Neonatal Physical Therapy. Part II: Practice Frameworks and Evidence-Based Practice Guidelines : Pediatric Physical Therapy

Secondary Logo

Journal Logo

Special Communication

Neonatal Physical Therapy. Part II: Practice Frameworks and Evidence-Based Practice Guidelines

Sweeney, Jane K. PT, PhD, PCS, FAPTA; Heriza, Carolyn B. PT, EdD, FAPTA; Blanchard, Yvette PT, ScD; Dusing, Stacey C. PT, PhD

Author Information
Pediatric Physical Therapy 22(1):p 2-16, Spring 2010. | DOI: 10.1097/PEP.0b013e3181cdba43
  • Free

Purpose: 

(1) To outline frameworks for neonatal physical therapy based on 3 theoretical models, (2) to describe emerging literature supporting neonatal physical therapy practice, and (3) to identify evidence-based practice recommendations.

Key Points: 

Three models are presented as a framework for neonatal practice: (1) dynamic systems theory including synactive theory and the theory of neuronal group selection, (2) the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health, and (3) family-centered care. Literature is summarized to support neonatal physical therapists in the areas of examination, developmental care, intervention, and parent education. Practice recommendations are offered with levels of evidence identified.

Conclusions: 

Neonatal physical therapy practice has a theoretical and evidence-based structure, and evidence is emerging for selected clinical procedures. Continued research to expand the science of neonatal physical therapy is critical to elevate the evidence and support practice recommendations.

INTRODUCTION

Evidence-based practice guidelines are presented to support pediatric physical therapists (PTs) preparing for and practicing in the advanced subspecialty of neonatology. Theoretical frameworks, the emerging literature base, and evidence-based practice recommendations highlight this second article of a 2-part series on neonatal physical therapy practice. In part I, neonatal physical therapy clinical competencies, neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) clinical training models, and a clinical decision-making algorithm were described.1 The series was developed by a NICU Task Force of pediatric PTs with neonatal expertise appointed by the Section on Pediatrics, American Physical Therapy Association, and reviewed by an expert panel of neonatal practitioners representing diverse geographic regions of the United States.

PRACTICE FRAMEWORK

The NICU Practice Guidelines are based on 3 theoretical concepts: (1) dynamic systems theory (DST) of development; (2) the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF); and (3) family-centered care. These concepts offer a theoretical structure for understanding and organizing neonatal physical therapy and provide a framework for optimizing functional movement and posture of infants to promote functional activities and development of the infant-family system.

Dynamic Systems

In the dynamic systems model, all system components interact to produce meaningful, functional behavior.2 Multiple interacting systems and environments influence neonatal functional performance (Fig 1). In the NICU, dynamic system components include the following:

F1-2
Fig. 1.:
Dynamic systems and interacting environments influencing neonatal functional performance and interaction of the infant-family system.
  • The infant's biological makeup (ie, physiologic, behavioral, physical, social, and psychological elements);
  • The sociocultural (ie, professionals and family) and physical environments in which neonatal movements and postural control develop; and
  • The task or goal of the neonate, such as self- regulation of physiologic processes, behavioral state, posture and movement, and attention to and interaction with caregivers.3

Biological components of the infant do not act independently of each other or the physical and sociocultural environment in which a behavioral task is accomplished. For example, to conserve energy, the neonate needs to organize sleep-wake periods. If the nursery environment has continuous 24-hour bright lighting or if the neonates are awakened frequently during deep sleep, they may experience difficulty organizing biological systems to promote either sleep or alertness.

In a dynamic systems context, the need for function recruits and assembles the necessary and most available elements to complete the movement for a task performed within the immediate environment.3,4 For example, an infant may bring a hand to the mouth to suck on the fingers to console and decrease stress. If the infant is in the supine position and lacks strength to move against gravity, efforts to bring the hand to the mouth will not be successful. Repeated attempts to accomplish the movement by pressing the head and trunk against a firm surface (ie, the mattress) may contribute to disruption of physiologic systems leading to apnea or bradycardia. If attempts are continued, with time, this may result in an excessively extended posture.

As illustrated in Figure 1, any system component theoretically can facilitate or constrain an infant's functional movement and postural control. In the previous example, the supine position constrains the infant's ability to be successful in the hand-to-mouth task. The interaction between the environmental component and the infant's neuromuscular system prevents the functional activity of engaging the hand in the mouth. A small change in one component may produce a large change in movement and postural control. By changing the infant's position from supine position to a supported side-lying position, the infant may then explore and practice strategies for bringing the hand to the mouth without the effects of gravity.

The infant's attempts to initiate, practice, and learn a motor task (associative learning and memory) such as the hand-to-mouth maneuver, signal an optimal time for the neonatal PT, and other caregivers to facilitate this movement. According to DST, periods of learning are periods of transition. It is during these periods that the system(s) is most responsive to change, and motor learning can be optimized.4

Both the synactive theory of development5 and the theory of neuronal group selection (TNGS)6 are examples of dynamic systems models. These theories, described separately below, combine to provide a behavioral organization and a neuronal framework applicable to neonatal physical therapy practice.

Synactive Theory of Development.

Als5,7 described a behavioral organization process of subsystem interaction and interdependence (synaction) as the neonate responds to the challenges of the extrauterine environment. In this dynamic systems model, physiologic stability is considered as the foundation system for organizing movement, behavioral state, attention/interaction, and self-regulation.

Als8 highlighted the infant's behavior as a continuous expression of brain function available for observation by caregivers. Ongoing observations of infant behaviors at rest, during and after care procedures allow caregivers to interpret the infant's adaptation to the new extrauterine environment. These systematic observations are the base for the Newborn Individualized Care and Development Program (NIDCAP) developed by Als9,10 and for her program of NIDCAP outcomes research on NICU developmental care.11–14

As shown in Figure 1, the neonatal PT can apply this model by observing infant communication through the (1) autonomic system (respiratory and heart rates, oxygen saturation, color, hiccoughs, sneezes, tremors, and startles), (2) musculoskeletal and neurologic systems (joint alignment, body posture, tone, and movement), (3) state system (range, robustness, attention, transitions, and capacity to orient to animate and inanimate objects), and (4) self- regulation of state, motor, and autonomic systems.15,16 By supporting organization of the motor system, the neonatal therapist also supports organization of the autonomic, state, and self-regulation systems, freeing the infant to attend and interact with parents and with the environment.

It is imperative that neonatal PTs focus not only on the physiologic and sensorimotor components of infant function but also on the maturation and organization of behavior in neonates. Three primary concepts guiding the clinical application of infant behavioral observation and providing a matrix for understanding behavioral organization in a developmental context16,17 are described in Table 1.

