caused by intubation, fatigue, and impaired state regulation.49 More significant gains in feeding were observed when mother's voice was used as an auditory reinforcer.30 Biological sounds such as mother's voice are identified as strong auditory reinforcers in existing literature.51 Mother's voice can also simulate womb-like auditory experience for the infants and assist with state regulation, positively impacting feeding.52 Finally, a minimum of 3 days of training is required to see changes with CPBI for feeding in very preterm infants aged 34 weeks of AA and older. This finding corroborates with previous studies, which state that preterm infants need more practice than full-term infants to demonstrate learning in contingency learning paradigms.13,53
Sticky Mittens
Sticky mittens constituted the majority of CPBI used for improving motor and cognitive outcomes in young infants. All but one of these studies were focused on term born infants aged 2 to 3 months.31,32,34–37,39–41 The results suggest that closed sticky mitten training, when provided in the “prereaching” stage, increases reaching, manual exploration, visual attention, gaze shifts, and face preference in term born infants.31,32,34,35,40,41 Several mechanisms have been proposed for the effectiveness of closed sticky mittens in term infants. First, sticky mittens reward the initial reaching attempts of infants allowing them to identify the effect of their self-generated actions on the environment, thus, motivating them to repeat these actions.41 Second, early interaction with objects during reaching allows infants to locate the position of their hand in space, contributing to the refinement of visual attention and development of complex visual skills such as gaze shifts.54 Third, the ability to successfully act on the environment using closed sticky mittens may help young infants see themselves as intentional agents in the world, improving nonverbal communication with caregivers in the form of increased face preference.33 Preliminary evidence from 2 studies also shows that closed sticky mittens training can help infants distinguish goal structure in other people's actions when tested in the laboratory setting.36,37 Action-goal understanding is the foundation for social referencing, language learning, and imitative learning in the second year of life.38 This finding should be tested in a real-world setting before making conclusive recommendations.
To summarize, the studies on closed sticky mittens provide promising results for improving manual motor skills and cognitive precursors in term infants. However, the overall effectiveness of the sticky mitten paradigm requires a more critical analysis. The 2 studies using the open sticky mittens paradigm reported no effect and a transient effect of sticky mitten training, respectively.39,42 Our TIDieR analysis (see Supplemental Digital Content 3, available at: https://links.lww.com/PPT/A352) revealed crucial differences in intervention protocols for closed versus open sticky mittens, which make comparisons and overall clinical recommendations difficult for sticky mittens. Based on the TIDieR analysis, we identified the following prerequisites for the sticky mitten paradigm to be effective: (a) demonstration of the contingency at the beginning of training, (b) parental verbal encouragement during the training, and (c) a dosage of minimum 10 sessions across 2 weeks (total 100 minutes of reaching training).
Although only 1 study in this review tested effectiveness of sticky mittens training in preterm infants,42 this training has potential for improving reaching in preterm infants. The onset of reaching is delayed in infants born preterm and this delay has been attributed to both intrinsic (presence of brain injury, lower muscle tone, poor muscle strength and coordination, postural control, and perceptual deficits) and extrinsic factors (extrauterine environment and movement experiences).55 Results for sticky mittens from term infants show that this training significantly increases reaching attempts. Replicating this finding in preterm infants can prove to be valuable, as increased number of reaches can positively predict neurodevelopment at 2.5 years in very preterm infants.56 In addition, training prereaching preterm infants using sticky mittens may help minimize the gravitational demands of “successful reaching,” while allowing opportunities for refining arm control and improving proximal muscle strength. Finally, sticky mittens afford reaching success without demanding grasping precision. Thus, reducing constraints to reaching using sticky mittens may facilitate repetition and motor learning, especially in preterm infants.57
Contingent Toys
Results from limited studies demonstrate that contingent toys can improve reaching and manual exploration in term and preterm infants aged 2 to 3 months, when provided in a dosage of minimum 10 sessions across 2 weeks (total 100 minutes of training).43,47 Contingent toy intervention when provided in an intensive dosage (minimum 400 minutes of training across 8 weeks) may also improve feet exploration or kicking in preterm infants. However, this finding is based on 1 study,44 which included preterm infants without brain injury and incorporated additional movement training components such as midline foot-toy contact and leg movement dissociation in training. Another study using similar dosage of “isolated” contingent toy training for kicking in preterm infants with brain injury did not report similar benefits.46 We hypothesize that scaffolded movement training combined with reinforcements may be necessary for motor learning in preterm infants. Moreover, presence of brain injury may impact contingency learning abilities in preterm infants requiring either increased dosage or learning support for noticing gains. Although limited, the evidence of CPBI combined with movement training is of value as atypical selective motor control during kicking is predictive of delayed walking attainment in preterm infants. Thus, CPBI need to be further explored for training feet exploration.
