Cortical visual impairment, also known as cerebral visual impairment or CVI, is a neurological disorder that results in bilateral visual impairment caused by damage to the posterior visual pathways.1–5 Clinically, it manifests as a bilateral reduction in visual acuity with normal ocular structures and normal pupil response.1–5 The most common etiology for CVI is hypoxic brain injury.1,2,6 Other causes of CVI include head injury (such as shaken baby syndrome), infection, hydrocephalus, and metabolic disorders.2,6,7
The most common clinical finding of CVI is reduced visual acuity, often associated with reduced contrast sensitivity, visual field defects, and ocular motility abnormalities.8–12 The severity of vision loss in patients with CVI is variable. Furthermore, the potential for recovery is also variable. Interestingly, some patients with CVI improve over time whereas others show no change in vision function.8,13 Currently, it is thought that the prognosis for patients with CVI may depend on the etiology, age of onset, and severity of brain damage.2 Additionally, patients with periventricular leukomalacia and subsequent damage to the optic radiations may have a poorer prognosis than patients with isolated cortical damage.3,8
Both preferential looking (PL) and the sweep visual-evoked potential (VEP) can be used to measure visual acuity.14–19 Both techniques have been shown to be reliable measures of visual acuity in patients with CVI.10,17,18 The VEP is an electrophysiological technique that provides general information about the occipital response to visual stimuli.19 Because the VEP represents a neural response, it is possible to obtain a visual acuity measure for a child who is not verbal and/or unable to generate an oculomotor response to communicate what they see. Even though the ability of the VEP to provide a measurement of vision without the influence of motor ability offers several advantages for the CVI population, a behavioral measure of vision may be more helpful in describing the patient's vision function in their natural environment.
Some studies report that children with vision impairment show lower visual acuity on behavioral measures of acuity compared to VEP measures.10,17,20 Furthermore, the difference between sweep VEP acuity and behavioral acuity increases with poorer visual acuity21 and in patients with developmental disabilities.10 At an initial examination, the sweep VEP is often the only testable method of visual acuity, especially in young children with multiple disabilities. As the child matures over time, behavioral measures of vision function often become possible and/or more accurate. Some think of VEP acuity as a measure of the integrity of the neural substrate used for vision processing and the behavioral measure of acuity as the level of vision the patient is able to access. So, even though the sweep VEP measure may suggest vision function better than what the child shows behaviorally at the initial examination, the child's behavioral acuity might improve over time to reach the acuity that was initially found with the sweep VEP. If true, it would be helpful to the child's parents and educators to know a general level of visual impairment that can be expected for the child in the future.
A previous study found that sweep VEP grating acuity is predictive of future recognition acuity in children with albinism.22 In this study, 5 of 13 patients had final recognition acuity within 1 Snellen line of initial sweep VEP acuity; and five additional patients had final recognition acuity that surpassed the initial sweep VEP acuity by 2 to 3 lines. They found that the mean duration for recognition acuity to reach the initial VEP acuity was 5.4 years. Another study reported that while grating acuity measured by either VEP or PL is predictive of future recognition acuity in low birth-weight children, abnormal VEP grating acuity is a stronger indicator of a long-term deficit than PL acuity.23 Furthermore, Thompson et al24 evaluated the correlation of recognition acuity to sweep VEP acuity in patients aged 4 to 16 years who underwent lensectomy for congenital cataracts at a young age. They reported a strong correlation between VEP estimation and later recognition acuity in the patients studied.
To date, there is no study in the literature comparing sweep VEP acuity with future behavioral visual acuity in young patients with CVI. The purpose of this study is to determine the relationship between early sweep VEP visual acuity and future behavioral acuity in patients with CVI. If predictive, early sweep VEP acuity measurements could be a guide for the potential of vision function development in patients with CVI. The initial sweep VEP may serve as a realistic expectation of future vision function for the parents and teachers of young patients with CVI.