T1-2
TABLE 1:
Guiding Concepts for Clinical Use of Infant Behavioral Observation to Support Development in Infants at High Risk16,17

Behavioral observation must guide examination, intervention, and parent teaching in neonatal physical therapy. Infant behavioral organization concepts are the cornerstone for understanding infant readiness to participate and maintain stability in all neonatal therapy contacts. Facilitating and supporting infant behavioral organization, reinforcing movement and postural components of infant self-regulation, and facilitating and supporting mutual affective regulation between parents and infants are priorities in neonatal therapy.16,17

Theory of Neuronal Group Selection.

Edelman's theory on how the nervous system becomes organized, stores information, and creates new behavioral patterns is identified as the TNGS.2,6,18–20 The theory is based on biological research and behavioral observations.21–23 A key concept of the theory is that the brain operates as a selective system. In addition, the brain is strongly affected by signals from the body and the environment either during fetal development or development after birth.18 As a result, no 2 brains are alike, and each person's brain is continually changing.

This theory has 3 main tenets2,19,20 for the development of the brain that are described in Table 2. Extensive evidence in adult animal models and humans is now available that the brain is a highly dynamic organ capable of structural and functional organization and reorganization in response to a variety of internal and external pressures. This neural plasticity is the mechanism by which the brain encodes experience and learns new behaviors.24–27

T2-2
TABLE 2:
Three Main Tenets of the Theory of Neuronal Group Selection2,6,18–23

Motor skill acquisition is associated with changes in gene expression, dendritic growth, synapse addition, and neuronal activity in the motor cortex and cerebellum (Fig 2).25–27 Practice of a newly learned behavior may be required to induce lasting neural changes. Some forms of plasticity therefore require not only the acquisition of a skill but also the continued performance of that skill over time.26 It is hypothesized that plasticity brought about through practice represents the instantiation of skill with neuronal circuitry making the acquired behavior resistant to decay in the absence of training.26,27

F2-2
Fig. 2.:
Neural events underlying motor map organization and reorganization and motor learning. Motor experience such as hand-to-mouth behavior induces a set of neural signaling pathways that activates gene expression within the motor cortex, which precedes synapse formation which in turn precedes motor map reorganization leading to learned sucking behavior.24–27 Adapted with permission from Kleim.25 The picture of infant depicting hand-to-mouth behavior is reprinted with permission from VandenBerg et al.28

Infants born preterm enter the world with a central nervous system that has had less time to mature within a protected uterine environment. The external NICU environment involves respiratory support, physiologic monitoring equipment, isolettes, absent postural containment provided by the uterus and amniotic fluid, aversive and painful stimuli, separation from parents, irregular patterns of handling from multiple caregivers, and unfiltered noise and light. Edelman6 hypothesized that when the brain is in unusual sensory circumstances, events of brain development are modified such as (1) preservation of cells that otherwise would be eliminated, (2) elimination of cells that otherwise would be preserved, (3) modification of dendritic and axonal pruning events, and (4) changes in connectivity (synapses). Neuronal changes, such as these, were reported by Bourjeois et al29 in monkeys delivered prematurely. The number of visual cortical cells was unchanged, but the synapses were significantly different in size, type, and laminar distribution, with the extent of these differences related to varying levels of prematurity. Neuronal changes were also reported by Als et al30 in infants born preterm (28–33 weeks) who received the NIDCAP program from 72 hours of NICU admission to age 2 weeks corrected for prematurity compared with a control group receiving standard care. Both groups were assessed at corrected ages of 2 weeks and 9 months on health status, growth, and neurobehavior; brain neurostructure (MRI) and neuroelectrophysiology (EEG) were compared at the corrected age of 2 weeks. Results indicated consistently better neurobehavioral function at 2 weeks and 9 months corrected age and more mature fiber structure in the cortex at 2 weeks corrected age for of infants receiving the NIDCAP program.

Neonatal PTs are responsible for nurturing brain growth. Brain development depends on a complex interplay between genes and environmental experiences. Early sensory information and motor experiences may have an effect on the architecture of the brain. Early interactions not only create a context but also directly affect the way the brain is “wired.”31 Because each infant has unique brain maturational levels at the time of birth, the same extrauterine environment and caregiving experiences may have different effects on brain structure. Neonatal therapy examination and intervention strategies should therefore be carefully modulated and paced to protect the architecture and maturation of the infant's brain.

International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health

The ICF is compatible with the dynamic systems model of development and learning. The ICF model represents interactive, complex relationships between an individual's health and the contextual factors of the environment and the person.32 This framework, adopted by the House of Delegates, American Physical Therapy Association, 200833 typifies PT practice and provides a structure for understanding and organizing practice. Within this framework, the neonatal therapist addresses the (1) functional and structural integrity of the body parts and systems, (2) promotion of age-appropriate postural and movement activities, and (3) appropriate interaction among the neonate, family, and professionals in the NICU. The neonatal therapist also considers impairments, activity limitations, and participation restrictions or changes in physical function or health resulting from injury, disease, or other causes.

Three components of function are (1) body and body parts, such as the ability of the neonate to control the physiologic function of breathing; (2) infant as a whole using the motor system to accomplish a task, such as bringing hands to the mouth or grasping a caregiver's finger; and (3) infant in the NICU, home, and community environments performing social functions such as interacting with caregivers during feeding in the NICU, home, or daycare center (Fig 3).32 These 3 components of function are essential to support the neonate's physiologic, behavioral, physical, social, and psychological well-being and to promote a meaningful life for the infants and their families.

F3-2
Fig. 3.:
International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF). Adapted with permission from the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health. 32

Three components of disease or active pathology, impairment, activity limitation, and participation restriction resulting from injury, disease, or other causes are also depicted in Figure 3. Impairment is a loss or abnormality of body structure or of a physiologic or psychological function. Examples in the neonate include the inability to control the physiologic systems resulting in apnea or restricted joint mobility contributing to decreased movement. Activity limitation is a restriction of the ability to perform a physical action, activity, or task in an efficient, typically expected, or competent manner. In the neonate, activity limitation may occur when the infant is unable to produce the antigravity, midline movement to bring hands to the mouth for sucking, a strategy often used for self-regulation. Participation restriction is the inability to participate in age-specific or gender-related roles in a particular social or physical environment. In the NICU, examples are feeding and attention/interaction activities such as auditory attention and visual interaction with caregivers during feeding.

The 2 contextual factors in Figure 3 are environmental (external influence on function) and personal (internal influence on function). Personal factors are the characteristics of the individual that are not part of a health condition or health state.32,34,35 These factors influence the relationships among body functions and structures, activities, and participation34 and can either constrain or promote function.

Examples of environmental factors in the NICU are light and noise. Excessively high noise and light levels in the NICU may impede the infant's physiologic, motor, or behavioral self-regulation; dimming the lights and decreasing the noise may promote physiologic, motor, or behavioral stability.

