Listening to Fast-Tempo Music Delays the Onset of Neuromuscular Fatigue : The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research

Secondary Logo

Journal Logo

Original Research

Listening to Fast-Tempo Music Delays the Onset of Neuromuscular Fatigue

Centala, Jacob1,2; Pogorel, Cameron1,2; Pummill, Scott W.1,2; Malek, Moh H.1,2

Author Information
Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research 34(3):p 617-622, March 2020. | DOI: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000003417
  • Free

Abstract

Centala, J, Pogorel, C, Pummill, SW, and Malek, MH. Listening to fast-tempo music delays the onset of neuromuscular fatigue. J Strength Cond Res 34(3): 617–622, 2020—Studies determining the effect of music on physical performance have primarily focused on outcomes such as running time to exhaustion, blood lactate, or maximal oxygen uptake. The electromyographic fatigue threshold (EMGFT) is determined through a single incremental test and operationally defined as the highest exercise intensity that can be sustained indefinitely without an increase in EMG activity of the working muscle. To date, no studies have examined the role of fast-tempo music on EMGFT. The purpose of this investigation, therefore, was to determine whether fast-tempo music attenuates neuromuscular fatigue as measured by the EMGFT. We hypothesized that listening to fast-tempo music during exercise would increase the estimated EMGFT compared with the control condition. Secondarily, we hypothesized that maximal power output would also increase as a result of listening to fast-tempo music during the exercise workbout. Ten healthy college-aged men (mean ± SEM: age, 25.3 ± 0.8 years [range from 22 to 31 years]; body mass, 78.3 ± 1.8 kg; height: 1.77 ± 0.02 m) visited the laboratory on 2 occasions separated by 7 days. The EMGFT was determined from an incremental single-leg knee-extensor ergometer for each visit. In a randomized order, subjects either listened to music or no music for the 2 visits. All music was presented as instrumentals and randomized with a tempo ranging between 137 and 160 b·min−1. The results indicated that listening to fast-tempo music during exercise increased maximal power output (No Music: 48 ± 4; Music: 54 ± 3 W; p = 0.02) and EMGFT (No Music: 27 ± 3; Music: 34 ± 4 W; p = 0.008). There were, however, no significant mean differences between the 2 conditions (no music vs. music) for absolute and relative end-exercise heart rate as well as end-exercise rating of perceived exertion for the exercised leg. These findings suggest that listening to fast-tempo music increased overall exercise tolerance as well as the neuromuscular fatigue threshold. The results are applicable to both sport and rehabilitative settings.

Introduction

Music may be used to increase motivation during an exercise activity or as a distraction from perceiving fatigue (18–20). In both sport and rehabilitative settings, listening to music may have potential utility to enhance performance (18–20). Studies determining the effect of music on physical activity have primarily focused on outcomes such as running time-to-exhaustion, blood lactate, or maximal oxygen uptake (9,33). In addition, studies suggest that slow-tempo music (≤80 b·min−1) does not influence physical performance and is comparable with the control condition (no music) (9,19,20). Thus, there is growing evidence that fast-tempo music may elicit a response that enhances physical performance (19,20,34). The potential physiological mechanism(s) underlying the effect of music on performance may reside in altering perceptions of fatigue as well as changes in brain activity (18–20,32). It has been suggested that emotional responses to the music such as eliciting a unique memory may also contribute to the observed increase in performance (19,20). What remains unknown is the influence of fast-tempo music on neuromuscular fatigue.

