Introduction
‘Healthy discontent is a prelude to progress’. – Mahatma Gandhi
Over time, humankind has employed an array of natural medicines and physical methods to alleviate pain and suffering. Ancient Indian and Chinese texts record the beneficial analgesic effects of cannabis and henbane. In Egypt, around 3000 B.C., the opium poppy, hellebore, beer and the legendary mandrake were used for similar purposes [1]. Other approaches to deal with surgical trauma and pain relied on physical methods such as cold, nerve compression, carotid artery occlusion or infliction of a cerebral concussion. The effectiveness of these historical agents is unknown but they allude to an ever present need.
A new era of anaesthesia arose as one of the advancements of the enlightenment. It began with the isolation of oxygen and the synthesis of nitrous oxide in the 1770s by Joseph Priestley. Initially, nitrous oxide was used solely as an intoxicant. American dentist Horace Wells was the first to recognize the anaesthetizing potential of nitrous oxide; however, his attempt to produce surgical anaesthesia in 1845 with this weak anaesthetic was a miserable failure.
Another compound known as ‘sweet vitriol’ by Paracelsus and renamed ‘aether’ in 1730 by German chemist Frobenius proved to be more potent. It was used for the first successful public demonstration of surgical anaesthesia, conducted by William Thomas Green Morton in Boston in 1846, the year after Wells' failure [1]. Prior to this, Crawford Long, an American physician practising in Georgia had performed surgery under ‘aether’ anaesthesia as early as 1842. In 1847, Scottish physician Sir James Young Simpson succeeded in demonstrating anaesthesia with chloroform, which had been discovered in 1831 by Souberain, Guthrie and Liebig. Chloroform was used by John Snow to oversee the painless delivery of Queen Victoria's eighth child, Leopold, in 1850 [1]. Fast forward to today, a large number of drugs are available to the modern anaesthetist to produce general anaesthesia, from intravenously administered hypnotics to halogenated vapours and gaseous agents such as xenon. The intravenous hypnotics, which provide a smooth induction of anaesthesia, include barbiturates such as sodium pentothal, introduced in 1934 in America by Lundy; etomidate, introduced and studied by Doenicke in 1973; and propofol, which has been widely accepted since its introduction in 1977. The structures of these disparate agents and others are shown in Fig. 1. This review will take a broad look at the mechanisms of action of both inhalational and injectable anaesthetics and will attempt to discriminate where they may overlap and where they may diverge.
Research efforts have pursued a reductionist path to approach the difficult problem of assigning the clinical effect of an anaesthetic to action at a single biological site. This so-called ‘unitary theory’ has animated efforts to produce a single molecular definition of the site of anaesthesic action. Subsequent research has revealed a multitude of targets and produced a more complex picture of how clinically relevant doses of anaesthetics affect molecular targets throughout the central nervous system (CNS). Ironically, in recent years, research groups have discovered that a few discrete sites within the CNS may indeed mediate the action of some general anaesthetics.
What does anaesthesia represent and where does it work?
‘I believe in a long, prolonged, derangement of the senses in order to obtain the unknown’. – Jim Morrison, The Doors
Anaesthesia embraces three controlling, yet reversible characteristics: immobility, amnesia and unconsciousness [2–4]. A more comprehensive definition of anaesthesia could include control of pain (analgesia), muscle relaxation, suppression of reflexes, prevention of nausea and vomiting and even reduction of long-term effects such as postoperative cognitive dysfunction [5]. A wide definition such as this encompasses many areas of clinical concern for anaesthetists and highlights the failure of any single drug to act effectively on all of these components.
Immobility
Immobility is the easiest anaesthetic endpoint to measure. In 1965, Eger et al. [6] introduced the minimal alveolar concentration (MAC) concept, which quantifies the potency of an inhalational anaesthetic. They defined 1.0 MAC as the partial pressure of an inhalational anaesthetic in the lungs, at which 50% of a population of nonrelaxed patients remained immobile at the time of a skin incision [7].
At the physiological level, it is clear that surgical immobility is based on the interaction between a volatile anaesthetic and the spinal cord. Several studies [8–11] in animal models over the last decade provide evidence for the direct effects of anaesthetics on subcortical structures to prevent motor responses to painful stimuli. The measurement of MAC is now widely accepted to be a spinal level phenomenon.
Recently, Eger et al. [12] proposed a new view of MAC. They [13] compiled powerful arguments against the unitary model by summarizing data from their laboratory and others to conclude that only a few of the ion channel targets that have been proposed as putative sites of anaesthetic action remain viable candidates to explain MAC. Instead, they suggest that immobility may result from nonspecific actions within the spinal cord. A major conundrum emerging from these studies is to understand how it is that the behavioural reflex (movement) in response to the nociceptive signalling is completely attenuated, whereas nociceptive neurotransmission to the spinal cord persists at surgical planes of anaesthesia.
Amnesia
The effect on memory arises from several locations in the CNS, including the hippocampus, amygdala, prefrontal cortex as well as regions of the sensory and motor cortices [14]. Much work has gone into understanding how anaesthetics influence one or more of these centres to produce differential effects on type of memory. These studies differentiate explicit memory, that is, specific awareness or consciousness under anaesthesia from implicit memory, the unconscious acquisition of information under adequate levels of anaesthesia. These studies have generally found that the formation of both forms of memory is prevented at low MAC values (0.2–0.4 MAC) [15]. However, there may be a greater possibility of implicit memory at these levels of anaesthesia [16–19]. Prevention of explicit memory (awareness) has spurred the development of monitors such as the bispectral index (BIS), electroencephalogram (EEG) and entropy monitoring or auditory evoked potential recording to recognize inadequate planes of anaesthesia. The importance of implicit memory during anaesthesia remains an unresolved question.
Consciousness
Probably the biggest and most difficult question in neuroscience research involves identifying the physiological processes and anatomical locations responsible for the formation of human consciousness. The ability of anaesthetic drugs to influence this peculiarly human characteristic is even less well understood. Currently, leading neuroscientists studying consciousness acknowledge three regions of the CNS that appear to participate in generating our personal awareness: the cerebral cortex, the thalamus and the reticular activating system [20–24]. These three regions are thought to interact as a cortical system via identified impulse pathways in a distributive way to make us awake, aware and perceiving [25,26].
This conceptualization does not help to narrow down the site of consciousness as a cortical system encompasses the whole cerebral cortex and other closely associated regions such as the thalamus, cerebellum and basal ganglia [25]. Before his death, Francis Crick, with collaborator Christof Koch [27], proposed special significance for the claustrum, a thin layer of neurons with extensive reciprocal connectivity to cerebral cortex, as an important brain structure for consciousness. Few electrophysiological investigations of this brain region have been conducted, and none have focused on determining the effects of anaesthetics.
The neuronal correlates of consciousness (NCC) were also defined by Crick and Koch [25] as the minimal neuronal mechanisms sufficient to support a specific conscious perception. Two competing theories focus on different anatomical units as the basis of NCC. One hypothesis, put forward by Koch, attributes each purposeful experience to a unique activation pattern within a group of neurons [28]. In contrast, Greenfield attributes each such experience to the synchronized activation of neurons over the entire cortex in a sphere of coordinated combination, which is deactivated upon the arrival of a new stimulus [28]. Koch's theory is neuron-specific, whereas Greenfield's is signal-specific. Depending on which, if either, of these theories proves correct, anaesthetics may disrupt consciousness by perturbing CNS function locally in a small cadre of neurons or globally in distantly acting signalling patterns.
Monitoring consciousness
These descriptive observations cannot bring us closer to knowing how anaesthetics induce unconsciousness until a more complete understanding of consciousness is reached. Nevertheless, a gradient of clinical disruption of CNS function by volatile anaesthetics may be described. As anaesthetic is introduced, the three basic elements of the anaesthetized state, amnesia, unconsciousness and immobility, are sequentially impaired. At low concentrations (0.1–0.3 MAC), anaesthetics produce sensory distortions and fragmentation, sleepiness and memory loss as individuals become increasingly more difficult to arouse. Sedation such as this, with low-dose volatile agents, has been found to act primarily on the cortex itself [29]. At MAC-awake, generally around 0.3–0.5 MAC, the response to verbal command is lost in 50% of patients, and they are on the verge of unconsciousness. Blunting consciousness at this stage by some intravenous anaesthetics has been localized to a small subcortical brain stem region called the tuberomammillary nucleus [30]. Finally, immobility to noxious stimulus is achieved at or above 1.0 MAC.