Examples of personal factors of infants born preterm are sensitivity or irritability to internal and external stressors (eg, pain, hunger, and handling) leading rapidly to overstimulation, agitation, or exhaustion, which may impede the infant's ability to self-calm or self-regulate the physiologic, motor, or state systems. Swaddling or the flexed, tucked side-lying position with the hands or fingers near the face and mouth may decrease physiologic distress, improve motor organization, and increase self-regulatory behavior.

The scope of the ICF framework and emphases on context and function can guide neonatal therapists in visualizing the complexity of the infant's internal and external environment and in anticipating how neonatal therapy procedures will support or overload infant and family functioning.

Family-Centered Care

Collaborative partnerships with families and neonatal practitioners are the cornerstone for caregiving success in neonatal physical therapy. Building parent and professional partnerships and adapting the care and teaching to family priorities, learning styles, emotional stresses, and cultural variables are essential considerations for making interventions effective.36 Centering neonatal physical therapy care on family needs requires understanding and empathy for the complex stresses and losses parents are experiencing as they cope with new roles as parents of medically fragile infants in the NICU environment. Although the NICU experience for parents may vary depending on the nature of the delivery, level of prematurity, and severity of illness, their consistent focus is on the baby's survival and developmental outcome. In addition, they are mourning the loss of the “imagined” or “wished for” baby as they struggle to develop a bond with their “real” baby.37,38 Emotional trauma relating to a preterm birth, unexpected financial stress on the family, and ongoing psychological stresses during parenting attempts in the NICU contribute to the posttraumatic stress disorder reported in parents with infants requiring intensive care.39–41

Neonatal PTs can play an important role in alleviating parent stress and anxiety and in helping parents interact with their infant who may be medically fragile by learning to respond to the baby's behavioral cues for signaling readiness to feed or engage in social interaction and to disengage for a break. Helping parents understand and respond to infant cues is considered critical to helping them maintain their roles as parents and mitigate levels of stress and depression.16,42–46 Developmentally appropriate interventions that focus on helping parents read infant cues, enhance parent-child interactions, and support the parent-child relationship are reported to improve developmental outcome and enhance the child's cognitive and socioemotional development.47–51 Neonatal PTs have a responsibility to individualize their approach and intervention strategies to meet the unique needs of the infant and family with respect to ensuring that family members acquire confidence and skill in movement and postural management with their infant related to holding, carrying, feeding, and dressing. The family is therefore a primary focus for collaboration and support by the neonatal PT and for early inclusion in NICU developmental intervention and discharge plans.

PHYSICAL THERAPY IN THE NICU: EVIDENCE FOR PRACTICE

A key competency in providing care in any physical therapy setting is critical appraisal of relevant research and the application of evidence to practice. In this section, an overview of recent evidence is provided for the developmental and therapeutic interventions in the NICU setting where neonatal therapists may participate in a variety of service delivery approaches involving program development, consultation, and direct therapy services. Program development services may focus on recommendations for advancing developmental care for all infants in the NICU with policy and training implications. Consultative services may include making infant-specific recommendations, which are updated at regular intervals, but the intervention is provided by the infant's primary caregivers. Direct neonatal physical therapy services are provided to address an infant's specific musculoskeletal, neuromuscular, or neurobehavioral needs. Family education is an important component of both consultative and direct therapy services and includes discharge teaching for coordination of follow-up through interdisciplinary clinics and local early intervention programs. This literature overview and evidence-based recommendations are intended for all service delivery models used by the PTs in NICU settings.

Examination

A range of tests and measures are available for examination of neonates. Many instruments, potentially useful in neonatal physical therapy practice for selected infants, are identified in the examination and evaluation competency in Part I of the NICU Practice Guidelines.1 Critical components in the examination competency for neonatal PTs are (1) determining infant readiness to begin neurologic and neuromotor examination and (2) monitoring and modifying changes in physiologic and behavioral stability during and after examination procedures. Significantly greater physiologic cost (increased heart rate and mean arterial pressure) and behavioral stress (increased finger splay, arm salute, hiccoughs, yawns, and mottled skin color) were demonstrated by neonates at 30 to 35 weeks post-conception compared with infants born at term during neurologic examination procedures administered by a neonatal PT. Greater physiologic and behavioral stress was demonstrated by both groups of neonates during the neuromotor items compared with neurobehavioral items.52,53

Risk management considerations for presumed benign neurologic examination procedures are raised from this preliminary research. For infants born at less than 35 weeks of gestation, the reliability, diagnostic benefit, and physiologic cost of neurologic examination, especially neuromotor items, must be scrutinized, and infants with borderline stability should be excluded from evaluative handling. At any gestational age or acuity level, nonhandling observation of an infant's movement, posture, behavioral organization, and physiologic stability (particularly during routine nursing care) are advised for determining baseline function and for collaboration with neonatal nurses and neonatologists on optimal timing for evaluative handling.

Intervention

Developmental Care.

This caregiving approach is designed to promote the neurobehavioral and physiologic organization of an infant's autonomic, motor, state/attention, and self-regulation systems as the infant matures.51 A variety of general environmental, behavioral, and care strategies can be used to modulate neonatal physiologic and behavioral changes during each episode of care and accommodate the varying maturation and acuity levels of infants.

Individualized developmental care is developmental care designed and updated for each infant in addition to a generalized developmental care plan for all infants. For example, in a NICU where general developmental care principles are applied, overhead lights may be dimmed, isolettes covered, and caregiving procedures clustered. In a nursery providing individualized developmental care using a NIDCAP approach, each infant's needs are evaluated and a specific developmental plan developed and evaluated on an ongoing basis.

In a Cochrane review of developmental care54 including 36 randomized clinical trials (RCTs) of both general and individualized developmental care, some benefits and no harmful effects were reported from neonatal developmental care for infants born prematurely. The benefits of individualized developmental care documented in small RCTs included reduced incidence of necrotizing enterocolitis and chronic lung disease following NIDCAP interventions,54 improved behavior in preschool-age children who received NIDCAP-based care in the NICU,55 and faster transition to all nipple feedings following a daily combination of vestibular, tactile, auditory, and visual stimulation.56 The results of these small RCTs are encouraging and suggest a positive, short-term effect of developmental care on the behavior of infants born preterm.54 Larger scale RCTs are needed to confirm these preliminary, positive findings.