Surface electromyography (EMG) is a noninvasive method of examining muscle function under various exercise modalities (2). The EMG amplitude is influenced by the number of activated motor units and their firing rates (2). The EMG amplitude was reported by Travis et al. (37) to be a stable and reliable measure for continuous muscle action. The electromyographic fatigue threshold (EMGFT) is the highest exercise intensity that an individual can maintain for an extended period without an increase in the EMG amplitude (6,12). It has been shown that the EMGFT is a highly reliable measure (24). In a validation study, Briscoe et al. (6) reported that subjects cycling at an exercise intensity corresponding to 130% of their EMGFT exhibited a significant increase in the EMG amplitude vs. time relationship and were able to maintain the exercise workbout for ∼12 minutes. For exercise intensities below the EMGFT, however, the EMG amplitude vs. time relationship was unchanged (6). Studies indicate that the EMGFT is influenced by different modalities such as low-dose caffeine consumption (29), cognitive fatigue (10), pre-exhaustive exercise (14), and repeated exercise workbouts separated by 1 hour (5). The EMGFT may provide a unique measure to determine the influence of fast-tempo music on neuromuscular fatigue.

To date, no studies have examined the role of fast-tempo music on EMGFT. The purpose of this investigation, therefore, was to determine whether fast-tempo music attenuates neuromuscular fatigue as measured by EMGFT. We hypothesized that listening to fast-tempo music during exercise would increase the estimated EMGFT compared with the control condition. Secondarily, we hypothesized that maximal power output would also increase as a result of listening to fast-tempo music during the exercise workbout.

Methods

Experimental Approach to the Problem

Each subject visited the laboratory on 2 separate occasions during a 7-day period. For 1 of the 2 visits, subjects listened to fast-tempo music ranging from 137 to 160 b·min−1 during the exercise test, whereas for the other visit no music was played during the exercise test. Both testing sessions (i.e., music and no music) were randomized for each subject, and each subject had EMG electrodes placed on their rectus femoris muscle to measure the EMG activity during the exercise test.

Subjects

Ten healthy college-aged men (mean ± SEM: age, 25.3 ± 0.8 years [range from 22 to 31 years]; body mass, 78.3 ± 1.8 kg; height: 1.77 ± 0.02 m) were recruited for this study. The subjects were asked to refrain from exercise 24 hours before each of the testing sessions. Although there were no dietary restrictions, each subject was asked to refrain from caffeine consumption 24 hours before each testing session (8,10,13,26,27). Furthermore, the 2 testing sessions, for each subject, were separated by 7 days and performed at relatively the same time (±1 hour). Moreover, each subject wore a Polar Heart Watch system (Polar Electro Inc., Lake Success, NY) to monitor heart rate during the 2 exercise sessions. All procedures were approved by the Wayne State University Institutional Review Board for Human Subjects (IRB010918M1E), and each subject signed an informed consent before testing.

Procedures

Mood Questionnaire

Each subject completed the Brunel Mood Scale (BRUMS) before and immediately after each exercise session. The BRUMS is a 24-item questionnaire used to assess moods of tension, depression, anger, vigor, fatigue, and confusion on 5-point Likert scale; 0 = not at all to 4 = extremely (10,35,36).

Motivation Questionnaire

To determine the subject's intrinsic motivation, at each exercise session, we used the questionnaire developed and validated by Matthews et al. (28) and used in our previous study (10). This inventory consists of 7 items, which determines the subject's level of motivation and interest. All questions are based on a 5-point Likert scale (0 = not at all to 4 = extremely).

Incremental Single-Leg Knee Extensor

The single-leg knee-extensor ergometry has been used previously in our laboratory (8,10,13,26,27) and exclusively focuses the exercise demand on the quadriceps femoris muscles (31). Briefly, each subject was semirecumbent in an adjustable chair with a special ankle boot placed on the nondominant leg and connected by a bar to the ergometer. The dominant leg (based on kicking preference) was on a platform attached to the knee-extensor device (8,10,13,26,27). Contractions of the quadriceps muscles caused the lower part of the leg to extend through an angle of ∼90 to ∼10°. Therefore, the lower leg traveled with an arc-shaped trajectory of approximately 80° in an alternating manner. The momentum of the ergometer returned the relaxed leg passively to the start position, and, as a result, the quadriceps muscle was functionally isolated (10,13,26,27,31). After a period of stabilization at rest, subjects began kicking at 4 W for 2 minutes. The power output was then increased by 4 W every minute throughout the test until subjects reached volitional fatigue. The cadence during the exercise workbout was maintained at 70 revolutions per minute (10,13,26,27). Termination of the exercise test was determined if the subject met the following criteria: (a) rating of perceived exertion of 8 or higher (scale 0–10) and (b) inability to maintain the targeted kicking cadence (10,13,26,27).