METHODS
A retrospective study was performed on 33 young patients (13 female and 20 male) with CVI. The research study was approved by the UC Berkeley Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects and informed consent was obtained from the patients and/or the patients' parent before being included in the study. Sweep VEP testing was performed at the first visit as part of a comprehensive eye examination. A behavioral measure of visual acuity (either PL cards or single symbols, depending on the patient's cognitive and motor ability) was also measured. The patients were then followed over time and retested at a later date. Significant refractive error was corrected before acuity testing. Patients with ocular pathology or whose reduction in vision could be attributed to a condition other than CVI were excluded. The median age of the patients at the initial visit was 4.8 years (range: 1.3 to 19.2 years). The study participants were then followed for an average of 6.9 years (SD: 3.5 years). At the final examination, behavioral acuity was retested as part of another comprehensive eye examination.
A summary of the study participants and etiology of CVI is listed in Table 1. The presumed causes of CVI are as follows: birth asphyxia (n = 8), acute episode of hypoxia (n = 4), infection (n = 4), periventricular leukomalacia (n = 3), trauma (n = 2), static encephalopathy (n = 2), hydrocephalus (n = 1), multiple complications (n = 3), and six cases with an unknown origin.
TABLE 1: Summary of participants (age, etiology of CVI, and the duration of time followed)
VEP Technique
The patients were presented with high contrast (80% Michelson) sinusoidal luminance vertical gratings on a high-resolution video display monitor with an average luminance of 80 cd/m2. Over a period of 10 s, the spatial frequency of the reversing grating was incremented logarithmically in 19 steps, spanning a 10 to 1 range of spatial frequencies expressed in cycles per degree. Testing was performed under binocular viewing with proper refractive error correction.
The gratings were modulated at a rate of 12 contrast reversals per second. The second harmonic component of the response at 12 Hz is extracted by a Discrete Fourier Transform and compared to an adjacent frequency band at 14 Hz, which did not contain any visual evoked activity. The adjacent frequency measurements were used to estimate the noise background during the trial and reject portions of the record with muscle spikes or movement artifacts. Sweep VEP acuity was estimated by the method described by Norcia and Tyler.19 Determination of visual acuity was based on the linear decline of the VEP amplitude and the increase in the implicit time near the acuity threshold.19 The extrapolation technique took into account the signal-to-noise ratio and phase statistics. Multiple sweeps were recorded and the trial yielding the highest spatial frequency that met scoring criteria was used for the visual acuity estimate.19 VEP grating acuity (in cycles per degree) was converted into logMAR for data analysis, using the conversion that 1 c/deg is mathematically equivalent to 20/600 (logMAR = 1.477) and 30 c/deg is mathematically equivalent to 20/20 (logMAR = 0).
Behavioral Acuity Technique
Berkeley Grating Acuity Cards,25 a clinical variation of the Teller Acuity Card procedure was used to measure forced choice PL acuity. The patient was presented with a rectangular card that had two targets positioned on a gray homogenous background. One target matched the gray background and the other target contained a grating pattern. If the patient responded by turning his/her eyes or head in the direction of the grating pattern, then the patient's response was interpreted as an ability of the patient to see the target. Threshold visual acuity was determined by observing the finest grating to which the patient reliably oriented their gaze for a minimum of three out-of four presentations (75% correct).
The acuity cards were presented either horizontally or vertically to minimize the effects of visual field defects and either lateral to or central to the gaze direction to minimize the effects of oculomotor abnormalities. The acuity cards were presented in sequential order from lower to higher spatial frequencies. Each card was presented as many times as necessary to decide whether the patient could resolve the grating on the card. The examiner was masked to the absolute frequency of the grating on the card and to the location of the grating on each card. As with VEP acuity, the PL grating acuity (in cycles per degree) was converted into logMAR for data analysis.