Many anaesthetists have adopted the bispectral index (BIS) to monitor cerebral function and depth of anaesthesia. Awareness under anaesthesia represents a failure of our agents or techniques to sufficiently blunt consciousness. Avidan et al. [31] have recently added more evidence to the growing consensus that this technique does not diminish the incidence of awareness during anaesthesia. BIS may be influenced by the degree of muscle relaxation [32], and although BIS can reflect disruption of the cerebral cortex [33], the choice of anaesthetic itself can influence BIS measurements [34]. In addition, no differences could be detected for key memory centres such as the hippocampus and various neural circuits, which exhibit different anaesthetic sensitivity [35].
Holistic versus specific regional effects of anaesthetics
How then might anaesthetic agents have their effect on consciousness? Do they specifically target Koch's limited groups of neurons; do they exert broad-based effects on specific signalling processes within neural networks [36]? As with our uncertain understanding of the basis of NCC, our ability to identify disruption of neural signalling caused by anaesthetics lacks sufficient resolution. The effects on brain activity of the inhaled anaesthetics halothane and isoflurane, and the intravenous anaesthetic propofol, have been studied by assessing regional glucose metabolism using PET scanning [37–39]. The inhaled agents produced a global decrease in brain glucose metabolism at the point at which consciousness is lost. Propofol produced a slightly different pattern with a significantly lower decrease in glucose metabolism for subcortical regions than in cortex. Fiset et al. [40] were able to confirm the ability of propofol to reduce metabolism in the medial thalamus and other important ‘arousal’ regions.
These findings have been integrated into a hypothesis that anaesthetics interfere with thalamocortical oscillations, which appear to be relevant for consciousness [41] or for corticothalamic reentry [42]. It is known that anaesthetics can act to hyperpolarize thalamocortical neurons and alter them from a tonically active state to a ‘burst-firing’ mode. This change may then convert cortical activity from rapidly oscillating field potentials in the range of γ frequencies to the slower δ and θ oscillations [36,41]. Interestingly, the differential partitioning of anaesthetics into the lipids constituting the grey and white matters of the brain may be an important factor at these sites. White matter has a higher tissue–gas partial pressure than grey matter, and values for the thalamus, hypothalamus and hippocampus lie in between [43].
At the cellular level, the influence of anaesthetics may act either by strengthening inhibition or by diminishing excitation within various parts of the brain. However, it is clear that not all neurons that may be involved in the formation of consciousness are constrained under the influence of anaesthetics [36]. This is the same problem identified by Eger et al. [12] in that neural transmission from the periphery to central pain centres still persists even at supra-MAC levels of anaesthesia. Nevertheless, our current state of understanding supports the following framework: sensory stimuli conducted through the reticular formation of the brain stem into supratentorial signalling loops, connecting the thalamus with various regions of the cortex, are the foundation of consciousness, and that these neural pathways involved in the development of consciousness are disturbed by anaesthetics [44].
The lipid bilayer as the site: the Meyer–Overton correlation
‘No diet will remove all the fat from your body because the brain is entirely fat. Without a brain, you might look good, but all you could do is run for public office’. – George Bernard Shaw
At the turn of the 19th century, work carried out by botanist Overton [45,46] in Zurich and pharmacologist Meyer [47] in Marburg independently identified a strong correlation between the potency of an anaesthetic and its solubility in olive oil (Fig. 2[48]). This meaningful correlation, which held true for over a 1000-fold range of anaesthetic solubilities, suggested a common unitary mechanism. Their recognition of the importance of lipid solubility was the first attempt to explain the functional mechanism of anaesthetics.
Since the recognition of the Meyer–Overton correlation, several lipid-based theories evolved over the last century to explain the anaesthetic phenomenon. These ideas included anaesthetic-induced volume expansion of the cell membrane [49], increased fluidity of the cell membrane [50] and increased lateral surface pressure [51,52]. Another impetus for these hypotheses stemmed from the dramatic findings that animals under conditions of increased hydrostatic or barometric pressure were resistant to the effects of volatile anaesthetics [53,54]. These observations, termed pressure reversal of anaesthesia, led to the further development of theories focused on the disruption of membrane structure by increased membrane fluidity as an explanation for the action of anaesthetics [55–59]. These theories were attractive because they relied on purely thermodynamic considerations and could account for the wide range of chemical structures represented by anaesthetic molecules. However, these ideas conflicted with other whole organism data showing that elevated body temperature, which disrupts membrane fluidity as much as anaesthetics, actually increases MAC, not reducing it as these hypotheses would predict [60].
Eclipse of Meyer–Overton correlation
Over the past 20 years, additional doubts have arisen about the focus on the lipid membrane as the site of anaesthetic action that arose from the Meyer–Overton correlation.
Luciferase
Fireflies employ the soluble enzyme luciferase to generate flashes of light. When the enzyme was purified lipid-free and studied in the presence of various anaesthetics, its bioluminescent function was impaired according to the potency predicted by the Meyer–Overton correlation [61]. These data demonstrated that proteins might also be affected by anaesthetics according to the Meyer–Overton correlation.
Nonimmobilizers
Eger and colleagues [62,63] identified a large number of compounds whose structure and lipid solubility would suggest general anaesthetic ability but when tested on rats at concentrations predicted by the Meyer–Overton correlation did not induce anaesthesia or induced it at higher than expected concentrations. Thus, these compounds fell off the Meyer–Overton correlation line and were labelled nonanaesthetics. More precisely, these compounds (e.g. dichlorohexafluorocyclobutane) have come to be termed nonimmobilizers because they do not prevent movement in response to noxious stimuli in the rodent MAC assay but are able to inhibit the higher CNS functions of learning and memory at concentrations near the predicted Meyer–Overton effective concentration [64,65]. They have, thus, come to be important control compounds for studies seeking to identify a specific mechanistic target of anaesthetics. As such, a legitimate site of action of anaesthetics for MAC ought to be unaffected by nonimmobilizer compounds.
Cutoff effect
The ‘cutoff’ effect describes a relationship between molecule size and anaesthetic potency. Within a series of chemically homologous anaesthetics such as the n-alkanes, potency increases with increasing molecular size, as predicted by the Meyer–Overton correlation, until a critical molecular dimension is reached (cutoff). Above this size, members of the series lose their anaesthetic effect despite increasing lipid solubility [66]. These results can be interpreted to mean that the site of action for anaesthetics represents a cavity or binding pocket of restricted size that constrains the effective size of drugs that induce anaesthesia. The fact that lipophilic drugs above this cutoff size fail to induce anaesthesia demonstrates another violation of the Meyer–Overton correlation.
Stereospecificity
A stereoselective difference in potency of anaesthetics has been shown for intravenous and inhalational anaesthetics. With stereoisomers of isoflurane, Lysko et al. [67] found that the (S+) enantiomer was 50% more potent than the (R−) enantiomer in rats using the standard MAC assay. This result was confirmed by Dickinson et al. [68], who determined a 40% increased potency for loss of righting reflex for (S+) over (R−) isoflurane when injected intravenously. Because stereoselectivity is presumed to arise from the interaction of the drug with an optically active site on the protein, these studies provide evidence for a specific molecular interaction rather than a more generalized physicochemical effect of the anaesthetics. The criterion of stereoselective potency differences has, thus, become an important discriminator for a legitimate site of anaesthetic action.
The studies described above led the anaesthesia mechanism research community away from purely lipid-based explanations for the action of anaesthetics. The new focus came to rest on protein components of the membrane as potential targets for general anaesthetics. The realization that proteins also contain lipophilic domains supports the continuing importance of the Meyer–Overton correlation [69]. Although it remained a formal possibility that anaesthetics interact with a single-protein site, the sudden increased number of protein candidates has come to make the unitary hypothesis of anaesthesia less and less tenable [70,71].