Listening to Music During Exercise

For the visit in which the subject listened to music, a selection of songs ranging from 137 to 160 b·min−1 was selected by the investigators. The compilation of instrumental music (∼30 minutes, double the anticipated exercise testing time) did not focus on any one genre (Table 1). The order of music presented was randomized, using the shuffle feature on the computer audio program, for each subject. Subjects wore over-the-ear headphones to listen to the music. The volume of the music was set at the same level for all subjects. For the control visit, subjects also wore the headphones, but no music was played. In addition, communication between the researchers and the subject were conducted through signs for both visits that replaced the strong verbal encouragement provided to subjects throughout the exercise workbout (10,13,26,27).

T1
Table 1:
List of instrumental music used in this study.*

Electromyography Electrode Placement

Separate bipolar (20-mm center-to-center) surface electrode (EL500-6; BIOPAC Systems, Inc., Santa Barbara, CA) arrangements were positioned over the longitudinal axes of the rectus femoris muscle. This site was traced with a permanent marker for the subsequent visit to maintain the consistency of the electrode placement (10,13,26,27). The electrodes for the rectus femoris muscle were placed at 50% the distance between the anterior superior iliac spine and the superior border of the patella (15). The reference electrode was placed over the iliac crest. The shaved skin at each electrode site was carefully abraded and cleaned with alcohol, and interelectrode impedance was kept below 2,000 ohms consistent with our previous work (10,13,26,27). The EMG signal from each electrode placement site was amplified (gain: ×1,000) using differential amplifiers (EMG 100B; BIOPAC Systems, Inc.).

Determination of EMGFT and Signal Processing

The EMGFT was determined for each subject at each visit. Briefly, the absolute EMG amplitude (microvolts root mean square, µVrms) was calculated from 6 data points for each 1-minute stage and then plotted vs. time (Figure 1) (5,10,14,22). Specifically, each data point represents a 10-second epoch in which all completed bursts within that window were selected for signal processing. The raw EMG signals were digitized at 1,000 Hz and stored in a personal computer (Dell Inspiron E1705; Dell Inc., Round Rock, TX) for subsequent analysis (10,13,26,27). All signal processing was performed using custom programs written with LabVIEW programming software (version 2014; National Instruments, Austin, TX). The EMG signals were bandpass-filtered (fourth-order Butterworth) at 10–500 Hz. Thus, for each power output, the EMG amplitude vs. time relationship was analyzed using linear regression (Figure 1). The EMGFT is a derived value from the average of the highest power output with a nonsignificant slope coefficient and the lowest power output with a significant positive slope coefficient (5,6,10,12–14,22).

F1
Figure 1.:
Graphical representation of the EMGFT for a single subject's responses to the no music and music conditions.

Statistical Analyses

All data presented in the current study are mean ± SEM. For the responses to the BRUMS questionnaire two-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVAs) were used (25). The EMGFT for each subject and each condition was calculated as described above. Therefore, the comparison of the EMGFT between the 2 conditions as well as other exercise outcomes was analyzed using a paired-sample t-test (25). In addition, to determine the potential influence of order effect, we compared the EMGFT from the 2 visits independent of the experimental conditions (i.e., no music vs. music) using a paired-sample t-test. Statistical significance was set at p ≤ 0.05, and data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences Software (SPSS, v. 25; IBM, Armonk, NY).