Lea single symbols cards were used for the patients who were capable of performing a recognition acuity task. The patient was presented with a pair of 10 cm square cards. One card had the Lea house (or arrow) symbol and the other card had the Lea apple (or heart) symbol positioned in the center of the card. The size of the symbols varied logarithmically between one pair of cards and the next. For each card pair, the patient was asked to either “point to the apple (or heart)” or “point to the house (or arrow).” Always starting at a size level where the recognition was easy for the patient and moving to smaller sized targets, the acuity was determined by a three out of four (75% correct) criteria following a forced-choice procedure.
Data Analysis
As previously stated, a primary goal of this study is to assess the relationship between the initial sweep VEP acuity and a later behavioral acuity in young patients with CVI. The initial sweep VEP acuities will first be compared to the initial behavioral acuities and then compared to the final behavioral acuities graphically and with a repeated measures t-test. Factors such as the age of the child, the duration between measures, and the etiology of CVI will also be examined as potential confounders for a patient's visual outcome. Additionally, three individual cases will be briefly described.
RESULTS
Sweep VEP testing was possible in all study participants at the first visit. A behavioral measure of visual acuity was possible in 31 of 33 patients at the first visit. Twenty-one of the patients were tested with PL cards and the remaining 12 patients were tested with single symbols. Every participant was measured with the same behavioral test at both the initial and final examination. VEP data from the final examination were not available for many of the participants; therefore, any change in VEP acuity over time was not included in this analysis.
All the participants had substantial visual impairment. The mean initial VEP acuity was 20/94 (0.67 logMAR), and the mean initial behavioral acuity was 20/475 (1.38 logMAR). The average difference between the two measures of acuity was 0.55 log unit, with the behavioral measure reporting a poorer visual acuity in all patients. Therefore, at the initial test, the behavioral measure of acuity was significantly worse than the VEP measure of acuity (t = 9.68; dF = 30; p < 0.0001). Fig. 1 graphically displays a comparison between VEP acuity and behavioral acuity at the initial examination. All the points fall above the 1:1 line, showing that all patients had better visual acuity measured with the VEP than measured behaviorally.
FIGURE 1.:
The initial behavioral acuity value (in logMAR form) is plotted as a function of the initial sweep VEP acuity value (also in logMAR form). The dashed black line is the 1:1 line. If a point is on the dashed line, the patient's initial behavioral visual acuity was the same as their initial VEP acuity. A higher point on the vertical axis and a point to the right on the horizontal axis indicate a worse visual acuity. If a point is above the 1:1 line, the patient's initial behavioral acuity was worse than their initial sweep VEP acuity.
The mean time between the initial sweep VEP testing and the final behavioral testing was 6.9 years (SD: 3.5 years). At the final visit, a behavioral measure of acuity was possible for all 33 patients. The mean final behavioral acuity was 20/150 (0.88 logMAR). The average difference between the initial VEP acuity and the final behavioral acuity was 0.01 log unit. The initial VEP measure was not statistically different from the final behavioral measure (t = 0.11; dF = 32; p = 0.45). Fig. 2 graphically displays a comparison between the initial VEP acuity and the later behavioral acuity.
FIGURE 2.:
The final behavioral acuity value (in logMAR form) is plotted as a function of the initial sweep VEP measure of acuity (also in logMAR form). The dashed black line is the 1:1 line. Points above the 1:1 line indicate the behavioral acuity did not improve to reach the initial VEP acuity. Points below the line show patients whose final behavioral acuity was better than their initial VEP acuity.
Twenty-six of the patients tested (79%) had a final behavioral acuity within 0.1 log unit (∼1 chart line) or better than their initial VEP acuity. Twenty-seven (82%) had a final behavioral acuity within 0.2 log unit (∼2 chart lines) or better than their initial VEP acuity.
The age of the child was not significantly correlated with the degree of agreement between the final behavioral acuity and the initial VEP acuity (r = 0.082; dF = 31; p = 0.65). The six patients whose behavioral acuity did not improve to within 0.2 log unit of the initial VEP were essential the same age as those who did improve (mean: 5.4 years vs. 5.7 years; t = −0.58; dF = 31; p = 0.28). Furthermore, in this study population, there was no significant relationship between the etiology of CVI and whether the child behaviorally met the acuity level measured by the initial sweep VEP (χ2 = 0.65; dF = 32; p = ns). Because the insult occurred within 3 months of birth for 89% of the participants with a known etiology, there was not enough statistical power to adequately assess the influence of the age of the insult on recovery.