LIMITATIONS
The inclusion of both term and preterm infants in this review could inflate the effectiveness of CPBI. However, this decision was made because of the lack of existing studies on CPBI in preterm infants. We outline the implications for results from term infants to design and modify interventions for preterm infants. Seven studies in this review were NRCTs with level III evidence, with majority having moderate to serious ROB.31–35,37,39,45 None of the 10 RCTs had a level I evidence, reducing the methodological rigor of the studies. All but 2 studies34,40 in this review were cross-sectional, which limits the findings to infants aged 6 months and younger, and no conclusions on the long-term effect of CPBI on developmental outcomes can be drawn. Despite many studies in this review, we refrained from performing a meta-analysis due to inconsistent reporting of outcomes between studies. None of the outcomes in the studies in this review were standardized motor and cognitive measures. Although task-specific outcomes provide valuable information, they lack stringent psychometric properties that limit confidence in the effectiveness of contingency paradigms on global development. Although we planned on reporting effect sizes, the variation in number of behavioral outcomes would have impacted the interpretation of effect size. Hence, we report only the P values. Finally, our strict search criteria may have failed to include studies that have contingent paradigms built into the intervention7,58,59 and relevant literature in other languages.
IMPLICATIONS FOR CLINICAL PRACTICE
The results of this review have several important clinical implications. The CPBI can be implemented by therapists in the NICU to promote optimal feeding outcomes in preterm infants and are most effective when initiated at 34 weeks of gestational age. Early reaching practice using closed sticky mitten protocol may increase reaching behaviors and set the stage for early onset of perceptual and cognitive milestones in term infants. Physical therapists can modify and use commercially available, inexpensive toys such as bells, rattles, and light up toy to set up a contingent response for training reaching and kicking in preterm infants, combined with intensive movement practice.43,44 Infants with or at risk for delays often need more practice and may demonstrate lower levels of persistence. The CPBI can address both these barriers and have the potential to advance visual, social, and causal cognition in infants at risk for delays.
IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH
Improvement in feeding outcomes after CPBI in preterm infants highlights their ability for contingency learning before term age. Additional research is needed to test the effectiveness of contingency paradigms for facilitating age-appropriate motor and cognitive skills in preterm infants in the NICU and its effect on later developmental outcomes. Our results show that CPBI can be an effective treatment strategy for improving reaching, visual attention, and face preference in term infants; however, its translation to at-risk infants is needed. Future research should also disentangle the role of movement practice and parental scaffolding from reinforcement training. The closed and open sticky mittens use different training protocols. More research is needed to determine whether one approach is better than the other. Objective documentation of other possible mechanisms for improvement in reaching with sticky mittens such as improved muscle strength and coordination, proprioceptive feedback, or sense of self-efficacy is needed. The CPBI may also have the potential to improve additional motor milestones such as head control development, prone position play,60,61 rolling, sitting, and walking. High-quality RCTs are needed to evaluate these theoretical findings. Future studies also need to measure the effectiveness of CPBI using robust study designs, standardized motor measures, and objective early cognitive measurement tools such as eye trackers.62
REFERENCES
1. Gibson E. Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. Annu Rev Psychol. 1988;39(1):1–41. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.39.1.1.
2. Masten AS, Cicchetti D. Developmental cascades. Dev Psychopathol. 2010;22(3):491–495. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000222.
3. Libertus K, Violi DA. Sit to talk: relation between motor skills and language development in infancy. Front Psychol. 2016;7:475. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00475.