Two large RCTs conducted in The Netherlands with infants born at less than 32 weeks of gestation have called into question the benefit of basic developmental care (isolette covers to filter light and sound and flexed, midline, nested body positioning procedures).57,58 In the first study, no group differences in respiratory support, neurologic status, or growth were documented at term equivalent age between infants receiving standard newborn care compared with infants receiving basic developmental care in the NICU.57 In the second study, basic developmental care was compared with the NIDCAP approach (weekly, individualized behavioral observation, and developmental care plan updates) to determine short- and long-term effects. No group differences between individualized (NIDCAP) and basic developmental care were found in respiratory support, intensive care duration, growth and development at term equivalent age, and growth, cognitive, or motor development at 1 or 2 years of age. Although these studies question the effect of individualized versus basic developmental care on neurodevelopmental and growth outcomes, neither of the RCTs included measures of behavior, self-regulation, feeding, or family coping which may have differed between groups.57,58

In contrast, Peters et al13 recently reported reduced length of intensive care stay and incidence of chronic lung disease in a well-designed and controlled intervention study of NIDCAP effects. Preliminary data from individualized developmental care (NIDCAP) procedures indicated reduction in the rate and severity of mental disability.11,13 In combination, these studies suggest that individualized developmental care may decrease potential medical complications and varied short-term outcomes such as length of stay, level of alertness, and feeding progression. Further research prioritizing long-term motor and cognitive outcomes will offer expanded analyses on the efficacy of basic and individualized developmental care. Larger longitudinal RCTs are needed to determine how individualized developmental care affects behavior, emotional stability, and parent-child interactions (Table 3).

T3-2
TABLE 3:
Developmental Outcomes with Developmental Care

Direct Intervention.

Direct therapy service refers to service provided by a neonatal therapist at regular intervals to address a specific impairment or activity limitation. Direct therapy includes, but is not limited to, handling to promote movement or postural control, joint alignment and range of motion, cranial shaping, feeding performance, environmental modulation, and behavioral stability during caregiving. Research on direct therapy services is limited to preliminary results with a strong need for replication using larger samples and randomization.

Body positioning.

Infants born preterm have immature musculoskeletal systems that are influenced by positioning.59 During fetal development, the uterine walls provide containment, facilitate trunk and extremity flexion, and provide reactionary forces in response to fetal movements.55 Infants born preterm must cope with gravity, a lack of fluid to support movement, and no reactionary forces to support bone and joint formation.60 Exposure to prolonged atypical positioning in the NICU has been associated with torticollis, positional plagiocephaly, reduced movement quality, and lower extremity malalignment.59,61–63 Developmentally supportive positioning may enhance the development of normal skeletal alignment and provide opportunities for normal movement patterns. Therapists can play a vital role in program development and consultation relating to positioning. As experts in the musculoskeletal system, PTs with neonatal training have the expertise to establish positioning guidelines for NICUs, aid in staff development, and consult on individualized positioning for infants. Because neonatal PTs do not position infants each time they need to be moved, a primary role of the therapist is to collaborate with and support caregivers in positioning infants to prevent secondary complications.

Infants born preterm are frequently cared for in multiple positions for respiratory support and skin care. Although these varied positions are counter to the American Academy of Pediatrics recommendations61 for a preferred supine sleeping position to reduce the risk of sudden infant death syndrome, the infants remain continuously monitored in the NICU setting. In an updated statement, the American Academy of Pediatrics advised that all infants transition to the supine sleeping position by hospital discharge and encouraged detailed discharge instruction on sudden infant death syndrome prevention including supine sleeping without soft bedding.64 The evidence in these practice guidelines relating to prone sleeping position and use of nests or blanket rolls may not be appropriate for infants approaching discharge.62,65–68

Although physiologic benefits contribute to the frequent use of the prone position in the NICU, neonates may develop “flat” trunks, hyperextended and excessively rotated cervical spines, and abducted hips when lying in the prone position, which may contribute to prolonged atypical postures in infancy.69 Clinicians frequently modify the prone position to improve posture while retaining physiologic benefits. The use of a horizontal positioning roll under the pelvis to elevate the hips and support hip flexion may lead to plagiocephaly, and to increased cervical and thoracic extension.65 In contrast, the use of a vertical positioning roll on the torso parallel to the spine (alone or in combination with a horizontal roll under the pelvis) has been associated with improved scapular position and hip flexion.67

In the supine position, infants born preterm may have increased hip and shoulder abduction and external rotation. Two small studies advocated use of body positioning “nests” to encourage flexion of the extremities and midline head position.62,66 In a pilot study of 10 infants, smooth, midline movements of the extremities were observed when infants were in a supine nest with extremity flexion support compared with the supine position without the nest.62 Although this study provided preliminary evidence that flexed, supine positioning provided opportunities for practicing flexed, midline motor patterns commonly seen in infants born at term gestation, no follow-up observations of the neonates were reported regarding effects on movement after the nest was discontinued.62 Varied positioning including supine, prone, and side-lying positions has been found to reduce the negative consequences that may arise from infants remaining in a single position.65,68

Swaddling with hands to mouth and lower extremity flexion while the infant is positioned in prone, supine, or side lying has been reported to improve neuromuscular development at 34 weeks post-conception.70

Although these studies provide preliminary evidence for the use of positioning strategies to encourage extremity flexion, no recent studies have addressed the effects of positioning devices on movement quality, muscle imbalances, postural control, or long-term musculoskeletal changes.

Joint range of motion.

Passive range of motion in the extremities has been advocated as an intervention to increase bone mineral density; however, a recent Cochrane review of 6 RCTs found only a small, transient increase in weight gain and bone mineral density immediately after a protocol of passive range of motion to multiple joints in all extremities 5 times per week for 3 to 4 weeks with no difference reported at 12 months of age.71 The Cochrane review panel concluded that this evidence was insufficient for implementing range of motion in infant born preterm to improve bone density and weight gain.71

Therapeutic neuromotor handling.

The use of therapeutic handling or therapist-provided, hands-on intervention to advance motor development of infants born preterm while in the NICU was investigated in 2 small studies with similar approaches to intervention.72,73 Girolami et al73 included infants born preterm with atypical or asymmetrical movement responses on the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS). Cameron et al72 selected infants at high risk of disabilities based on birth weight (<1500 g) and gestational age (<32 weeks) but with no evidence of a movement abnormality. In both studies, intervention was provided 5 days per week focused on midline hand skills, trunk and extremity flexion, and postural (neck/trunk) control. Girolami et al73 reported improved Supplemental Motor Test scores including enhanced antigravity and midline movements before NICU discharge. Cameron et al72 found no group differences on the Alberta Infant Motor Scales at 4 months of corrected age; however, no postintervention comparison was conducted before NICU discharge. The findings from these 2 studies suggest that infants with a high risk birth history, but no atypical or disorganized movement, are not likely to need direct physical therapy services, but infants at 35 weeks post-conception with asymmetrical or atypical development may benefit from therapeutic neuromotor handling.73 Additional RCTs with rigorous subject selection, discriminative and evaluative measures to determine eligibility and outcomes, and detailed intervention methods are needed to expand the evidence regarding therapeutic handling in the NICU for infants with atypical movement.

Multimodal sensory stimulation.