The duration of time between acuity measures was correlated with the degree of final agreement of the two measures (r = 0.400; dF = 31; p < 0.05). The six participants whose behavioral acuity did not improve to within 0.2 log unit of the initial VEP were followed for a slightly shorter duration when compared to those who did improve to meet their initial VEP acuity (t = −1.36; dF = 31; p = 0.09). The average duration of follow-up for those who did not improve to the acuity level of the initial sweep VEP acuity was 5.2 years (SD: 2.6 years). The average duration of follow-up for those who did improve to meet the initial VEP acuity was 7.3 years (SD: 3.6 years). Additionally, the average difference between the initial VEP measure and the initial behavioral measure of acuity was larger for those who did not behaviorally meet the initial VEP acuity value (r = 0.493; dF = 31; p < 0.01). These two findings (a larger difference between the initial values of acuity and a shorter duration of follow-up) may suggest that if given more time, the children who did not behaviorally meet the acuity measured with the initial VEP might continue to improve to eventually meet the level of acuity measured by the initial VEP. However, as described below, further analysis revealed that a third component, rate of behavioral acuity improvement, was also different between the two groups.
There is a significant difference in the rate of behavioral acuity improvement between the two groups (t = 2.27; dF = 29; p = 0.01). The average rate of change for those who did improve to their initial VEP acuity was 0.11 log unit per year (approximately one chart line per year). Although the average rate of change for those who did not improve to their initial VEP acuity was only 0.03 log unit per year. The significantly slower rate of improvement for the patients who have not yet met their initial VEP acuity suggests that that it will take much longer for them to do so, if they ever do. Furthermore, this finding suggests that the difference in duration of follow-up and the difference in initial discrepancy between the two measures for this study population are not solely responsible for the difference between the two groups. The few participants who did not behaviorally meet their initial VEP acuity are improving at a much slower rate than the majority of the patients tested.
Fig. 3A is a graphical representation of one of the 18 patients whose final behavioral acuity met the initial VEP acuity. At the initial examination, this patient was 3.8 years old and had a 0.70 log unit difference between his VEP acuity and his behavioral acuity. However, during the course of 11.7 years, his behavioral acuity improved to become very similar to his initial VEP acuity (0.48 log unit vs. 0.41 log unit). Behavioral data were collected three times for this patient and this is shown graphically. However, only the first and last measures are included in the data analysis.
FIGURE 3.:
A, Initial sweep VEP acuity (solid symbol) and behavioral acuity (open symbols) is plotted as a function of age for a patient whose final behavioral acuity met their initial VEP acuity. Visual acuity is expressed in terms of logMAR on the left vertical axis. A higher point on the vertical axis indicates a worse visual acuity. B, Initial sweep VEP acuity (solid symbol) and behavioral acuity (open symbols) is plotted as a function of age for a patient whose final behavioral acuity did not meet their initial VEP acuity. C, Initial sweep VEP acuity (solid symbol) and behavioral acuity (open symbols) is plotted as a function of age for another patient whose final behavioral acuity did not meet their initial VEP acuity.
Fig. 3B and C are graphical representations of the six patients who did not behaviorally meet their initial sweep VEP acuity. The pattern seen in Fig. 3B is an example of the majority of patients (4 of the 6) who did not meet the initial VEP acuity. This child was first seen at 5.5 years of age. At this initial examination, there was a 0.90 log unit difference between her VEP and behavioral acuity. When she was seen again 8 years later, her behavioral acuity remained approximately the same as that reported at her initial examination. Her average behavioral acuity rate of change was only 0.01 log unit per year. The rates of change for the other three patients with a similar outcome ranged from 0.00 to 0.02 log unit per year.