4. Clearfield MW. Learning to walk changes infants' social interactions. Infant Behav Dev. 2011;34(1):15–25. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.04.008.
5. Khurana S, Kane AE, Brown SE, Tarver T, Dusing SC. Effect of neonatal therapy on the motor, cognitive, and behavioral development of infants born preterm: a systematic review. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2020;62(6):684–692. doi:10.1111/dmcn.14485.
6. Spittle A, Orton J, Anderson PJ, Boyd R, Doyle LW. Early developmental intervention programmes provided post hospital discharge to prevent motor and cognitive impairment in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015;2015(11):CD005495. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD005495.pub4.
7. Harbourne RT, Dusing SC, Lobo MA, et al. START-Play physical therapy intervention impacts motor and cognitive outcomes in infants with neuromotor disorders: a multisite randomized clinical trial. Phys Ther. 2021;101(2):pzaa232. doi:10.1093/ptj/pzaa232.
8. Morgan C, Darrah J, Gordon AM, et al. Effectiveness of motor interventions in infants with cerebral palsy: a systematic review. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2016;58(9):900–909. doi:10.1111/dmcn.13105.
9. Lobo MA, Harbourne RT, Dusing SC, McCoy SW. Grounding early intervention: physical therapy cannot just be about motor skills anymore. Phys Ther. 2013;93(1):94–103
10. Christy JB, Lobo MA, Bjornson K, et al. Technology for children with brain injury and motor disability: executive summary from research summit IV. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2016;28(4):483–489. doi:10.1097/PEP.0000000000000317.
11. Gewirtz JL, Peláez-Nogueras M. B.F. Skinner's legacy in human infant behavior and development. Am Psychol. 1992;47(11):1411–1422. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.47.11.1411.
12. Sen U, Gredebäck G. Making the world behave: a new embodied account on mobile paradigm. Front Syst Neurosci. 2021;15:643526. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2021.643526.
13. Heathcock JC, Bhat AN, Lobo MA, Galloway J. The performance of infants born preterm and full-term in the mobile paradigm: learning and memory. Phys Ther. 2004;84(9):808–821. doi:10.1093/ptj/84.9.808.
14. Sargent B, Schweighofer N, Kubo M, Fetters L Infant exploratory learning: influence on leg joint coordination. PLoS One. 2014;9(3):e91500. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0091500.
15. Angulo-Kinzler RM, Ulrich B, Thelen E. Three-month-old infants can select specific leg motor solutions. Motor Control. 2002;6(1):52–68. doi:10.1123/mcj.6.1.52.
16. Tripathi T, Dusing S, Pidcoe PE, Xu Y, Shall MS, Riddle DL. A motor learning paradigm combining technology and associative learning to assess prone motor learning in infants. Phys Ther. 2019;99(6):807–816. doi:10.1093/ptj/pzz066.
17. Morgan C, Novak I, Badawi N. Enriched environments and motor outcomes in cerebral palsy: systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2013;132(3):e735–e746.
https://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/pediatrics/132/3/e735.full.pdf. Accessed September 8, 2019.
18. Novak I, Morgan C, Adde L, et al. Early, accurate diagnosis and early intervention in cerebral palsy: advances in diagnosis and treatment. JAMA Pediatr. 2017;171(9):897–907. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.1689.
19. Dusing SC, Harbourne RT. Variability in postural control during infancy: implications for development, assessment, and intervention. Phys Ther. 2010;90(12):1838–1849. doi:10.2522/ptj.2010033.
20. Dusing SC, Kyvelidou A, Mercer VS, Stergiou N Infants born preterm exhibit different patterns of center-of-pressure movement than infants born at full term. Phys Ther. 2009;89(12):1354–1362. doi:10. 2522/ptj.20080361.
21. Kolobe THA, Christy JB, Gannotti ME, et al. Research summit III proceedings on dosing in children with an injured brain or cerebral palsy: executive summary. Phys Ther. 2014;94(7):907–920. doi:10.2522/ptj.20130024.
22. Jewell DV. Guide to Evidence-Based Physical Therapist Practice. Burlington, MA: Jones & Bartlett Learning; 2014.
23. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. J Clin Epidemiol. 2009;62(10):e1–e34. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.006.