Results

Mood and Motivation Questionnaire

For the BRUMS questionnaire, we examined fatigue and vigor that are the 2 categories most relevant to the present investigation. For fatigue, the 2 [exercise condition: no music or music] × 2 [time: before and immediately after exercise] repeated-measures ANOVA indicated no significant interaction [F(1,18) = 1.26; p = 0.276] or main effect for exercise condition [F(1,18) = 0.037; p = 0.850]. There was, however, a significant main effect for time [F(1,18) = 8.08; p = 0.011], which revealed a significant mean difference (pre: 1.7 ± 0.3 vs. post: 3.9 ± 0.7; p = 0.011).

For vigor, the 2 [exercise condition: no music or music] × 2 [time: before and immediately after exercise] repeated-measures ANOVA indicated no significant interaction [F(1,18) = 1.73; p = 0.205] or main effect for exercise condition [F(1,18) = 0.334; p = 0.570]. There was, however, a significant main effect for time [F(1,18) = 12.03; p = 0.003], which revealed a significant mean difference (pre: 3.8 ± 0.6 vs. post: 5.3 ± 0.8; p = 0.03).

For the motivation questionnaire, we performed a paired-sample t-test since subjects completed the questionnaire once for each exercise condition. Therefore, we found that intrinsic motivation (no music: 22.0 ± 1.0 vs. music: 22.2 ± 0.9; p = 0.805) and success motivation (no music: 17.4 ± 1.5 vs. music: 18.4 ± 1.5; p = 0.177) were not statistically different between the 2 exercise conditions. Moreover, we found no significant mean differences between the 2 conditions when subjects were asked to score the following question “I am motivated to do this task” (no music: 3.3 ± 0.2 vs. music: 3.0 ± 0.1; p = 0.08).

Exercise Responses

As shown in Table 2, there were significant mean differences for maximal power output between the 2 exercise conditions. For the absolute and relative maximal heart rate values; however, there were no significant mean differences between the 2 exercise conditions. For the 2 exercise conditions, a similar pattern of responses was found for the rating of perceived exertion for the exercised leg. For the EMGFT (Figure 1), we found that listening to music increased the subject's neuromuscular threshold relative to the no music condition (Table 2). In addition, the paired-sample t-test indicated no influence of order effect [t(9) = 0.993; p = 0.35] on the EMGFT for the 2 visits independent of experimental condition (mean ± SEM: visit 1: 27 ± 5 W vs. visit 2: 32 ± 3 W).

T2
Table 2:
Comparison of exercise outcomes for the no music and music conditions.*

Discussion

The main and unique finding of this study was that listening to fast-tempo music, during incremental single-leg knee-extensor ergometry, increased the EMGFT compared with listening to no music. In addition, we found that subjects achieved a higher maximal power output when listening to music during the exercise workbout. Moreover, we found that fast-tempo music did not influence absolute or relative end-exercise heart rate. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to show that the EMGFT, derived from incremental single-leg knee-extensor ergometry, may be influenced by fast-tempo music.

Previous studies have examined the relationship between listening to music during various modes of exercise (9,17,23). For example, Hutchinson et al. (17) examined the effects of fast-tempo music while subjects were performing the Wingate test. To this extent, the investigators found that peak power and mean power significantly increased by ∼7.2 and ∼6.3%, respectively, when subjects were listening to music compared with the no music condition (17). Stork et al. (33) examined the influence of self-selected music on performance and perceived enjoyment of sprint interval exercise in a group of healthy college-aged men and women. Similar to the Hutchinson et al. (17) study, the Wingate test was performed with and without subjects listening to music. It should be noted, however, that Stork et al. (33) had subjects perform the Wingate test for the 2 conditions (music or no music) on the same day separated by 4 minutes of rest. The investigators reported significant increases in peak power and mean power as well as increased perceived enjoyment for the music condition (33). Overall, the results of these studies indicate that music can enhance exercise performance; however, several factors need to be considered.

