Face mask wearing (FMW) has been demonstrated to be an effective practice in populations for limiting the transmission of airborne infectious diseases and has become more common via mask mandates and public health recommendations in many countries, such as the UK, India, and South Korea, since the arrival of the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) in 2019.[1–3] This case series explores this finding and assesses the association of FMW with the occurrence of inferior altitudinal visual field defects in participants with no known history of ocular conditions.
Case Series
Ten cases underwent visual field testing in both eyes using an age-corrected Peripheral 60 Point Suprathreshold Test on a Humphrey Visual Field Analyzer 3 (ZEISS, Oberkochen, Germany). Participants’ age ranged from 22 to 70 years, with a mean (standard deviation [SD]) of 30.2 (17) years. No subjects possessed known histories of ocular disease or existing visual field defects. All participants repeated visual field testing with three categories of facial coverings: no mask, three-ply surgical mask, and duckbill N95 mask [Fig. 1a–c]. All masks were securely positioned on the face of each participant. Sixty targets were presented to each eye for each patient. Errors below the midline of the visual field were recorded and were measured by four metrics. The primary outcome was identified as accuracy errors (unidentified targets), and secondary outcomes were identified as fixation losses (response to previously selected blind spots), false positives (responses to no presentation of targets), and false negatives (failure to respond to a target identified earlier). Accuracy errors were also stratified by distance from the center of the visual field: among the five concentric regions of the visual field visualization, targets were presented in only the third, fourth, and fifth (furthest) rings [Fig. 1d]. Both total and distance-stratified datasets of errors were compared and cross analyzed via two-tailed, paired-subjects t-tests in three groups: no mask versus three-ply surgical mask, no mask versus duckbill N95 mask, and three-ply surgical mask versus duckbill N95 mask (α = 0.05). Patients provided informed consent for the study as well as for the publication of associated images.
Figure 1: Photographs of mask types and example of visual field exam result. (a–c) Three conditions of mask wearing were investigated: no mask, three-ply surgical mask, and duckbill N95 mask. (d) Exam results were described as targets within five concentric regions (marked by numbers) from the center of the visual field. Accuracy errors are marked as squares
The average accuracy error was 6.3% (95% confidence interval [CI]: 3.6–8.9) when not wearing a face mask, 5.2% (95% CI: 3.3–7.1) when wearing a three-ply surgical mask, and 9.5% (95% CI: 6.4–12.6) when wearing a duckbill N95 mask. Under all three conditions, most accuracy errors (88%, 94%, and 90% of all accuracy errors for no mask, surgical mask, and N95, respectively) were located in the fifth and furthest concentric region of the visual field visualization.
Accuracy errors were consistently associated with duckbill N95 mask wearing. When such errors were aggregated across all participants, the probability of producing an error when wearing a duckbill N95 mask was 1.61 times the probability when wearing surgical masks (95% CI: 1.04–2.49, P = 0.01) and 1.57 times the probability when wearing no masks (95% CI: 1.04–2.37, P = 0.08); there was no such relationship of visual field obstruction between wearing surgical masks and wearing no masks [Table 1]. Similarly, there were no statistically significant differences in the secondary outcomes of fixation loss, false-positive error, or false-negative error between any two conditions (P > 0.05) [Table 1]. When stratified by distance from the center of the visual field, the accuracy errors were significantly concentrated in the fifth and furthest ring; when aggregated across all mask types, the probability of the occurrence of errors in this region in total sample data was 13.29 times the probability of errors in the third ring of visual field (95% CI: 5.81–30.43, P < 0.001). The correlation between wearing N95 masks and obstruction of the visual field was preserved only at this most extreme region (surgical vs. N95: odds ratio [OR]: 1.87, 95% CI: 1.16–2.99, P < 0.01; no mask vs. N95: OR: 1.63, 95% CI: 1.03–2.58, P < 0.05) [Table 2].
Table 1: Comparison of occurrence of visual field errors with different mask-wearing practices
Table 2: Accuracy errors stratified by distance from the center of the visual field
Discussion
Due to the persistence of SARS-CoV-2 and the recent increased presence of its Omicron variant, FMW remains a relevant subject of interest and study. Beyond the pandemic, FMW also presents a pragmatic public health strategy for other issues such as protection against other acute respiratory infections and harmful particulate matter in regions with high air pollution.[4] Despite their benefits and popularity, FMW practices have been associated with ocular issues; for example, previous studies have associated widespread mask use with an increased incidence of chalazion in 2020, when compared to previous years.[5] Additionally, FMW has been shown to affect the visual field integrities of six suspected glaucoma patients by inducing artifacts that may be confounded with glaucomatous defects.[6,7] These studies have also placed focus on the usage of surgical masks; however, there is no known research that compares visual field integrity in different types of masks. Additionally, the impact of FMW practices on visual integrity in patients with no risk of glaucoma has not been extensively characterized. To resolve these described gaps, this case series examines such correlations with multiple types of masks in cases with no known eye conditions.