24. Hoffmann TC, Glasziou PP, Boutron I, et al. Better reporting of interventions: Template for Intervention Description and Replication (TIDieR) checklist and guide. BMJ. 2014;348:g1687. doi:10.1136/bmj.g1687.
25. Sterne JA, Hernán MA, Reeves BC, et al. ROBINS-I: a tool for assessing risk of bias in non-randomised studies of interventions. BMJ. 2016;355:i4919. doi:10.1136/bmj.i4919.
26. Sterne JAC, Savović J, Page MJ, et al. RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2019;366:l4898. doi:10.1136/bmj.l4898.
27. Darrah J, Hickman R, O'Donnell M, Vogtle L, Wiart L. AACPDM Methodology to Develop Systematic Reviews of Treatment Interventions (Revision 1. 2) 2008 Version The Step-by-Step Process for Authors Creating an AACPDM Systematic Review. Milwaukee, WI: AACPDM; 2008:1–30.
28. Standley JM. The effect of music-reinforced nonnutritive sucking on feeding rate of premature infants. J Pediatr Nurs. 2003;18(3):169–173. doi:10.1053/jpdn.2003.34.
29. Standley JM, Cassidy J, Grant R, et al. The effect of music reinforcement for non-nutritive sucking on nipple feeding of premature infants. Pediatr Nurs. 2010;36(3):138–145.
30. Chorna OD, Slaughter JC, Wang L, Stark AR, Maitre NL. A pacifier-activated music player with mother's voice improves oral feeding in preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2014;133(3):462–468. doi:10.1542/peds.2013-2547.
31. Needham A, Barrett T, Peterman K. A pick-me-up for infants' exploratory skills: early simulated experiences reaching for objects using “sticky mittens” enhances young infants object exploration skills. Infant Behav Dev. 2002;25(3):279–295. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00097-8.
32. Libertus K, Needham A. Teach to reach: the effects of active vs. passive reaching experiences on action and perception. Vision Res. 2010;50(24):2750–2757. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.001.
33. Libertus K, Needham A. Reaching experience increases face preference in 3-month-old infants. Dev Sci. 2011;14(6):1355–1364. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01084.x.
34. Libertus K, Joh AS, Needham AW. Motor training at 3 months affects object exploration 12 months later. Dev Sci. 2016;19(6):1058–1066. doi:10.1111/desc.12370.
35. Libertus K, Needham A. Encouragement is nothing without control: factors influencing the development of reaching and face preference. J Mot Learn Dev. 2014;2(1):16–27. doi:10.1123/jmld.2013-0019.
36. Rakison DH, Krogh L. Does causal action facilitate causal perception in infants younger than 6 months of age? Dev Sci. 2012;15(1):43–53. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01096.x.
37. Gerson SA, Woodward AL. Learning from their own actions: the unique effect of producing actions on infants' action understanding. Child Dev. 2014;85(1):264–277. doi:10.1111/cdev.12115.
38. Gerson SA, Woodward AL. The joint role of trained, untrained, and observed actions at the origins of goal recognition. Infant Behav Dev. 2014;37(1):94–104. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.12.013.
39. Williams JL, Corbetta D, Guan Y. Learning to reach with “sticky” or “non-sticky” mittens: a tale of developmental trajectories. Infant Behav Dev. 2015;38:82–96. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.01.001.
40. Wiesen SE, Watkins RM, Needham AW. Active motor training has long-term effects on infants' object exploration. Front Psychol. 2016;7:599. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00599.
41. Needham AW, Wiesen SE, Hejazi JN, Libertus K, Christopher C. Characteristics of brief sticky mittens training that lead to increases in object exploration. J Exp Child Psychol. 2017;164:209–224. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.009.
42. Nascimento AL, Toledo AM, Merey LF, Tudella E, Soares-Marangoni DA. Brief reaching training with “sticky mittens” in preterm infants: randomized controlled trial. Hum Mov Sci. 2019;63(November 2018):138–147. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2018.11.015.