Most studies examining the effects of music on physical performance have focused on in-task applications (21). That is, subjects listen to music during the exercise workbout (21). It should be noted, however, that pretask and posttask applications of music are also studied, but to a lesser extent. Nevertheless, for in-task studies of music, there is a distinction between music that is synchronous and asynchronous to the physical activity the subject is performing (21). Exercise to synchronous music is operationally defined as when the subject's movement is in concert with the rhythm of the music, whereas exercise to asynchronous music is operationally defined as when the subject's movement is not in concert with the rhythm of the music (21). For example, Bacon et al. (1) reported that oxygen uptake measured during cycle ergometry was significantly lower for the synchronous music condition than the slow-tempo asynchronous condition. Crust (7) reported that although subjects were significantly more motivated when listening to familiar asynchronous music during treadmill exercise than listening to unfamiliar asynchronous music. Moreover, there were no significant mean differences for heart rate response between the 2 music conditions (7).

In the present investigation, we found that our in-task application of listening to fast-tempo music (Table 1) increased maximal power output and the EMGFT by ∼13 and ∼26%, respectively, for incremental single-leg knee-extensor ergometry (Table 2). Moreover, the music tempo and movement of the single-leg knee-extensor ergometry were asynchronous. That is, subjects were asked to maintain a kicking cadence of 70 revolutions·min−1, whereas the tempo of the music subjects listened to was twice the kicking cadence (mean = 146; SEM = 4 b·min−1). In addition, there were no significant exercise condition by time interaction for the BRUMS questionnaire related to fatigue and vigor, yet there was no significant difference in the level of motivation between the 2 conditions. Other physiological outcomes such as absolute and relative end-exercise heart rates were not significantly different between the 2 conditions. This finding was consistent with those of Crust (7). One potential explanation for the lack of increase in end-exercise heart rate, in the current study, may be the exercise paradigm that was used. The single-leg knee-extensor ergometry was designed to minimize the contribution of the cardiovascular and respiratory systems to exercise since it is a single-joint exercise (5,26,31). Taken together, these results suggest that listening to fast-tempo music during incremental exercise, as described in this study, may increase exercise tolerance and influence neuromuscular fatigue.

The increased maximal power output and EMGFT observed in the current study when subjects listened to music during the exercise workbout may be, in part, due to potential changes in brain activity (18–20). Specifically, investigators have used techniques such as EEG (electroencephalography) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to identify various changing in the brain that are influenced by listening to music during exercise. For example, Bigliassi et al. (4) used a 64-channel EEG system to measure the electrical activity in the brain of subjects while performing submaximal isometric ankle-dorsiflexion task with and without listening to music. In addition, the investigators also measured EMG activity in the tibialis anterior and lateral gastrocnemius muscles (4). Bigliassi et al. (4) concluded that listening to music during the exercise task may result in switching from associative to dissociative thoughts, which increased performance. This was, in part, observed in the EEG signal that indicated a reduction in theta waves in various regions of the brain when the task was performed with music relative to the no music condition (4). To determine which regions of the brain are activated when listening to music during exercise investigators have used fMRI (3). Briefly, fMRI is a noninvasive methodology of measuring the brain's activity during various modalities (11,16). Bigliassi et al. (3) used fMRI to identify brain regions that are effected by listening to music during exercise. It should be noted, however, that the exercise mode used in the study was isometric grip test (3). Indeed, the investigators reported that the temporal regions and insular cortex showed increased activity when subjects listened to music during exercise than the no music condition (3). Moreover, the authors stated, “…significant activation in the LIFG [left inferior frontal gyrus] that was elicited by the interactive effects of exercise and music…” (3) (p. 136). Taken together, these results suggest that listening to music, during an exercise workbout, influences brain regions that result in attenuating internal cues related to fatigue (3).