Protein targets for volatile anaesthetics
‘The great thing in the world is not so much where we stand, as in what direction we are moving’. – Oliver Wendell Holmes, American jurist
The studies by Franks and Lieb [61] with luciferase stimulated interest in a whole new repertoire of protein targets for anaesthetics. At that time, improvements in the electrophysiological analysis of ion channel activity and in molecular cloning techniques were enabling researchers to study the molecular pharmacology of anaesthetic drugs on pure populations of membrane receptors and ion channels. Other research groups demonstrated that soluble cytoplasmic proteins, such as protein kinase C, an important signal transduction enzyme involved in regulating the release of neurotransmitters and ion channel activity [72,73], and mitochondrial proteins are also affected by anaesthetics and should not be overlooked [74]. Even structural elements in support cells of the CNS such as gap junctions of astrocytes, which could influence communication between astrocytes, were found to be susceptible to the effects of anaesthetics [75]. The problem for investigators shifted from discovering the key physicochemical effect of anaesthetics on a homogeneous lipid membrane to the new burden of too many possible protein targets.
Diversity of possible functional sites
We now know that the human genome contains the coding sequence for between 20 000 and 21 000 proteins [76]. In order to sort through the abundance of potential clinically relevant functional sites for anaesthetics, the following criteria have been put forward [77]:
- anaesthetics must produce a reversible effect at a functional site with clinically relevant concentrations;
- a functional site must be situated at a plausible anatomical location to mediate the specific behavioural effects of an anaesthetic;
- stereoselectivity of anaesthetic effects in vivo should duplicate the stereoselective effects observed in vitro;
- a functional site should be insensitive to the effects of nonimmobilizers.
These requirements are met by several different receptors and ion channel families, which now constitute the leading molecular candidates for mediating the actions of anaesthetics (Tables 1 and 2) [12,78]. Before considering specific proteins, we start with an exploration of some theoretical questions about the effect of anaesthetics on them.
Table 1: Effects of various anaesthetics on ion channel/receptor targets
Table 2: Neurophysiological roles for putative protein targets of anaesthetics
Specific or nonspecific interactions with proteins
By virtue of their complex structure, proteins as a possible site of anaesthetic action are presumed to contain a specific binding site within their folded structure to mediate a functional interaction between protein and anaesthetic. But how can the variety of chemical structures (Fig. 1) capable of causing anaesthesia interact with such functional specificity? The best evidence at present is that anaesthetic molecules exert their effects on proteins by occupying gaps or pockets.
Proteins are known to harbour packing defects or cavities that may be critical for protein function [79]. Binding cavities for anaesthetics in functionally relevant proteins have been identified, and occupation of these domains by anaesthetic molecules can stabilize the protein in its native, folded state [80]. These interactions may occur with low affinity, consistent with the fact that clinical concentrations of general anaesthetics fall into the tens to hundreds micromolar range. So these postulated sites have a dual nature. They are specific in that they may exert an allosteric effect on the function of the protein but are nonetheless unspecific in that they can accommodate a variety of molecular profiles within the folded structure of the protein [81]. Entry to these sequestered areas of a complex, folded protein structure will depend on a molecule's ability to penetrate hydrophobic amino acid sequences, thus abiding by constraints of the Meyer–Overton correlation.
Presynaptic versus postsynaptic interaction
Anaesthetics may affect neurons at various functional levels, but the primary focus has been on the synapse. A presynaptic action may alter the release of neurotransmitters [82,83], whereas a postsynaptic effect may change the frequency or amplitude of impulses transversing the synapse [73,84,85]. From a functional standpoint, the action of anaesthetics may arise from changing the balance between inhibition and excitation of neural transmission within the CNS. Studies on isolated spinal cord tissue have demonstrated that excitatory transmission is impaired by anaesthetics much more strongly than inhibitory transmissions are potentiated to contribute to a mechanism of immobility [84,86]. To add to the complexity, the change in this balance could occur on the afferent side, sensing the intensity of the nociceptive stimulus, or on the efferent side, controlling movement [87]. Moreover, these two arms of the functional CNS may have varying sensitivity. Effects on sodium or potassium channels, which have a predominant role in axonal conduction as well as for determining the resting membrane potential, could mediate a similar effect [85,88].
Thus, chloride channels [gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)A and glycine receptors] and potassium channels (K2P, possibly Kv and KATP channels) remain the primary inhibitory ion channels that fulfil the criteria for a legitimate anaesthetic action site [89–94]. On the excitatory side, anaesthetic-induced inhibition of ion channels activated by acetylcholine (nicotinic and muscarinic receptors) [95,96], excitatory amino acids [amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazol-propionic acid (AMPA), kainate and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors] [97,98] and serotonin (5-HT2 and 5-HT3 receptors) are currently held as possible ion channel targets of anaesthetic action [88]. Below, we consider in-vitro and in-vivo data on these channels.
Anaesthetic effects on inhibitory ion channels
GABAA and glycine receptors
Clinical concentrations of volatile anaesthetics are able to activate GABAA and glycine receptors expressed in vitro using heterologous expression systems as well as in vivo on the postsynaptic membrane [99,100]. The enhanced GABA-activated chloride current causes hyperpolarization of the neuronal membrane to reduce neuronal activity (Fig. 3a). In contrast, the gaseous anaesthetics xenon, nitrous oxide and cyclopropane as well as the intravenous agent ketamine have minimal or no effect on GABAA receptor subtypes [98,101–104]. Another population of GABAA receptors, the so-called extrajunctional GABAA receptors, can be activated by very low concentrations of GABA and are distinguished structurally from synaptic GABAA receptors. Currents passed by these receptors are potentiated by low concentrations of volatile anaesthetics and are widely expressed in important brain regions such as the hippocampus, thalamus, cortex and cerebellum [105]. The relevance of extrajunctional GABAA receptors in anaesthesia mechanisms is an open question.
The intravenous anaesthetics propofol and etomidate also enhance GABAA receptor function to produce immobility [106–108], and some molecular interaction sites have been elucidated. Rudolph et al. [109] discovered a point mutation on the mouse α1 GABAA subunit that moderated whole animal benzodiazepine-induced behavioural patterns. Barbiturates act on GABAA receptors depending on their concentration at the receptor by means of positive allosteric modulation, direct activation and inhibition. Drafts and Fisher [110] identified a mutation in the GABAA α subunit that abolishes the action of barbiturates; however, activation by GABA and potentiation by etomidate were not affected. Alcohol also enhances GABAA-mediated transmissions, which may play a role in mediating its intoxicating effects [111].
With the help of chimeric channel constructs, Mihic et al. [89] identified a domain, 45 amino acids in length, relevant for mediating the effect of volatile anaesthetics and etomidate [112], but not propofol. Further mutation of GABAA receptor subunits revealed two key amino acids involved in the interaction with volatile anaesthetics that are closely placed in the primary sequence. Hence, these residues may contribute to a binding pocket for the anaesthetic [113].
The role of inhibitory GABAA and glycine receptors has been well studied at the spinal level. To suppress a motor response, as determined by measurement of MAC, spinal injections of glycine receptor antagonists appear to have greater effect on MAC than GABAA receptor antagonists [88]. Possible spinal functional sites for inhalational anaesthetics include both glycine and glutamate receptors [12,114].
Potassium channels
Franks and Lieb [115] identified an isoflurane-activated potassium current in specific neurons of the freshwater snail Lymnaea stagnalis. This current had the characteristics of a leak or background K channel because it lacked voltage-dependent activation, was noninactivating and passed currents closely predicted by the Goldman–Hodgkin–Katz equation for ion conduction through a passive, K-selective pore [116]. Subsequently, a unique family of K subunits with two pore-lining sequences (K2P channels) was discovered that had a wide phylogenetic range and was activated by volatile anaesthetics at clinically relevant concentrations [92,117,118]. Activation of these background K channels in response to volatile anaesthetics results in hyperpolarization and silencing of neuronal activity (Fig. 3) [115,119]. Members of the family can also be activated by xenon [120] and nitrous oxide [121], and differentially activated by isoflurane stereoisomers [122].