Significant obstructions in inferior altitudinal visual fields were associated with duckbill N95 masks and not three-ply surgical masks. This discrepancy reinforces findings from previous studies, which reported that significant disturbances in visual field integrity from surgical masks were resolved after secure attachment of the masks to the face and nose of the patients.[6,7] Similarly, three-ply surgical masks firmly positioned on the face of each participant did not display evident visual obstruction. Such effects support the importance of proper mask wearing not only for the prevention of pathogen transmission, but also for preventing secondary visual impairment.
Regarding public health guidance, there exists suggestions that face masks can obstruct lower peripheral vision and increase the risk of falls or other accidents.[8] The presence of statistically significant differences in inferior altitudinal visual fields with, particularly, duckbill N95 masks provides evidence for this attribution of hazards to FMW. Notably, visual field integrity was altered with FMW only at the extreme limit of peripheral vision; as a result, daily activities that incorporate this range of vision, like reading, walking on stairs, or even driving, should be approached with caution when wearing duckbill N95 masks.[9,10] Because of a relatively small sample size and skewed distribution of participant ages, the impact of other factors like age and original visual acuity could not be analyzed in this case series. Further research should investigate these relationships and examine the potential differences in inferior visual field defects between proper mask wearing and improper mask wearing. Future results will also be strengthened by the inclusion of a larger sample size.
Conclusion
Our results indicate that significant obstructions in inferior altitudinal visual fields are associated with duckbill N95 masks and not three-ply surgical masks, and thus support the public health guidance that has attributed the risks of falls and accidents to improper face mask wearing.
Declaration of patient consent
The authors certify that they have obtained all appropriate patient consent forms. In the form, the patients have given their consent for their images and other clinical information to be reported in the journal. The patients understand that their names and initials will not be published and due efforts will be made to conceal their identity, but anonymity cannot be guaranteed.
Financial support and sponsorship
Jules Stein Eye Institute, UCLA David Geffen School of Medicine.
Conflicts of interest
There are no conflicts of interest.
Acknowledgements
We acknowledge and thank the support of the Jules Stein Eye Institute for providing the equipment for this study.
References
1. Cheng VC-C, Wong S-C, Chuang VW-M, So SY-C, Chen JH-K, Sridhar S, et al. The role of community-wide wearing of face masks for control of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) epidemic due to SARS-CoV-2 J Infect 2020;81:107–14.
2. Brooks JT, Butler JC. Effectiveness of mask wearing to control community spread of SARS-CoV-2 JAMA 2021;325:998–9.
3. Chughtai AA, Seale H, Macintyre CR. Effectiveness of cloth masks for protection against severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 Emerg Infect Dis 2020;26:e200948. doi: 10.3201/eid2610.200948.
4. Hansstein FV, Echegaray F. Exploring motivations behind pollution-mask use in a sample of young adults in urban China Global Health 2018;14:122.
5. Silkiss RZ, Paap MK, Ugradar S. Increased incidence of chalazion associated with face mask wear during the COVID-19 pandemic Am J Ophthalmol Case Rep 2021;22:101032.
6. El-Nimri NW, Moghimi S, Fingeret M, Weinreb RN.
Visual field artifacts in glaucoma with face mask use during the COVID-19 pandemic J Glaucoma 2020;29:1184–8.
7. Young SL, Smith ML, Tatham AJ.
Visual field artifacts from face mask use J Glaucoma 2020;29:989–91.
8. Kal EC, Young WR, Ellmers TJ. Face masks, vision, and risk of falls BMJ 2020;371:m4133. doi:10.1136/bmj.m4133.
9. Rietdyk S, Rhea CK. The effect of the visual characteristics of obstacles on risk of tripping and gait parameters during locomotion Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2011;31:302–10.
10. Bowers A, Peli E, Elgin J, McGwin G, Owsley C. On-road driving with moderate
visual field loss Optom Vis Sci 2005;82:657–67.