Fig. 3C is an example of a patient who did not behaviorally met the initial VEP acuity, but if the current trend continues, the child is likely to behaviorally meet the initial VEP acuity value in the future. This pattern was seen in two of the six patients who did not behaviorally meet their initial VEP. The example patient was first seen at 4.4 years of age and there was a 1.1 log unit difference between the two measures of acuity. Nine years later, a 0.45 log unit difference remains. However, if the trend continues, the child is likely to behaviorally meet the initial VEP acuity value in the near future. As previously mentioned, only two of the patients followed this pattern where there was a sizable improvement in their behavioral acuity and if given more time it is conceivable that they will behaviorally meet their initial VEP acuity value. The average rate of change for both of these patients was 0.07 log unit per year.
DISCUSSION
Because CVI is often associated with global cognitive and motor disabilities, the assessment of visual acuity in patients with CVI can be challenging. Assessment of vision function with behavioral measures is often not possible for patients with severe CVI or for young infants with CVI. Sweep VEP is a technique that can be used to assess visual acuity in infants and children with severe CVI. However, considerable discrepancies in threshold visual acuity have been noted between behavioral and electrophysiological techniques. As seen in this data set, the discrepancy between the two measures can be quite large.
In this study, the behavioral measure of acuity was worse than the sweep VEP measure of acuity in all study participants at the initial examination (mean: 20/110 vs. 20/475). However, even though the initial VEP measure was much better than the initial behavioral measure, the VEP measure was statistically similar to the behavioral visual acuity measured on average 6.9 years later.
Our study demonstrates that sweep VEP testing can be used as a predictive tool for at least the lower limit of future behavioral acuity in patients with CVI. Eighty-two percent of the patients studied either met or surpassed the visual acuity measured by the initial sweep VEP. In four of the six patients whose behavioral acuity did not improve to the level of their initial VEP acuity, it appears unlikely that they will improve behaviorally to meet their initial VEP acuity in the near future. This is because their rate of improvement (average of 0.03 log unit per year) is far below that of the other study participants (average of 0.11 log unit per year).
Despite the fact that CVI is the leading cause of bilateral visual impairment in industrialized nations, there have been very few longitudinal evaluations of vision function in patients with CVI to date. Therefore, there is a pressing need for more research quantifying and evaluating the various aspects of vision loss in patients with CVI. Quantitative information about a patient's condition will not only be useful clinically but also be extremely valuable in enabling better information about the patient's visual capabilities to be provided to the young patient's educators and family. Currently, the prognosis of vision function potential in patients with CVI is unknown. If predictive, an early sweep VEP acuity measurement could be a guide for the potential of vision function development in young patients with CVI. An early VEP may provide a conservative estimation of future behavioral vision function in young patients with CVI. Even though there may be a large discrepancy between VEP and behavioral acuity measures at initial examination, parents and teachers of young patients with CVI can be informed that it is likely that their child's vision function will improve to at least the level of the initial VEP.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was supported by National Eye Institute, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, grant numbers: T32 EY07043, K12 EY017269, and NIDRR H133E060001.
Tonya Watson
School of Optometry
University of California, Berkeley
200 Minor Hall
Berkeley, California 94720-2020
e-mail: [email protected]
REFERENCES
1. Fulton AB, Hansen RM, Mayer L, Afshari NA, Afshari MA. The visually inattentive infant. In: Albert DM, Jakobiec FA, eds. Principles and Practice of Ophthalmology, 2nd ed. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders; 2000:4483–90.
2. Good WV, Jan JE, DeSa L, Barkovich AJ, Groenveld M, Hoyt CS.
Cortical visual impairment in children. Surv Ophthalmol 1994;38:351–64.
3. Lambert SR, Hoyt CS, Jan JE, Barkovich J, Flodmark O. Visual recovery from hypoxic cortical blindness during childhood. Computed tomographic and magnetic resonance imaging predictors. Arch Ophthalmol 1987;105:1371–7.