Evidence from K2P knockout mice has further implicated these channels in the mechanism of action of volatile anaesthetics. TWIK-related K+ channel-1 (TREK-1) knockout mice were resistant to the effects of six volatile anaesthetics as determined by the standard MAC assay [123]. Knockout mice in which other members of the K2P channel family have been inactivated [TWIK-related acid-sensitive K+ channel-1 (TASK-1) and TASK-3] also show some resistance to the anaesthetizing action of volatile anaesthetics. Additional studies with multiple K2P knockouts will be needed to understand their full importance.
Other potential K channel targets include voltage-gated K channels (Kv) and ATP-activated K channels (KATP). Kv channels were first isolated from mutant Drosophila that displayed an abnormal ‘shaking’ reaction upon exposure to ether [124]. The shaker phenotype arose from inactivation of a voltage-gated K channel gene, but in-vitro studies [125,126] of the effects of anaesthetics on this channel family have found them to be inhibited at supraclinical anaesthetic concentrations. Likewise, administration of KATP channel-blocking drugs (pinacidil and cromakalim) into the neuroaxis did not change isoflurane MAC [127]. Thus, the primary focus of the anaesthetic mechanism involving K channels remains on background K channels.
Effect of anaesthetics on excitatory ion channels and neurotranmission
Glutamatergic ion channels
Figure 4 depicts a model of an excitatory synapse. Glutamatergic neurotransmission can occur via activation of three distinct families of ligand-gated ion channels: AMPA, kainate or NMDA receptors. The gaseous anaesthetics, xenon, nitrous oxide and cyclopropane, as well as the intravenous agent ketamine have been shown to suppress excitatory, glutamate-mediated synaptic transmission by blocking NMDA receptors on the postsynaptic membrane [98,101–104]. In addition, enflurane and urethane inhibited NMDA-stimulated excitatory flow in NMDA-expressing Xenopus oocytes [128,129]. Blockade of AMPA receptors can decrease MAC by 60% [130], and inhaled anaesthetics potentiate currents through kainate receptors containing a GluR6 subunit [131]. Hollmann et al. [132] were able to demonstrate a reversible dose-dependent inhibition of recombinant NMDA receptors by isoflurane, sevoflurane and desflurane. These in-vitro findings support a postsynaptic role of glutamate receptors in anaesthetic action. Volatile anaesthetics may also suppress the excitatory glutamatergic transmission via presynaptic inhibition of glutamate release [133]. However, studies [134–136] with knockout mice have up to now failed to find a significant role in vivo.
The molecular site of action for xenon and isoflurane to inhibit NMDA receptors occurs by binding to the glycine coagonist site [137]. This finding may lead to the design of new anaesthetics, as some clinically well tolerated neuroprotective compounds are known to bind to this site as well.
Neuronal acetylcholine receptors
The primary role of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the CNS appears to be the modulation of synaptic conduction [138]. Many volatile anaesthetics inhibit acetylcholine receptors at clinical concentrations, but administration of nicotinic or muscarinic antagonists within the neuroaxis does not change anaesthetic potency [139,140]. Rather, these receptors appear to be more involved in modulating nociception than in mediating anaesthetic-induced immobility [141,142].
Adenosine receptors
Administration of adenosine or adenosine agonists can decrease MAC in vivo (rat and dog models) [143,144]. However, administration of a subtype-selective adenosine antagonist does not change halothane MAC, arguing against a significant role in inhaled anaesthetic mechanisms.
Serotonin receptors
The neurotransmitter serotonin causes neuronal signalling by activating a ligand-gated channel (5-HT3 subtype) as well as G-protein-coupled receptor subtypes (5-HT2 and others) [145]. Inhaled anaesthetics block activation of 5-HT2A receptors and reduce nociception. However, the nonimmobilizer 1,2-dichlorohexafluorocyclobutane causes the same degree of inhibition of 5-HT2A receptors as halothane at a 1 MAC equivalency [146]. Blockade of 5-HT3 receptors by ondansetron does not alter MAC of isoflurane [147]. Thus, there is minimal evidence that serotonin receptors are mediators of anaesthetic action.
From neuroprotection to neurotoxicity: a spectrum of anaesthetic side effects
‘It takes your enemy and your friend, working together to hurt you to the heart; the one to slander you and the other to get the news to you’. – Mark Twain
The effect of anaesthetics is not limited to the desired short-term action in the operating room to allow surgery on patients. The ability of anaesthetics to produce immobility, amnesia and unconsciousness seem completely and quickly reversible. However, in addition to beneficial effects such as neuroprotection and cardioprotection, increasing concern over long-term detrimental effects such as neurotoxicity, neurodegeneration and postoperative cognitive dysfunction has recently arisen.
Neuroprotection
Much effort has gone into understanding the neuroprotective ability of anaesthetics in order to best use this advantageous aspect during neurosurgical anaesthesia. It is well established that inhalational agents, such as isoflurane, and intravenous anaesthetics (propofol and barbiturates) provide protection from focal ischaemic and metabolic insults as confirmed by both in-vitro and in-vivo experimental models [148]. The exact mechanism of neuroprotection remains unclear, but its effects can remain for an extended period of weeks to months [149]. In-vitro studies [150,151] using rat hippocampal slice cultures verified the involvement of inhibitory GABAA and excitatory NMDA receptors in the global CNS protection afforded by volatile anaesthetics. However, clinical trials using various blockers of NMDA receptors have failed to demonstrate benefit for global insults. A similar protective effect against cerebral ischaemia was found for the selective α2-adrenoceptor agonist dexmedetomidine, which may involve the expression of the active focal adhesion kinase, a tyrosine kinase important for the adaptability and longevity of cells [152].
The molecular mechanism of volatile anaesthetic neuroprotection may also involve anaesthetic activation of K2P channels. Overexpression of the acid-sensitive K2P channel TASK-3 in neurons of rat hippocampal slices protected them from a hypoxic and glucose deprivation injury [153]. Members of the TREK subfamily [TREK-1 and TWIK-related arachidonic acid-stimulated K+ channel (TRAAK)] are activated by the neuroprotective substances riluzole, polyunsaturated fatty acids and lysophospholipids and may contribute to neuroprotection by hyperpolarizing neurons during ischaemic insult [121]. Heurteaux et al. [123] found that TREK-1 knockout mice were more vulnerable to kainic acid and pentylenetetrazol to induce seizures and spinal cord ischaemia than wild-type mice. The molecular sites of action for both effects remain unknown. However, the involved channels overlap significantly with those that are amongst leading candidates for mediating the anaesthetizing effects of anaesthetics.
Neurotoxicity
Persistent detrimental effects of anaesthetics on CNS activity have also been identified recently. Postoperative cognitive dysfunction can be detected in approximately 25% of patients after 1 week and in almost 10% of patients at 3 months following prolonged exposure to general anaesthetics. Advanced age, low education level, preoperative depression and the presence of postoperative pain, infection or respiratory complications contribute additional risk factors [154,155]. Additionally, animal studies have found decreased memory only in aged rats after anaesthesia with isoflurane [156] or nitrous oxide [157]. However, the interaction between anaesthetics and the process linked to the progression of Alzheimer's disease has not been established. Furthermore, two meta-analyses [155,158] comparing rates of postoperative cognitive dysfunction in elderly patients undergoing general versus regional anaesthesia have failed to demonstrate a decreased incidence of cognitive dysfunction when administration of general anaesthesia was avoided.
In addition to the functional disruption of mental process, neurodegenerative changes can be detected in the brain of young organisms following exposure to anaesthetic drugs. A seminal observation was made by Jevtovic-Todorovic et al. [159], who discovered that anaesthetic exposure to rodents within a particularly vulnerable developmental period overlapping the first postnatal week caused dramatic neurodegeneration within the hippocampus, a brain structure critical for memory. A variety of anaesthetics, including isoflurane, ketamine, benzodiazepines and propofol, have been found to cause neural injury. On the basis of this profile of damaging agents, it appears that both blockade of excitatory as well as potentiation of inhibitory (chloride) ion channels can induce injury to young neurons. The common denominator is depolarization of neonatal neurons. This apparent contradiction can be understood with the realization that the chloride gradient is reversed in young neurons compared with adult neurons, so that the enhancement of GABAergic transmission causes chloride to leave the cell resulting in depolarization. Neurotoxic effects appear to follow this electrophysiologic effect.