Practical Applications

The findings of the current investigation indicate that listening to fast-tempo music increased overall exercise tolerance and the neuromuscular fatigue threshold. These results are applicable to both sport and rehabilitative settings. In either scenario, listening to fast-tempo music during the exercise workbout may, in part, distract the individual from the perception or sensation of fatigue. This, in turn, may allow the athlete (or client) to continuing performing the exercise workbout rather than stopping. One caveat that should be noted, however, is that in the current study, the exercise mode was an isolated single-joint aerobic exercise that minimizes the contribution of the cardiovascular and respiratory responses to exercise (10,14,30). Nevertheless, our findings are consistent with results from other laboratories showing the beneficial effects of listening to fast-tempo music during whole-body exercise.

References

1. Bacon CJ, Myers TR, Karageorghis CI. Effect of music-movement synchrony on exercise oxygen consumption. J Sports Med Phys Fitness 52: 359–365, 2012.
2. Basmajian JV, De Luca CJ. Muscles Alive, Their Functions Revealed by Electromyography. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins, 1985.
3. Bigliassi M, Karageorghis CI, Bishop DT, Nowicky AV, Wright MJ. Cerebral effects of music during isometric exercise: An fMRI study. Int J Psychophysiol 133: 131–139, 2018.
4. Bigliassi M, Karageorghis CI, Nowicky AV, Orgs G, Wright MJ. Cerebral mechanisms underlying the effects of music during a fatiguing isometric ankle-dorsiflexion task. Psychophysiology 53: 1472–1483, 2016.
5. Bremer N, Peoples G, Hasler B, et al. Repeated incremental workbouts separated by 1 hour increase the electromyographic fatigue threshold. J Strength Cond Res Epub ahead of print.
6. Briscoe MJ, Forgach MS, Trifan E, Malek MH. Validating the EMGFT from a single incremental cycling testing. Int J Sports Med 35: 566–570, 2014.
7. Crust L. Effects of familiar and unfamiliar asynchronous music on treadmill walking endurance. Percept Mot Skills 99: 361–368, 2004.
8. Eason T, Gavel CR, Hawley KA, Galen SS, Malek MH. Reliability of the log-transformed EMG amplitude-power output relationship for incremental knee-extensor ergometry. Muscle Nerve 52: 428–434, 2015.
9. Edworthy J, Waring H. The effects of music tempo and loudness level on treadmill exercise. Ergonomics 49: 1597–1610, 2006.
10. Ferris JR, Tomlinson MA, Ward TN, Pepin ME, Malek MH. Reduced electromyographic fatigue threshold after performing a cognitive fatiguing task. J Strength Cond Res Epub ahead of print.
11. Fontes EB, Okano AH, De Guio F, et al. Brain activity and perceived exertion during cycling exercise: An fMRI study. Br J Sports Med 49: 556–560, 2015.
12. Galen SS, Guffey DR, Coburn JW, Malek MH. Determining the electromyographic fatigue threshold following a single visit exercise test. J Vis Exp Online: 101: e52729, 2015.
13. Galen SS, Malek MH. A single electromyographic testing point is valid to monitor neuromuscular fatigue during continuous exercise. J Strength Cond Res 28: 2754–2759, 2014.
14. Harlan KG, Merucci RB, Weaver JJ, Windle TC, Malek MH. Preexhaustion exercise differentially influences neuromuscular fatigue based on habitual physical activity history. J Strength Cond Res. Epub ahead of print.
15. Hermens HJ, Freriks B, Disselhorst-Klug C, Rau G. Development of recommendations for SEMG sensors and sensor placement procedures. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 10: 361–374, 2000.
16. Hurd MW, Vincent DJ. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): A brief exercise for an undergraduate laboratory course. J Undergrad Neurosci Educ 5: A22–A27, 2006.