4. Chen TC, Weinberg MH, Catalano RA, Simon JW, Wagle WA. Development of object vision in infants with permanent
cortical visual impairment. Am J Ophthalmol 1992;114:575–8.
5. Afshari MA, Afshari NA, Fulton AB.
Cortical visual impairment in infants and children. Int Ophthalmol Clin 2001;41:159–69.
6. Fazzi E, Signorini SG, Bova SM, La Piana R, Ondei P, Bertone C, Misefari W, Bianchi PE. Spectrum of visual disorders in children with
cerebral visual impairment. J Child Neurol 2007;22:294–301.
7. Huo R, Burden SK, Hoyt CS, Good WV. Chronic
cortical visual impairment in children: aetiology, prognosis, and associated neurological deficits. Br J Ophthalmol 1999;83:670–5.
8. Watson T, Orel-Bixler D, Haegerstrom-Portnoy G. Longitudinal quantitative assessment of vision function in children with
cortical visual impairment. Optom Vis Sci 2007;84:471–80.
9. Dutton GN, Jacobson LK.
Cerebral visual impairment in children. Semin Neonatol 2001;6:477–85.
10. Good WV. Development of a quantitative method to measure vision in children with chronic
cortical visual impairment. Trans Am Ophthalmol Soc 2001;99:253–69.
11. Cannao M, Morretti G, Ricci A. Complex visual problems in childhood. Neuroophthalmology 1986;6:321–7.
12. Jacobson LK, Dutton GN. Periventricular leukomalacia: an important cause of visual and ocular motility dysfunction in children. Surv Ophthalmol 2000;45:1–13.
13. Matsuba CA, Jan JE. Long-term outcome of children with
cortical visual impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol 2006;48:508–12.
14. Allen D, Tyler CW, Norcia AM. Development of grating acuity and contrast sensitivity in the central and peripheral visual field of the human infant. Vision Res 1996;36:1945–53.
15. Teller DY, McDonald MA, Preston K, Sebris SL, Dobson V. Assessment of
visual acuity in infants and children: the acuity card procedure. Dev Med Child Neurol 1986;28:779–89.
16. Mayer DL, Dobson V. Assessment of vision in young children: a new operant approach yields estimates of acuity. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1980;19:566–70.
17. Tinelli F, Pei F, Guzzetta A, Bancale A, Mazzotti S, Baldassi S, Cioni G. The assessment of
visual acuity in children with periventricular damage: a comparison of behavioural and electrophysiological techniques. Vision Res 2008;48:1233–41.
18. Birch EE, Bane MC. Forced-choice preferential looking acuity of children with
cortical visual impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol 1991;33:722–9.
19. Norcia AM, Tyler CW. Infant VEP acuity measurements: analysis of individual differences and measurement error. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1985;61:359–69.
20. Katsumi O, Denno S, Arai M, De Lopes Faria J, Hirose T. Comparison of preferential looking acuity and pattern reversal visual evoked response acuity in pediatric patients. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 1997;235:684–90.
21. Orel-Bixler D, Haegerstrom-Portnoy G, Hall A. Visual assessment of the multiply handicapped patient. Optom Vis Sci 1989;66:530–6.
22. Bradfield YS, France TD, Verhoeve J, Gangnon RE. Sweep visual evoked potential testing as a predictor of recognition acuity in albinism. Arch Ophthalmol 2007;125:628–33.
23. O'Connor AR, Birch EE, Leffler J, Salomao SR, Spencer R. Infant visual evoked potential (VEP) and preferential-looking (PL) grating acuity in low birth weight children. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2003;44:E-abstract 2712.
24. Thompson DA, Moller H, Russell-Eggitt I, Kriss A.
Visual acuity in unilateral cataract. Br J Ophthalmol 1996;80:794–8.
25. Haegerstrom-Portnoy G, Schneck ME, Lott LA, Brabyn JA. The relation between
visual acuity and other spatial vision measures. Optom Vis Sci 2000;77:653–62.