The primate brain has also been shown to be susceptible to these toxic effects. The most recent data have been a retrospective examination of children exposed to anaesthetics, in which two or more exposures within the first 2 years of life was strongly associated with learning disabilities later in life [160]. The study of this alarming aspect of anaesthetic action is in the early stages but is being pursed with vigour because of its important implications.
Future research directions
‘A problem well stated is a problem half solved’. – Charles F. Kettering, inventor
Through the persistent efforts of a number of research groups around the world, the understanding of how anaesthetics work has advanced significantly in the past 25 years. Stricter criteria of what constitutes a legitimate site of action have been described and generally accepted. As a result targets that were once accepted as possible sites have been ruled out, and new potential candidates will have to fulfil these criteria to continue to demand research attention. However, an implicit assumption is that this definition of anaesthesia would be valid for all anaesthetics, but would simultaneously allow for the differentiation between various modes of action and effects [4].
The introduction of whole animal models, in which the receptors and channels that anaesthetics affect can be altered or eliminated, has produced additional insight. Targeted mutations in animals offer an elegant approach to compare in-vitro observations with in-vivo behaviour. Research findings from organisms such as Caenorhabditis elegans[161] and Drosophila[162] are being complemented today by studies in knockout and transgenic mice. These results emphasize the power of using animal models in anaesthesia mechanisms research and how they will continue to allow major experimental advancements in the future.
Finally, at the molecular level, additional attention will be directed at the interplay between proteins and the lipid bilayer. For example, halothane molecules appear to group particularly in the upper regions of fatty acid chains and alternatively below the carbonyl group of the lipid. Thus, halothane induces a lateral expansion accompanied by a contraction of the lipid bilayer with consecutive changes in the electrical properties of the membrane [163]. At the same time, it has been shown that halothane integrates with GABA receptors as well, where it causes increased inhibitory transmission [164].
Discovery of a single fundamental mechanism, which seeks to explain the whole of anaesthesia described above, has become less and less likely in recent years. The state of anaesthesia is more likely to be based on many different effects and on multiple molecular biological targets. These integrative thoughts sum up the ‘multiple target hypothesis’. In this sense, different anaesthetics produce various effects at different functional sites. These paradigms can be confirmed by computer models, which in the future may be useful in the design of new anaesthetics [165].
Acknowledgements
Research work of Charles S. Yost is supported by US National Institutes of Health grant #GM58149. The authors thank Mrs Allison Dwileski for illustrating Figures 3 and 4 and for editing the manuscript.
References
1 Brandt L. Illustrated history of anaesthesia. Stuttgart, Germany: Wissenschaftliche Verlagsgesellschaft mbH; 1997.
2 Eger EI. What is general anesthetic action? Anesth Analg 1993; 77:408–409.
3 Antognini JF, Carstens E. In vivo characterization of clinical anaesthesia and its components. Br J Anaesth 2002; 89:156–166.
4 Eger EI, Sonner JM. Anaesthesia defined (gentlemen, this is no humbug). Best Pract Res Clin Anaesthesiol 2006; 20:23–29.
5 Urban BW, Bleckwenn M. Concepts and correlations relevant to general anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 2002; 89:3–16.
6 Eger EI, Saidman LJ, Brandstater B. Minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration: a standard of anesthetic potency. Anesthesiology 1965; 26:756–763.
7 De Jong RH, Eger EI. MAC expanded: AD50 and AD95 values of common inhalation anesthetics in man. Anesthesiology 1975; 42:384–389.
8 Antognini JF, Schwartz K. Exaggerated anesthetic requirements in the preferentially anesthetized brain. Anesthesiology 1993; 79:1244–1249.
9 Rampil IJ, Mason P, Singh H. Anesthetic potency (MAC) is independent of forebrain structures in the rat. Anesthesiology 1993; 78:707–712.
10 King BS, Rampil IJ. Anesthetic depression of spinal motor neurons may contribute to lack of movement in response to noxious stimuli. Anesthesiology 1994; 81:1484–1492.
11 Rampil IJ. Anesthetic potency is not altered after hypothermic spinal cord transection in rats. Anesthesiology 1994; 80:606–610.
12 Eger EI, Raines DE, Shafer SL,
et al. Is a new paradigm needed to explain how inhaled anesthetics produce immobility? Anesth Analg 2008; 107:832–848.
13 Eger EI, Tang M, Liao M,
et al. Inhaled anesthetics do not combine to produce synergistic effects regarding minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration in rats. Anesth Analg 2008; 107:479–485.
14 Kandel ER. The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue between genes and synapses. Science 2001; 294:1030–1038.
15 Alkire MT, Gruver R, Miller J,
et al. Neuroimaging analysis of an anesthetic gas that blocks human emotional memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2008; 105:1722–1727.
16 Bennett HL, Davis HS, Giannini JA. Nonverbal response to intraoperative conversation. Br J Anaesth 1985; 57:174–179.
17 Block RI, Ghoneim MM, Sum Ping ST, Ali MA. Human learning during general anaesthesia and surgery. Br J Anaesth 1991; 66:170–178.
18 Ghoneim MM, Block RI. Learning and consciousness during general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1992; 76:279–305.
19 Iselin-Chaves IA, Willems SJ, Jermann FC,
et al. Investigation of implicit memory during isoflurane anesthesia for elective surgery using the process dissociation procedure. Anesthesiology 2005; 103:925–933.
20 Angel A, LeBeau F. A comparison of the effects of propofol with other anaesthetic agents on the centripetal transmission of sensory information. Gen Pharmacol 1992; 23:945–963.
21 Berg-Johnsen J, Langmoen IA. Isoflurane hyperpolarizes neurones in rat and human cerebral cortex. Acta Physiol Scand 1987; 130:679–685.
22 Moruzzi G, Magoun HW. Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995; 7:251–267.
23 Shimoji K, Fujioka H, Fukazawa T,
et al. Anesthetics and excitatory/inhibitory responses of midbrain reticular neurons. Anesthesiology 1984; 61:151–155.
24 Steriade M. Arousal: revisiting the reticular activating system. Science 1996; 272:225–226.
25 Crick F, Koch C. A framework for consciousness. Nat Neurosci 2003; 6:119–126.
26 Tononi G, Koch C. The neural correlates of consciousness: an update. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008; 1124:239–261.
27 Crick FC, Koch C. What is the function of the claustrum? Phil Trans R Soc B 2005; 360:1271–1279.
28 Koch C, Greenfield S. How does consciousness happen? Sci Am 2007; 297:76–83.
29 Hentschke H, Schwarz C, Antkowiak B. Neocortex is the major target of sedative concentrations of volatile anaesthetics: strong depression of firing rates and increase of GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition. Eur J Neurosci 2005; 21:93–102.
30 Nelson LE, Guo TZ, Lu J,
et al. The sedative component of anesthesia is mediated by GABA(A) receptors in an endogenous sleep pathway. Nat Neurosci 2002; 5:979–984.
31 Avidan MS, Zhang L, Burnside BA,
et al. Anesthesia awareness and the bispectral index. N Engl J Med 2008; 358:1097–1108.
32 Hans P, Giwer J, Brichant JF,
et al. Effect of an intubation dose of rocuronium on spectral entropy and Bispectral Index responses to laryngoscopy during propofol anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 2006; 97:842–847.
33 Dahaba AA. Different conditions that could result in the bispectral index indicating an incorrect hypnotic state. Anesth Analg 2005; 101:765–773.
34 Schwab HS, Seeberger MD, Eger EI,
et al. Sevoflurane decreases bispectral index values more than does halothane at equal MAC multiples. Anesth Analg 2004; 99:1723–1727.
35 Orser BA. Depth-of-anesthesia monitor and the frequency of intraoperative awareness. N Engl J Med 2008; 358:1189–1191.