43. Heathcock JC, Lobo M, Galloway JC. Movement training advances the emergence of reaching in infants born at less than 33 weeks of gestational age: a randomized clinical trial. Phys Ther. 2008;88(3):310–322. doi:10.2522/ptj.20070145.
44. Heathcock JC, Galloway JCC. Exploring objects with feet advances movement in infants born preterm: a randomized controlled trial. Phys Ther. 2009;89(10):1027–1038. doi:10.2522/ptj.20080278.
45. Needham A, Joh AS, Wiesen SE, Williams N. Effects of contingent reinforcement of actions on infants' object-directed reaching. Infancy. 2014;19(5):496–517. doi:10.1111/infa.12058.
46. Campbell SK, Cole W, Boynewicz K, et al. Behavior during tethered kicking in infants with periventricular brain injury. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2015;27(4):403–412. doi:10.1097/PEP.0000000000000173.
47. Williams JL, Corbetta D. Assessing the impact of movement consequences on the development of early reaching in infancy. Front Psychol. 2016;7:587. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00587.
48. Libertus K, Landa RJ. Scaffolded reaching experiences encourage grasping activity in infants at high risk for autism. Front Psychol. 2014;5:1071. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01071.
49. Garber J. Oral-motor function and feeding intervention. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr. 2013;33(1):111–138. doi:10.3109/01942638.2012.750864.
50. Gerhardstein P, Kraebel K, Tse J. Using operant techniques with humans infants. Behav Anal Today. 2006;7(1):56–69. doi:10.1037/h0100142.
51. Pelaez M, Monlux K. Operant conditioning methodologies to investigate infant learning. Eur J Behav Anal. 2017;18(2):212–241. doi:10.1080/15021149.2017.1412633.
52. Webb AR, Heller HT, Benson CB, Lahav A Mother's voice and heartbeat sounds elicit auditory plasticity in the human brain before full gestation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(10):3152–3157. doi:10.1073/pnas.1414924112.
53. Sargent B, Reimann H, Kubo M, Fetters L. Infant intralimb coordination and torque production: influence of prematurity. Infant Behav Dev. 2017;49:129–140. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.08.009.
54. van den Berg L, Gredebäck G. The sticky mittens paradigm: a critical appraisal of current results and explanations [published online ahead of print October 5, 2020]. Dev Sci.2021;24(5):e13036. doi:10.1111/desc.13036.
55. Guimarães EL, Cunha AB, De D, Soares A, Tudella E. Reaching behavior in preterm infants during the first year of life: a systematic review. 2013;17(4):340–354.
56. Kaul YF, Rosander K, Grönqvist H, Strand Brodd K, Hellström-Westas L, von Hofsten C. Reaching skills of infants born very preterm predict neurodevelopment at 2. 5 years. Infant Behav Dev. 2019;57:101333. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101333.
57. Colombo-Dougovito AM. The role of dynamic systems theory in motor development research: how does theory inform practice and what are the potential implications for autism spectrum disorder? Int J Disabil Hum Dev. 2017;16(2):141–155. doi:10.1515/ijdhd-2016-0015.
58. Harbourne RT, Berger SE. Embodied cognition in practice: exploring effects of a motor-based problem-solving intervention. Phys Ther. 2019;99(6):786–796. doi:10.1093/ptj/pzz031.
59. Morgan C, Novak I, Dale RC, Guzzetta A, Badawi N. GAME (Goals-Activity-Motor Enrichment): protocol of a single blind randomised controlled trial of motor training, parent education and environmental enrichment for infants at high risk of cerebral palsy. BMC Neurol. 2014;14(1):1–9.
60. Kolobe THA, Fagg AH. Robot reinforcement and error-based movement learning in infants with and without cerebral palsy. Phys Ther. 2019;99(6):677–688. doi:10.1093/ptj/pzz043.
61. Tripathi T, Dusing SC, Pidcoe PE, Xu Y, Shall MS, Riddle DL. A clinical trial based on reward contingency to improve prone tolerance and motor development is feasible in 3- to 6-month-old infants. J Mot Learn Dev. 2020;83:497–515. doi:10.1123/jmld.2019-0029.
62. Aslin RN. Infant eyes: a window on cognitive development. Infancy. 2012;17(1):126–140. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00097.x.