Because infants born preterm are exposed to excessive overhead light, loud noises, and noxious procedures in intensive care environments, techniques to limit the consequences of negative stimuli are often included in developmental care plans,51 and provision of positive sensory experiences (tactile, vestibular, auditory, and visual) has been encouraged in some settings. A 2006 Cochrane review, which summarized evidence for sensory intervention, indicated that a program including a combination of sensory stimuli may enhance state regulation, speed transition to full nipple feedings, and shorten length of hospital stay, whereas tactile stimulation alone may improve short-term growth and reduce length of stay.54 All studies included in the review had small samples, and many had methodologic limitations including wide variations in sensory conditions. Two recent, small RCTs extended the Cochrane review findings. A study comparing massage (tactile), massage plus kinesthetic stimulation (passive joint motion), and no intervention found no group differences in weight gain or length of stay for infants born weighing less than 1500 g.74 In an RCT including massage as one component of a multimodal stimulation intervention for infants between 31 and 34 weeks post- conception, infants exhibited reduced heart rate, increased visual-auditory orientation, and increased sensorimotor skills after the intervention. In addition, after the multimodal interventions, infants had increased body length and decreased length of stay compared with infants without intervention.75

In combination, the 2006 Cochrane review54 and these recent RCTs74,75 suggest that a multimodal approach to intervention may improve sensorimotor development, increase weight gain, and reduce length of stay in selected infants born preterm. Neonatal PTs must scrutinize these data and continue to explore appropriate thresholds and types of sensory stimuli for neonates. White-Traut et al76 cautioned that multimodal sensory stimulation may be contraindicated for infants with periventricular leukomalacia. Additional research is needed to determine the physiologic and behavioral readiness of infants born preterm for multimodal intervention in the NICU and developmental outcomes after NICU discharge (Table 4).

T4-2
TABLE 4:
Direct Interventions in the NICU
Support of infant feeding.

Feeding is a functional activity of the highest priority for infants and caregivers and as such is an integral part of neonatal PT practice. Minimal evidence is currently available to support oral-facial stimulation to hasten feeding progression in neonates. In a small RCT reported by Fucile et al,77 a decrease in transition time (days) to all oral (nipple) feedings was documented in infants receiving an oral stimulation program compared with controls. No differences occurred, however, in hospital stay or in postconceptual age when all oral feedings were achieved.77 Researchers affirmed in 2 recent RCTs that oral-facial stimulation did not decrease the postconceptual age at which infants attained a daily schedule of all oral feedings.78,79 In contrast, a 2005 Cochrane review panel80 analyzed evidence from 21 studies, including 15 RCTs, and found nonnutritive sucking with a pacifier before, during, or after gavage or bottle feedings to significantly decrease length of hospital stay and to improve feeding behavior (decreased defensive or fussy behavior during and after feeding). Long-term benefits of nonnutritive sucking have not been established.80

Evidence is emerging for pacing and flow rate interventions to support feeding in infants born preterm. Paced feeding, allowing 3 to 5 sucks on the bottle followed by 3- to 5-second breathing pauses, seems beneficial. In a small, nonrandomized trial Law-Morstatt et al81 found fewer bradycardia episodes and improved sucking skill maturity at discharge in infants (n = 18) receiving pacing techniques during feeding compared with controls receiving caregiver education intervention after discharge.81

Slowing the fluid flow rate has been shown to improve efficiency and physiologic stability during feeding. In a crossover design, 20 infants were randomly given a fast-flowing (cross-cut) nipple or a standard-flowing, single-hole nipple.82 When fed with the faster flowing nipple, infants were less efficient, took longer to feed, consumed less volume, and had higher respiratory rates than when fed with a single-hole nipple. Future research is needed to expand the level of evidence on pacing effects and flow rate adaptations and to explore the effects of feeding positions.

Infant behavioral readiness is beginning to guide feeding decisions rather than volume requirements only.83–85 Kirk et al83 reported decreased postconceptual age (6 days) when all oral (nipple) feedings were reached by 51 infants in a historical control design with feedings initiated and advanced according to behavioral cues. McCain84 demonstrated a similar decrease (5 days) to all oral feedings when infant arousal cues and nonnutritive sucking behaviors were used as indicators of readiness to feed in an RCT of 81 infants in 2 NICUs.

Neonatal PTs can support behavioral readiness and feeding progression by teaching parents to interpret and value infant behavioral cues and by supporting infant physiologic and motor organization. Consistent with DST, infants who are organized and practice feeding while maintaining behavioral and physiologic stability are more likely to be successful feeders.

Adjunct interventions.

Neonatal massage as a single modality has been advocated to reduce stress levels and improve infant-parent attachment in the NICU environment86; however, a 2004 Cochrane review of massage in neonates stressed the need for additional research before integration of massage into routine care.87 Supporting an infant's body with hand swaddling (facilitated body tuck) without massage88 and skin-to-skin holding by parents89 provide human touch and are less likely to contribute to potential overstimulation in neonates who are vulnerable.90 Similarly, the Cochrane review on the efficacy of chest physical therapy with neonates indicated that additional research was required before inclusion into routine neonatal care related to reported adverse effects of bruising, rib fractures, and intracranial lesions.91

Neonatal hydrotherapy for neonates who are medically stable with movement impairment was introduced in a multiple case series92 in which adequate physiologic tolerance (7% increase in mean arterial pressure and 7% increase in heart rate) was documented and a hydrotherapy protocol was detailed. In a later study, effects of hydrotherapy (immersion in swaddled and semiflexed position) before feeding indicated improved feeding efficiency and short-term mean daily weight gain in 31 infants born preterm (32–36 weeks post-conception) with no difference found between high- and low-risk groups.93 Further investigation of neonatal hydrotherapy effects on breast-feeding proficiency, bone density, gastrointestinal reflux patterns, and behavioral organization may expand future clinical use of this aquatic intervention in the NICU setting for stable neonates.

Parent Education in the NICU

Parent and caregiver teaching is a primary role of therapists in the NICU. Research on educating parents to interpret the meaning of their infant's behavioral cues and developmental status has been shown to reduce parental stress94 and improve parental mental health.95 Derived primarily from the NBAS or NIDCAP models, the following instruments have been developed to attune parents to their infant's capabilities: (1) Mother's Assessment of the Behavior of the Infant,96 (2) Family Administered Neonatal Activities,97 (3) Creating Opportunities for Parent Empowerment,95 and (4) Newborn Behavioral Observation System.16 A meta-analysis by Das Eiden et al98 indicated that NBAS-based interventions during the neonatal period have a small to moderate beneficial effect on the quality of later parenting.

Emerging research on parents' preferred instructional method in the NICU indicates support for multimodal (discussion, demonstration, video, and written) instruction.99–102 Effects of NICU parent education programs specifically designed to promote infant motor organization and development have not been reported but parent-delivered motor programs after NICU discharge are supported.103,104 Additional research with both parent and infant outcome measures is needed to further investigate the efficacy of parent education and parent-delivered intervention in the NICU.