36 Arhem P, Klement G, Nilsson J. Mechanisms of anesthesia: towards integrating network, cellular, and molecular level modeling. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003; 28(Suppl 1):S40–S47.
37 Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Shah NK, Anderson CT. Positron emission tomography study of regional cerebral metabolism in humans during isoflurane anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1997; 86:549–557.
38 Alkire MT, Pomfrett CJ, Haier RJ,
et al. Functional brain imaging during anesthesia in humans: effects of halothane on global and regional cerebral glucose metabolism. Anesthesiology 1999; 90:701–709.
39 Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Barker SJ,
et al. Cerebral metabolism during propofol anesthesia in humans studied with positron emission tomography. Anesthesiology 1995; 82:393–403.
40 Fiset P, Paus T, Daloze T,
et al. Brain mechanisms of propofol-induced loss of consciousness in humans: a positron emission tomographic study. J Neurosci 1999; 19:5506–5513.
41 Alkire MT, Haier RJ, Fallon JH. Toward a unified theory of narcosis: brain imaging evidence for a thalamocortical switch as the neurophysiologic basis of anesthetic-induced unconsciousness. Conscious Cogn 2000; 9:370–386.
42 Alkire MT, McReynolds JR, Hahn EL, Trivedi AN. Thalamic microinjection of nicotine reverses sevoflurane-induced loss of righting reflex in the rat. Anesthesiology 2007; 107:264–272.
43 Neumann MA, Eger EI, Weiskopf RB. Solubility of volatile anesthetics in bovine white matter, cortical gray matter, thalamus, hippocampus, and hypothalamic area. Anesth Analg 2005; 100:1003–1006.
44 Schneider G, Kochs EF. The search for structures and mechanisms controlling anesthesia-induced unconsciousness. Anesthesiology 2007; 107:195–198.
45 Overton E. About the general osmotic properties of the cell, their presumable causes, and their importance for physiology. Vierteljahresschr Naturforsch Ges Zürich 1899; 44:88–135.
46 Overton E. Studies of narcosis. A contribution to general pharmacology. Jena, Germany: Gustav Fischer; 1901.
47 Meyer HH. Which property of anaesthetics causes their narcotic effect? Arch Exp Pathol Pharmakol (Naunyn-Schmiedebergs) 1899; 42:109–118.
48 Campagna JA, Miller JW, Forman SA. Mechanisms of actions of inhaled anesthetics. N Engl J Med 2003; 348:2110–2124.
49 Mori T, Matubayasi N, Ueda I. Membrane expansion and inhalation anesthetics. Mean excess volume hypothesis. Mol Pharmacol 1984; 25:123–130.
50 Ueda I, Hirakawa M, Arakawa K, Kamaya H. Do anesthetics fluidize membranes? Anesthesiology 1986; 64:67–71.
51 Cantor RS. The lateral pressure profile in membranes: a physical mechanism of general anesthesia. Biochemistry 1997; 36:2339–2344.
52 Janoff AS, Miller KW. A critical assessment of the lipid theories of general anesthetic action. In: Chapman D, editor. Biological membranes. London, UK: Academic Press; 1982.
53 Halsey MJ, Wardley-Smith B. Pressure reversal of narcosis produced by anaesthetics, narcotics and tranquillisers. Nature 1975; 257:811–813.
54 Bailey CP, Green CJ, Halsey MJ, Wardley-Smith B. High pressure and intravenous steroid anesthesia in rats. J Appl Physiol 1977; 43:183–188.
55 Trudell JR. A unitary theory of anesthesia based on lateral phase separations in nerve membranes. Biophys J 1977; 18:358–359.
56 Rosenberg PH, Jansson SE, Gripenberg J. Effects of halothane, thiopental, and lidocaine on fluidity of synaptic plasma membranes and artificial phospholipid membranes. Anesthesiology 1977; 46:322–326.
57 Ueda I, Shieh DD, Eyring H. Anesthetic interaction with a model cell membrane: expansion, phase transition, and melting of the lecithin monolayer. Anesthesiology 1974; 41:217–225.
58 Trudell JR, Hubbell WL, Cohen EN. The effect of two inhalation anesthetics on the order of spin-labeled phospholipid vesicles. Biochim Biophys Acta 1973; 291:321–327.
59 Miller KW, Pang KY. General anaesthetics can selectively perturb lipid bilayer membranes. Nature 1976; 263:253–255.
60 Eger EI, Saidman LJ, Brandstater B. Temperature dependence of halothane and cyclopropane anesthesia in dogs: correlation with some theories of anesthetic action. Anesthesiology 1965; 26:764–770.
61 Franks NP, Lieb WR. Do general anaesthetics act by competitive binding to specific receptors? Nature 1984; 310:599–601.
62 Koblin DD, Chortkoff BS, Laster MJ,
et al. Polyhalogenated and perfluorinated compounds that disobey the Meyer-Overton hypothesis. Anesth Analg 1994; 79:1043–1048.
63 Fang Z, Sonner J, Laster MJ,
et al. Anesthetic and convulsant properties of aromatic compounds and cycloalkanes: implications for mechanisms of narcosis. Anesth Analg 1996; 83:1097–1104.
64 Kandel L, Chortkoff BS, Sonner J,
et al. Nonanesthetics can suppress learning. Anesth Analg 1996; 82:321–326.
65 Sonner JM, Li J, Eger EI. Desflurane and the nonimmobilizer 1,2-dichlorohexafluorocyclobutane suppress learning by a mechanism independent of the level of unconditioned stimulation. Anesth Analg 1998; 87:200–205.
66 Raines DE, Miller KW. On the importance of volatile agents devoid of anesthetic action. Anesth Analg 1994; 79:1031–1033.
67 Lysko GS, Robinson JL, Casto R, Ferrone RA. The stereospecific effects of isoflurane isomers in vivo. Eur J Pharmacol 1994; 263:25–29.
68 Dickinson R, White I, Lieb WR, Franks NP. Stereoselective loss of righting reflex in rats by isoflurane. Anesthesiology 2000; 93:837–843.
69 Franks NP, Lieb WR. Molecular and cellular mechanisms of general anaesthesia. Nature 1994; 367:607–614.
70 Eckenhoff RG, Johansson JS. On the relevance of ‘clinically relevant concentrations’ of inhaled anesthetics in in vitro experiments. Anesthesiology 1999; 91:856–860.
71 Harrison NL. Ion channels take center stage: twin spotlights on two anesthetic targets. Anesthesiology 2000; 92:936–938.
72 Hemmings HC, Adamo AI. Effects of halothane and propofol on purified brain protein kinase C activation. Anesthesiology 1994; 81:147–155.
73 Gomez RS, Guatimosim C, Gomez MV. Mechanism of action of volatile anesthetics: role of protein kinase C. Cell Mol Neurobiol 2003; 23:877–885.
74 Eckenhoff RG. Promiscuous ligands and attractive cavities: how do the inhaled anesthetics work? Mol Interv 2001; 1:258–268.
75 Mantz J, Cordier J, Giaume C. Effects of general anesthetics on intercellular communications mediated by gap junctions between astrocytes in primary culture. Anesthesiology 1993; 78:892–901.
76 Pennisi E. Genetics. Working the (gene count) numbers: finally, a firm answer? Science 2007; 316:1113.
77 Hemmings HC, Akabas MH, Goldstein PA,
et al. Emerging molecular mechanisms of general anesthetic action. Trends Pharmacol Sci 2005; 26:503–510.
78 Sleigh J. Mechanisms of General Anesthesia.
81st Meeting of the International Anesthesia Research Society. Orlando, Florida, USA: IARS, Cleveland, USA; 2007.
79 Brunori M, Vallone B, Cutruzzola F,
et al. The role of cavities in protein dynamics: crystal structure of a photolytic intermediate of a mutant myoglobin. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2000; 97:2058–2063.
80 Eckenhoff RG, Johansson JS. Molecular interactions between inhaled anesthetics and proteins. Pharmacol Rev 1997; 49:343–367.
81 Eckenhoff MF, Eckenhoff RG. Quantitative autoradiography of halothane binding in rat brain. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1998; 285:371–376.