17. Hutchinson JC, Sherman T, Davis L, et al. The influence of asynchronous motivational music on a supramaximal exercise bout. Int J Sport Psychol 42: 135–148, 2011.
18. Karageorghis CI, Bigliassi M, Guerin SMR, Delevoye-Turrell Y. Brain mechanisms that underlie music interventions in the exercise domain. Prog Brain Res 240: 109–125, 2018.
19. Karageorghis CI, Priest DL. Music in the exercise domain: A review and synthesis (part I). Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol 5: 44–66, 2012.
20. Karageorghis CI, Priest DL. Music in the exercise domain: A review and synthesis (part II). Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol 5: 67–84, 2012.
21. Karageorghis CI, Terry PC, Lane AM, Bishop DT, Priest DL. The BASES expert statement on use of music in exercise. J Sports Sci 30: 953–956, 2012.
22. Khan FL, Lawal JM, Kapture DO, Swingle JD, Malek MH. Revisiting the single visit protocol for determining the electromyographic fatigue threshold. J Strength Cond Res 31: 3503–3507, 2017.
23. Lopes-Silva JP, Lima-Silva AE, Bertuzzi R, Silva-Cavalcante MD. Influence of music on performance and psychophysiological responses during moderate-intensity exercise preceded by fatigue. Physiol Behav 139: 274–280, 2015.
24. Mahmutović S, Sprout EY, Fontaine JC, et al. Test-retest reliability of the electromyographic fatigue threshold for cycle ergometry. Muscle Nerve 53: 803–807, 2015.
25. Malek MH, Coburn JW, Marelich WD. Advanced Statistics for Kinesiology and Exercise Science: A Practical Guide to ANOVA and Regression Analyses. Milton Park, Abingdon, New York, NY: Routledge, 2018. pp. 30–70.
26. Malek MH, Coburn JW, Tedjasaputra V. Comparison of electromyographic responses for the superficial quadriceps muscles: Cycle versus knee-extensor ergometry. Muscle Nerve 39: 810–818, 2009.
27. Malek MH, Coburn JW, Tedjasaputra V. Comparison of mechanomyographic amplitude and mean power frequency for the rectus femoris muscle: Cycle versus knee-extensor ergometry. J Neurosci Methods 181: 89–94, 2009.
28. Matthews G, Campbell S, Falconer S. Assessment of motivational states in performance environments. Proc Annu Meet Hum Factors Ergon Soc 45: 906–910, 2001.
29. Morse JJ, Pallaska G, Pierce PR, et al. Acute low-dose caffeine supplementation increases electromyographic fatigue threshold in healthy men. J Strength Cond Res 30: 3236–3241, 2016.
30. Noble EB, Pilarski JM, Vora HK, Zuniga JM, Malek MH. Log-transformed emg amplitude-power output relationship: Single-leg knee-extensor versus single-leg cycle ergometry. J Strength Cond Res Epub ahead of print.
31. Richardson RS, Frank LR, Haseler LJ. Dynamic knee-extensor and cycle exercise: Functional MRI of muscular activity. Int J Sports Med 19: 182–187, 1998.
32. Rietschel JC, McDonald CG, Goodman RN, et al. Psychophysiological support of increasing attentional reserve during the development of a motor skill. Biol Psychol 103: 349–356, 2014.
33. Stork MJ, Kwan MY, Gibala MJ, Martin Ginis KA. Music enhances performance and perceived enjoyment of sprint interval exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc 47: 1052–1060, 2015.
34. Terry PC, Karageorghis CI, Saha AM, D'Auria S. Effects of synchronous music on treadmill running among elite triathletes. J Sci Med Sport 15: 52–57, 2012.
35. Terry PC, Lane AM, Fogarty GJ. Construct validity of the POMS-A for use with adults. Psychol Sport Exerc 4: 125–139, 2003.
36. Terry PC, Lane AM, Lane HJ, Keohane L. Development and validation of a mood measure for adolescents. J Sports Sci 17: 861–872, 1999.
37. Travis LA, Arthmire SJ, Baig AM, Goldberg A, Malek MH. Intersession reliability of the EMG signal during incremental cycle ergometry: Quadriceps femoris. Muscle Nerve 44: 937–946, 2011.
Keywords:

sport; physical performance; rehabilitative; quadriceps

© 2019 National Strength and Conditioning Association