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE RECOMMENDATIONS

Implementation of clinical procedures with research evidence and participation by practitioners in clinical studies in the NICU are critical to the advancement of neonatal physical therapy. As the state of our science expands, the evidence base to support neonatal physical therapy practice will continue to emerge. Practice recommendations, rated according to a 5-level hierarchy of evidence (Table 5), are outlined in Table 6 to guide neonatal PTs in designing plans of care. The evidence levels range between I and V with the majority of practice recommendations graded at B level evidence (consistent levels II to III).105

T5-2
TABLE 5:
Hierarchy of Evidence105
T6-2
TABLE 6:
Evidence-Based Recommendations for Neonatal Physical Therapy

CONCLUSIONS

As specialists in movement and postural control, neonatal PTs have a unique window of opportunity to shape the musculoskeletal system and motor organization of infants requiring intensive care and to support parents and caregivers in optimizing infant brain development during the NICU stay. Guiding this advanced practice are clinical training models, clinical competencies, and a decision-making algorithm described in Part I1 and a theoretical framework, literature base, and evidence-based practice recommendations detailed in Part II. The science of this pediatric subspecialty is evolving with many areas of new and continued research needed to advance the rigor of evidence and to expand these neonatal physical therapy clinical guidelines.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors express appreciation to medical illustrator Thomas Pierce, BA, for graphic expertise; Marie Reilly, PT, PhD, and Beth McManus, PT, MPH, ScD, for content review; and Erin Sundseth Ross, PhD, CCC-SLP, for consultation and expertise on neonatal feeding.