82 Perouansky M, Hemmings HC, Pearce RA. Anesthetic effects on glutamatergic neurotransmission: lessons learned from a large synapse. Anesthesiology 2004; 100:470–472.
83 Westphalen RI, Krivitski M, Amarosa A,
et al. Reduced inhibition of cortical glutamate and GABA release by halothane in mice lacking the K
+ channel, TREK-1. Br J Pharmacol 2007; 152:939–945.
84 Cheng G, Kendig JJ. Enflurane directly depresses glutamate AMPA and NMDA currents in mouse spinal cord motor neurons independent of actions on GABAA or glycine receptors. Anesthesiology 2000; 93:1075–1084.
85 Cheng G, Kendig JJ. Pre and postsynaptic volatile anaesthetic actions on glycinergic transmission to spinal cord motor neurons. Br J Pharmacol 2002; 136:673–684.
86 Wang MY, Rampil IJ, Kendig JJ. Ethanol directly depresses AMPA and NMDA glutamate currents in spinal cord motor neurons independent of actions on GABAA or glycine receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1999; 290:362–367.
87 Antognini JF, Wang XW, Carstens E. Quantitative and qualitative effects of isoflurane on movement occurring after noxious stimulation. Anesthesiology 1999; 91:1064–1071.
88 Sonner JM, Antognini JF, Dutton RC,
et al. Inhaled anesthetics and immobility: mechanisms, mysteries, and minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration. Anesth Analg 2003; 97:718–740.
89 Mihic SJ, Ye Q, Wick MJ,
et al. Sites of alcohol and volatile anaesthetic action on GABA(A) and glycine receptors. Nature 1997; 389:385–389.
90 Perouansky M, Pearce RA. Is anesthesia caused by potentiation of synaptic or intrinsic inhibition? Recent insights into the mechanisms of volatile anesthetics. J Basic Clin Physiol Pharmacol 2000; 11:83–107.
91 Li X, Czajkowski C, Pearce RA. Rapid and direct modulation of GABAA receptors by halothane. Anesthesiology 2000; 92:1366–1375.
92 Patel AJ, Honore E, Lesage F,
et al. Inhalational anesthetics activate two-pore-domain background K
+ channels. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2:422–426.
93 Franks NP, Lieb WR. Background K
+ channels: an important target for volatile anesthetics? Nat Neurosci 1999; 2:395–396.
94 Kindler C. Effects of different classes of anesthetics on a new family of potassium channels with two pore domains in tandem [dissertation]. Basel, Switzerland: University of Basel; 2001. pp. 74–95.
95 Tonner PH, Miller KW. Molecular sites of general anaesthetic action on acetylcholine receptors. Eur J Anaesth 1995; 12:21–30.
96 Flood P, Ramirez-Latorre J, Role L. Alpha 4 beta 2 neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the central nervous system are inhibited by isoflurane and propofol, but alpha 7-type nicotinic acetylcholine receptors are unaffected. Anesthesiology 1997; 86:859–865.
97 Dildy-Mayfield JE, Eger EI, Harris RA. Anesthetics produce subunit-selective actions on glutamate receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1996; 276:1058–1065.
98 Franks NP, Dickinson R, de Sousa SL,
et al. How does xenon produce anaesthesia? Nature 1998; 396:324.
99 Harrison NL, Kugler JL, Jones MV,
et al. Positive modulation of human gamma-aminobutyric acid type A and glycine receptors by the inhalation anesthetic isoflurane. Mol Pharmacol 1993; 44:628–632.
100 Krasowski MD, Harrison NL. General anaesthetic actions on ligand-gated ion channels. Cell Mol Life Sci 1999; 55:1278–1303.
101 Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Todorovic SM, Mennerick S,
et al. Nitrous oxide (laughing gas) is an NMDA antagonist, neuroprotectant and neurotoxin. Nat Med 1998; 4:460–463.
102 Raines DE, Claycomb RJ, Scheller M, Forman SA. Nonhalogenated alkane anesthetics fail to potentiate agonist actions on two ligand-gated ion channels. Anesthesiology 2001; 95:470–477.
103 Zeilhofer HU, Swandulla D, Geisslinger G, Brune K. Differential effects of ketamine enantiomers on NMDA receptor currents in cultured neurons. Eur J Pharmacol 1992; 213:155–158.
104 Flood P, Krasowski MD. Intravenous anesthetics differentially modulate ligand-gated ion channels. Anesthesiology 2000; 92:1418–1425.
105 Orser BA. Lifting the fog around anesthesia. Sci Am 2007; 296:54–61.
106 Krasowski MD, Jenkins A, Flood P,
et al. General anesthetic potencies of a series of propofol analogs correlate with potency for potentiation of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) current at the GABA(A) receptor but not with lipid solubility. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2001; 297:338–351.
107 Tomlin SL, Jenkins A, Lieb WR, Franks NP. Stereoselective effects of etomidate optical isomers on gamma-aminobutyric acid type A receptors and animals. Anesthesiology 1998; 88:708–717.
108 Hales TG, Lambert JJ. The actions of propofol on inhibitory amino acid receptors of bovine adrenomedullary chromaffin cells and rodent central neurones. Br J Pharmacol 1991; 104:619–628.
109 Rudolph U, Crestani F, Benke D,
et al. Benzodiazepine actions mediated by specific gamma-aminobutyric acid(A) receptor subtypes. Nature 1999; 401:796–800.
110 Drafts BC, Fisher JL. Identification of structures within GABAA receptor alpha subunits that regulate the agonist action of pentobarbital. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2006; 318:1094–1101.
111 Borghese CM, Werner DF, Topf N,
et al. An isoflurane- and alcohol-insensitive mutant GABA(A) receptor alpha(1) subunit with near-normal apparent affinity for GABA: characterization in heterologous systems and production of knockin mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2006; 319:208–218.
112 Belelli D, Lambert JJ, Peters JA,
et al. The interaction of the general anesthetic etomidate with the gamma-aminobutyric acid type A receptor is influenced by a single amino acid. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1997; 94:11031–11036.
113 Nishikawa K, Jenkins A, Paraskevakis I, Harrison NL. Volatile anesthetic actions on the GABAA receptors: contrasting effects of alpha 1(S270) and beta 2(N265) point mutations. Neuropharmacology 2002; 42:337–345.
114 Jinks SL, Atherley RJ, Dominguez CL,
et al. Isoflurane disrupts central pattern generator activity and coordination in the lamprey isolated spinal cord. Anesthesiology 2005; 103:567–575.
115 Franks NP, Lieb WR. Stereospecific effects of inhalational general anesthetic optical isomers on nerve ion channels. Science 1991; 254:427–430.
116 Hille B. Ionic channels of excitable membranes. Sunderland, USA: Sinauer Associates, Inc.; 2001.
117 Gray AT, Winegar BD, Leonoudakis DJ,
et al. TOK1 is a volatile anesthetic stimulated K
+ channel. Anesthesiology 1998; 88:1076–1084.
118 Gray AT, Zhao BB, Kindler CH,
et al. Volatile anesthetics activate the human tandem pore domain baseline K
+ channel KCNK5. Anesthesiology 2000; 92:1722–1730.
119 Winegar BD, Owen DF, Yost CS,
et al. Volatile general anesthetics produce hyperpolarization of Aplysia neurons by activation of a discrete population of baseline potassium channels. Anesthesiology 1996; 85:889–900.
120 Gruss M, Bushell TJ, Bright DP,
et al. Two-pore-domain K
+ channels are a novel target for the anesthetic gases xenon, nitrous oxide, and cyclopropane. Mol Pharmacol 2004; 65:443–452.
121 Mathie A, Veale EL. Therapeutic potential of neuronal two-pore domain potassium-channel modulators. Curr Opin Investig Drugs 2007; 8:555–562.
122 Liu C, Au JD, Zou HL,
et al. Potent activation of the human tandem pore domain K channel TRESK with clinical concentrations of volatile anesthetics. Anesth Analg 2004; 99:1715–1722.
123 Heurteaux C, Guy N, Laigle C,
et al. TREK-1, a K
+ channel involved in neuroprotection and general anesthesia. EMBO J 2004; 23:2684–2695.
124 Papazian DM, Schwarz TL, Tempel BL,
et al. Cloning of genomic and complementary DNA from Shaker, a putative potassium channel gene from
Drosophila. Science 1987; 237:749–753.
125 Haydon DA, Urban BW. The actions of some general anaesthetics on the potassium current of the squid giant axon. J Physiol 1986; 373:311–327.
126 Friederich P, Urban BW. Interaction of intravenous anesthetics with human neuronal potassium currents in relation to clinical concentrations. Anesthesiology 1999; 91:1853–1860.
127 Zucker JR. ATP-sensitive potassium channel agonists do not alter MAC for isoflurane in rats. Anesthesiology 1992; 76:560–563.
128 Hara K, Harris RA. The anesthetic mechanism of urethane: the effects on neurotransmitter-gated ion channels. Anesth Analg 2002; 94:313–318.
129 Lin LH, Chen LL, Harris RA. Enflurane inhibits NMDA, AMPA, and kainate-induced currents in Xenopus oocytes expressing mouse and human brain mRNA. FASEB J 1993; 7:479–485.
130 McFarlane C, Warner DS, Todd MM, Nordholm L. AMPA receptor competitive antagonism reduces halothane MAC in rats. Anesthesiology 1992; 77:1165–1170.
131 Minami K, Wick MJ, Stern-Bach Y,
et al. Sites of volatile anesthetic action on kainate (Glutamate receptor 6) receptors. J Biol Chem 1998; 273:8248–8255.
132 Hollmann MW, Liu HT, Hoenemann CW,
et al. Modulation of NMDA receptor function by ketamine and magnesium. Part II: interactions with volatile anesthetics. Anesth Analg 2001; 92:1182–1191.
133 Maclver MB, Mikulec AA, Amagasu SM, Monroe FA. Volatile anesthetics depress glutamate transmission via presynaptic actions. Anesthesiology 1996; 85:823–834.
134 Joo DT, Gong D, Sonner JM,
et al. Blockade of AMPA receptors and volatile anesthetics: reduced anesthetic requirements in GluR2 null mutant mice for loss of the righting reflex and antinociception but not minimum alveolar concentration. Anesthesiology 2001; 94:478–488.
135 Sonner JM, Vissel B, Royle G,
et al. The effect of three inhaled anesthetics in mice harboring mutations in the GluR6 (kainate) receptor gene. Anesth Analg 2005; 101:143–148.
136 Sato Y, Kobayashi E, Murayama T,
et al. Effect of
N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor epsilon1 subunit gene disruption of the action of general anesthetic drugs in mice. Anesthesiology 2005; 102:557–561.
137 Dickinson R, Peterson BK, Banks P,
et al. Competitive inhibition at the glycine site of the
N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor by the anesthetics xenon and isoflurane: evidence from molecular modeling and electrophysiology. Anesthesiology 2007; 107:756–767.
138 Role LW, Berg DK. Nicotinic receptors in the development and modulation of CNS synapses. Neuron 1996; 16:1077–1085.
139 Flood P, Sonner JM, Gong D, Coates KM. Heteromeric nicotinic inhibition by isoflurane does not mediate MAC or loss of righting reflex. Anesthesiology 2002; 97:902–905.
140 Eger EI, Zhang Y, Laster M,
et al. Acetylcholine receptors do not mediate the immobilization produced by inhaled anesthetics. Anesth Analg 2002; 94:1500–1504.
141 Flood P, Role LW. Neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptor modulation by general anesthetics. Toxicol Lett 1998; 100–101:149–153.
142 Flood P, Sonner JM, Gong D, Coates KM. Isoflurane hyperalgesia is modulated by nicotinic inhibition. Anesthesiology 2002; 97:192–198.
143 Seitz PA, ter Riet M, Rush W, Merrell WJ. Adenosine decreases the minimum alveolar concentration of halothane in dogs. Anesthesiology 1990; 73:990–994.
144 Ma HC, Wang YF, Feng CS,
et al. Effects of adenosine agonist R-phenylisopropyl-adenosine on halothane anesthesia and antinociception in rats. Acta Pharmacol Sin 2005; 26:181–185.
145 Maricq AV, Peterson AS, Brake AJ,
et al. Primary structure and functional expression of the 5HT3 receptor, a serotonin-gated ion channel. Science 1991; 254:432–437.
146 Minami K, Minami M, Harris RA. Inhibition of 5-hydroxytryptamine type 2A receptor-induced currents by n-alcohols and anesthetics. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1997; 281:1136–1143.
147 Rampil IJ, Laster MJ, Eger EI. Antagonism of the 5-HT(3) receptor does not alter isoflurane MAC in rats. Anesthesiology 2001; 95:562–564.
148 Fukuda S, Warner DS. Cerebral protection. Br J Anaesth 2007; 99:10–17.
149 Sakai H, Sheng H, Yates RB,
et al. Isoflurane provides long-term protection against focal cerebral ischemia in the rat. Anesthesiology 2007; 106:92–99.
150 Bickler PE, Warner DS, Stratmann G, Schuyler JA. gamma-Aminobutyric acid-A receptors contribute to isoflurane neuroprotection in organotypic hippocampal cultures. Anesth Analg 2003; 97:564–571.
151 Zhan X, Fahlman CS, Bickler PE. Isoflurane neuroprotection in rat hippocampal slices decreases with aging: changes in intracellular Ca
2+ regulation and
N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor-mediated Ca
2+ influx. Anesthesiology 2006; 104:995–1003.
152 Dahmani S, Rouelle D, Gressens P, Mantz J. Effects of dexmedetomidine on hippocampal focal adhesion kinase tyrosine phosphorylation in physiologic and ischemic conditions. Anesthesiology 2005; 103:969–977.
153 Liu C, Cotten JF, Schuyler JA,
et al. Protective effects of TASK-3 (KCNK9) and related 2P K channels during cellular stress. Brain Res 2005; 1031:164–173.
154 Moller JT, Cluitmans P, Rasmussen LS,
et al. Long-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly ISPOCD1 study. Lancet 1998; 351:857–861.
155 Fong HK, Sands LP, Leung JM. The role of postoperative analgesia in delirium and cognitive decline in elderly patients: a systematic review. Anesth Analg 2006; 102:1255–1266.
156 Crosby C, Culley DJ, Baxter MG,
et al. Spatial memory performance 2 weeks after general anesthesia in adult rats. Anesth Analg 2005; 101:1389–1392.
157 Culley DJ, Raghavan SV, Waly M,
et al. Nitrous oxide decreases cortical methionine synthase transiently but produces lasting memory impairment in aged rats. Anesth Analg 2007; 105:83–88.
158 Rasmussen LS, Johnson T, Kuipers HM,
et al. Does anaesthesia cause postoperative cognitive dysfunction? A randomised study of regional versus general anaesthesia in 438 elderly patients. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 2003; 47:260–266.
159 Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Hartman RE, Izumi Y,
et al. Early exposure to common anesthetic agents causes widespread neurodegeneration in the developing rat brain and persistent learning deficits. J Neurosci 2003; 23:876–882.
160 Wilder RT, Flick RP, Sprung J,
et al. Relationship between exposure to anesthesia and subsequent learning disabilities in children [ASA abstract]. Anesthesiology 2008; 109:A1416.
161 Rajaram S, Spangler TL, Sedensky MM, Morgan PG. A stomatin and a degenerin interact to control anesthetic sensitivity in
Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 1999; 153:1673–1682.
162 Campbell DB, Nash HA. Use of
Drosophila mutants to distinguish among volatile general anesthetics. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1994; 91:2135–2139.
163 Koubi L, Tarek M, Klein ML, Scharf D. Distribution of halothane in a dipalmitoylphosphatidylcholine bilayer from molecular dynamics calculations. Biophys J 2000; 78:800–811.
164 Kitamura A, Sato R, Marszalec W,
et al. Halothane and propofol modulation of gamma-aminobutyric acid A receptor single-channel currents. Anesth Analg 2004; 99:409–415.
165 Gottschalk A, Haney P. Computational aspects of anesthetic action in simple neural models. Anesthesiology 2003; 98:548–564.