REFERENCES

1. Sweeney JK, Heriza CB, Blanchard Y. Neonatal physical therapy. Part 1: Clinical competencies and NICU clinical training models. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2009;21:296–307.
2. Thelen E, Smith LB. A Dynamic Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press/Bradford Books Series in Cognitive Psychology; 1994.
3. Heriza CB, Sweeney J. Pediatric physical therapy. Part 1: Practice scope, scientific foundations, and theory. Infants Young Child. 1994;7:20–32.
4. Heriza CB. Motor development: traditional and contemporary theories. In: Lister M, ed. Contemporary Management of Motor Control Problems. Proceedings of the II Step Conference. Alexandria, VA: The Foundation for Physical Therapy; 1991:99–126.
5. Als H. Toward a synactive theory of development: promise for the assessment and support of infant individuality. Infant Mental Health J. 1982;3:229–243.
6. Edelman GM. Neural Darwinism. The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection. New York, NY: Basic Books, Inc; 1987.
7. Als H. A synactive model of neonatal behavioral organization: framework for the assessment of neurobehavioral development in the premature infant and for support of infants and parents in the neonatal intensive care environment. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr. 1986;6:3–53.
8. Als H. Neurobehavioral development of the preterm infant. In: Martin RJ, Fanaroff AA, Walsh M, eds. Fanaroff and Martin's Neonatal-Perinatal Medicine: Diseases of the Fetus and Infant. 8th ed. St Louis, MO: Mosby; 2006;1051–1068.
9. Als H, Lawhon G, Brown E, et al. Individualized behavioral and environmental care for the very low birth weight preterm infant at high risk for bronchopulmonary dysplasia: neonatal intensive care unit and developmental outcome. Pediatrics. 1986;78:1123–1132.
10. Als H. Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program (NIDCAP): new frontier for neonatal and perinatal medicine. J Neonatal Perinatal Med. 2009:138–147.
11. McAnulty GB, Duffy FH, Butler SC, et al. Effects of the Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program (NIDCAP) at age 8 years: prelimary data. Clin Pediatr (Philadelphia). 2009; [Epub ahead of print].
12. McAnulty G, Duffy F, Butler S, et al. Individualized developmental care for a large sample of very preterm infants: health, neurobehaviour and neurophysiology. Acta Paediatr. 2009;12:1920–1926.
13. Peters KL, Rosychuk RJ, Hendson L, et al. Improvement of short- and long-term outcomes for very low birth weight infants: the Edmonton NIDCAP trial. Pediatrics. 2009;124:1009–1020.
14. Maguire CM, Walther FJ, Sprij AJ, et al. Effects of individualized developmental care in a randomized, controlled trial on preterm infants <32 weeks. Pediatrics. 2009;124:1021–1030.
15. Als H. Reading the premature infant. In Goldson E, ed. Nurturing the Premature Infant: Developmental Interventions in the Neonatal Intensive Care Nursery. New York: Oxford University Press; 1999:18–85.
16. Nugent JK, Keefer CH, Minear S, et al. Understanding Newborn Behavior and Early Relationships. The Newborn Behavioral Observations (NBO) System Handbook. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 2007.
17. Blanchard Y, Mouradian LM. Integrating neurobehavioral concepts into early intervention eligibility evaluation. Infants Young Child. 2000;13:41–50.
18. Edelman GM. Second Nature: Brain Science and Human Knowledge. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 2006:23–34.
19. Edelman GM, Tononi G. A Universe of Consciousness. How Matter Becomes Imagination. New York, NY: Basic Books; 2000:79–92.
20. Sporns O, Edelman GM. Solving Bernstein's problem: a proposal for the development of coordinated movement by selection. Child Dev. 1993;64:960–981.
21. Campbell SK. The child's development of functional movement. In: Campbell SK, Vander Linden DW, Palisano RJ, eds. Physical Therapy for Children. 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders; 2006:33–76.
22. Thelen E. Coupling perception and action in the development of skill: a dynamic approach. In: Bloch H, Bertenthal BI, eds. Sensory-Motor Organization and Development in Infancy and Early Childhood. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic; 1990:39–56.
23. Campbell SK. Revolution in progress: a conceptual framework of examination and intervention. Part II. Neurol Rep. 2000;24:42–46.
24. Adkins DL, Boychuk J, Remple MS, et al. Motor training induces experience-specific patterns of plasticity across motor cortex and spinal cord. J Appl Physiol. 2006;101:1776–1782.
25. Kleim JA. Experience dependent brain plasticity: Translating basic science into clinical practice. Paper presented at the: III Step Conference, Linking Movement Science and Intervention, Pediatric and Neurology Sections, American Physical Therapy Association. July 17, 2005, Salt Lake City, UT.
26. Kleim JA, Jones TA. Principles of experience-dependent neural plasticity: implications for rehabilitation after brain damage. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51:S225–S239.
27. Monfils MH, Plautz EJ, Kleim JA. In search of the motor engram: motor map plasticity as a mechanism for encoding motor experience. Neuroscientist. 2005;11:471–483.
28. VandenBerg K, Browne JV, Perez, et al. Getting to Know Your Baby. Oakland, CA. Special Start Training Program. Mills College, Department of Education; 2003.
    29. Bourjeois JP, Jastreboff PJ, Rakic P. Synaptogenesis in visual cortex of normal and preterm monkeys: evidence for intrinsic regulation of synaptic overproduction. Proc Nat Acad Sci U S A. 1989;86:4297–4301.
    30. Als H, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, et al. Early experience alters brain function and structure. Pediatrics. 2004;113:846–857.
    31. Shore R. Rethinking the Brain: New Insights into Early Development. New York, NY: Families and Work Institute; 1997.
    32. International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2001.
    33. APTA endorses ICF Model. PT Bulletin Online. 2008;9:4.
    34. Palisano RJ. A collaborative model of service delivery for children with movement disorders: a framework for evidence-based decision making. Phys Ther. 2006;86:1295–1305.
    35. Jette AM. Toward a common language for function, disability, and health. Phys Ther. 2006;86:726,734.
    36. Dunn MS, Reilly MC, Johnston AM, et al. Development and dissemination of potentially better practices for the provision of family-centered care in neonatology: the Family-Centered Care Map. Pediatrics. 2006;118:S95–S107.
    37. Bruschweiler-Stern N. Mere a terme et mere premature. In: Dugnat M, ed. Le monde relationnel du bebe. Ramonville Saint-Agne: ERES; 1997.
    38. Klaus MH, Kennell JH, Klaus PH. Bonding. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley; 1995.
    39. DeMier RL, Hynan MT, Harris HB, et al. Perinatal stressors as predictors of symptoms of posttraumatic stress in mothers of infants at high-risk. J Perinatol. 1996;16:276–280.
    40. Callahan JL, Borja SE, Hynan MT. Modification of the Perinatal PTSD Questionnaire to enhance clinical utility. J Perinatol. 2006;26:533–539.
    41. Shaw RJ, Bernard RS, Deblois T, et al. (2009). The relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder in the neonatal intensive care unit. Psychosomatics. 2009;50:131–137.
    42. Browne JV, Talmi A. Family-based intervention to enhance infant-parent relationships in the neonatal intensive care unit. J Pediatr Psychol. 2005;30:667–677.
    43. Lawhon G. Facilitation of parenting the premature infant within the newborn intensive care unit. J Perinat Neonatal Nurs. 2002;16:71–83.
    44. Loo KK, Espinosa M, Tyler R, et al. Using knowledge to cope with stress in the NICU: how parents integrate learning to read the physiologic and behavioral cues of the infant. Neonatal Netw. 2003;22:31–37.
    45. Parker S, Zahr LK, Cole JCD, et al. Outcomes after developmental intervention in the neonatal intensive care unit for mothers of preterm infants with low socioeconomic status. J Pediatr. 1992;120:780–785.
    46. Redshaw ME. Mothers of babies requiring special care: attitudes and experiences. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 1997;15:109–121.
    47. Als H, Lawhon G, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Gibes-Grossman R, Blickman JG. Individualized developmental care for the very low-birth-weight preterm infants. Medical and neurofunctional effects. JAMA. 1994;272:853–105.
    48. Kleberg A, Westrup B, Stjernqvist K. Developmental outcome, child behaviour and mother-child interaction at 3 years of age following Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Intervention Program (NIDCAP) intervention. Early Hum Dev. 2000;60:123–135.
    49. Nugent JK, Brazelton TB. (2000). Preventive infant mental health: uses of the Brazelton Scale. In: Osofsky J, Fitzgerald HE, eds. WAIMH Handbook of Infant Mental Health. Vol. II. New York, NY: Wiley; 2000:159–202.
    50. Sameroff AJ. Models of development and developmental risk. In: Zeanah CH, ed. Handbook of Infant Mental Health, New York, NY: Guilford; 1997:3–13.
    51. Vandenberg KA. Individualized developmental care for high risk newborns in the NICU: a practice guideline. Early Hum Dev. 2007;83:433–442.
    52. Sweeney JK. Physiological adaptation of neonates to neurological assessment. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr. 1986;6:155–167.
    53. Sweeney JK. Physiological and behavioral effects of neurological assessment in preterm and full-term neonates [abstract]. Pediatr Phys Ther. 1989;9:144.
    54. Symington A, Pinelli J. Developmental care for promoting development and preventing morbidity in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2006;2:CD001814.
    55. Westrup B, BohmB, Lagercrantz H, et al. Preschool outcome in children born very prematurely and cared for according to the Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program (NIDCAP). Acta Paediatr. 2004;93:498–507.
    56. White-Traut RC, Nelson MN, Silvestri JM, et al. Effects of auditory, tactile, visual, and vestibular intervention on length of stay, alertness, and feeding progression in preterm infants. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2002;44:91–97.
    57. Maguire CM, Veen S, Sprij A, et al. Leiden Developmental Care Project. Effects of basic developmental care on neonatal morbidity, neuromotor development, and growth at term age of infants who were born at <32 weeks. Pediatrics. 2008;121:239–245.
    58. Maguire CM, Walther FJ, Zwieten PH, et al. Follow-up outcomes at 1 and 2 years of infants born less than 32 weeks after Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program. Pediatrics. 2009;123:1081–1087.
    59. Sweeney JK, Gutierrez T. Musculoskeletal implications of preterm infant positioning in the NICU. J Perinat Neonatal Nurs. 2002;16:58–70.
    60. de Vries JI, Fong BF. Normal fetal motility: an overview. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2006;27:701–11.
    61. American Academy of Pediatrics Task Force on Sudden Infant Death Syndrome. The changing concept of sudden infant death syndrome: diagnostic coding shifts, controversies regarding the sleeping environment, and new variables to consider in reducing risk. Pediatrics. 2005;116:1245–1255.
    62. Ferrari F, Bertoncelli N, Gallo C, et al. Posture and movement in healthy preterm infants in supine position in and outside the nest. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2007;92:F386–F390.
    63. Van Vlimmeren LA, van der Graff Y, Boere-Boonekamp MM, et al. Risk factors for deformational plagiocephaly at birth and at 7 weeks of age: a prospective cohort study. Pediatrics. 2008;119:408–418.
    64. Joint Committee on Infant Hearing, American Academy of Audiology, American Academy of Pediatrics, American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, and Directors of Speech and Hearing Programs in State Health and Welfare Agencies. Year 2000 position statement: principles and guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs. Pediatrics. 2000;106:798–817.
    65. Vaivre-Douret L, Ennouri K, Jrad I, et al. Effect of positioning on the incidence of abnormalities of muscle tone in low-risk, preterm infants. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2004;8:21–34.
    66. Vaivre-Douret L, Golse B. Comparative effects of 2 positional supports on neurobehavioral and postural development in preterm neonates. J Perinat Neonatal Nurs. 2007;21:323–330.
    67. Monterosso L, Kristjanson LJ, Cole J, et al. Effect of postural supports on neuromotor function in very preterm infants to term equivalent age. J Paediatr Child Health. 2003;39:197–205.
    68. McManus BM, Capistran PS. A case presentation of early intervention with dolichocephaly in the NICU: collaboration between the primary nursing team and the developmental care specialist. Neonatal Netw. 2008;27:307–15.
    69. Monterosso L, Kristjanson L, Cole J. Neuromotor development and the physiologic effects of positioning in very low birth weight infants. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 2002;31:138–46.
    70. Short MA, Brooks-Brunn JA, Reeves DS, et al. The effects of swaddling versus standard positioning on neuromuscular development in very low birth weight infants. Neonatal Netw. 1996;15:25–31.
    71. Schulzke SM, Trachsel D, Patole SK. Physical activity programs for promoting bone mineralization and growth in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2007;2:CD005387.
    72. Cameron EC, Maehle V, Reid J. The effects of an early physical therapy intervention for very preterm, very low birth weight infants: a randomized controlled clinical trial. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2005;17:107–119.
    73. Girolami GL, Campbell SK. Efficacy of a neuro-developmental treatment program to improve motor control in infants born prematurely. Pediatr Phys Ther. 1994;6:175–184.
    74. Massaro AN, Hammad TA, Jazzo B, et al. Massage with kinesthetic stimulation improves weight gain in preterm infants. J Perinatol. 2009;29:352–357.
    75. Vaivre-Douret L, Oriot D, Blossier P, et al. The effect of multimodal stimulation and cutaneous application of vegetable oils on neonatal development in preterm infants: a randomized controlled trial. Child Care Health Dev. 2009;35:96–105.
    76. White-Traut RC, Nelson MN, Silvestri JM, et al. Developmental patterns of physiological response to a multisensory intervention in extremely premature and high-risk infants. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 2004;33:266–275.
    77. Fucile S, Gisel E, Lau C. Oral stimulation accelerates the transition from tube to oral feeding in preterm infants. J Pediatr. 2002;141:230–236.
    78. Boiron M, Da Nobrega L, Roux S, et al. Effects of oral stimulation and oral support on non-nutritive sucking and feeding performance in preterm infants. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2007;49:439–444.
    79. Bragelien R, Rokke W, Markestad T. Stimulation of sucking and swallowing to promote oral feeding in premature infants. Acta Paediatr. 2007;96:1430–1432.
    80. Pinelli J, Symington A. Non-nutritive sucking for promoting physiologic stability and nutrition in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2005;4:CD001071.
    81. Law-Morstatt L, Judd DM, Snyder P, et al. Pacing as a treatment technique for transitional sucking patterns. J Perinatol. 2003;23:483–488.
    82. Chang YJ, Lin CP, Lin YJ, et al. Effects of single-hole and cross-cut nipple units on feeding efficiency and physiological parameters in premature infants. J Nurs Res. 2007;15:215–223.
    83. Kirk AT, Alder SC, King JD. Cue-based oral feeding clinical pathway results in earlier attainment of full oral feeding in premature infants. J Perinatol. 2007;27:572–578.
    84. McCain GC. An evidence-based guideline for introducing oral feeding to healthy preterm infants. Neonatal Netw. 2003;22:45–50.
    85. McGrath JM, Medoff-Cooper B. Alertness and feeding competence in extremely early born preterm infants. Newborn Infant Nurs Rev. 2002;2:174–186.
    86. Beachy M. Premature infant massage in the NICU. Neonatal Netw. 2003;3:39–45.
    87. Vickers A, Ohlsson A, Lacy JB, et al. Massage for promoting growth and development of preterm and/or low birth-weight infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2004;2:CD000390.
    88. Harrison L, Oliver L, Cunningham K, et al. Effects of gentle human touch on preterm infants. Neonatal Netw. 1996;15:35–42.
    89. Ludington-Hoe SM, Anderson GC, Swinth JY, et al. Randomized controlled trial of kangaroo care: cardiorespiratory and thermal effects on healthy preterm infants. Neonatal Netw. 2004;23:39–48.
    90. Browne JV. Considerations for touch and massage in the neonatal intensive care unit. Neonatal Netw. 2000;19:61–63.
    91. Hough JL, Flenady V, Johnston L, et al. Chest physiotherapy for reducing respiratory morbidity in infants requiring ventilatory support. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2008;3:CD006445.
    92. Sweeney JK. Neonatal hydrotherapy: an adjunct to developmental intervention in an intensive care nursery setting. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr. 1983;3:39–52.
    93. Sweeney JK. Feeding proficiency in preterm neonates following hydrotherapy in the NICU setting [abstract]. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2003;15:63.
    94. Kaaresen PI, Rønning JA, Ulvund SE, et al. A randomized, controlled trial of the effectiveness of an early-intervention program in reducing parenting stress after preterm birth. Pediatrics. 2006;118:e9–e19.
    95. Melnyk BM, Feinstein NF, Alpert-Gillis L, et al. Reducing premature infants' length of stay and improving parents' mental health outcomes with the Creating Opportunities for Parent Empowerment (COPE) neonatal intensive care unit program: a randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2006;118:e1414–e1427.
    96. Field TM, Dempsey J, Hallock N, et al. Mothers' assessments of the behaviors of their infants. Infant Behav Dev. 1978;1:157–167.
    97. Cardone LA, Gilkerson L. Family administered neonatal activities: a first step in the integration of parental perceptions and newborn behavior. Inf Mental Health J. 1990;11:127–131.
    98. Das Eiden R, Reifman A. Effects of Brazelton demonstrations on later parenting. J Pediatr Psych. 1996;21:857–868.
    99. Goldstein LA, Campbell SK. Effectiveness of the Test of Infant Motor Performance as an educational tool for mothers. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2008;20:152–159.
    100. Dusing SC, Murray T, Stern M. Parent preferences for motor development education in the neonatal intensive care unit. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2008;20:363–368.
    101. Scales LH, McEwen IR, Murray C. Parents' perceived benefits of physical therapists' direct intervention compared with parental instruction in early intervention. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2007;19:196–202.
    102. Byrne E, Constantinou J, Sweeney JK, et al. Comparison of neonatal physical therapy instructional methods on parent competency in a neonatal intensive care setting [abstract 90]. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009;51 (suppl 5):35.
    103. Lekskulchai R, Cole J. Effect of a developmental program on motor performance in infants born preterm. Aust J Physiother. 2001;47:169–176.
    104. Heathcock JC, Lobo M, Galloway JC. Movement training advances the emergence of reaching in infants born at less than 33 weeks of gestational age: a randomized clinical trial. Phys Ther. 2008;88:310–322.
    105. Oxford Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine. Levels of Evidence (March 2009). Available at http://www.cebm.net. Accessed September 9, 2009.
    Keywords:

    clinical practice guidelines; evidence-based practice; high-risk infant; neonatal intensive care units; neonatology; physical therapy; infant born preterm; reference standards/clinical

    © 2010 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Inc.