INTRODUCTION
Even before birth, during the development of the auditory cortex, auditory stimulation facilitates spoken language as well as social, emotional, and cognitive development (Sharma et al. 2009; Moon et al. 2013; Kral et al. 2016). Therefore, intervention should commence as early as possible to enable access to sound and the perception of speech to enhance speech and language development (Sharma et al. 2009; Kral et al. 2016). For this reason, neonatal hearing screening programs have been devised. In the Netherlands, a program has been available to all Dutch newborns since 2006 and hearing aids (HAs) are indicated for children with mild to severe hearing loss (HL), while cochlear implants (CIs) are administered to children who are hard of hearing (HH) or deaf, with a hearing threshold of 85 dB HL or higher (CI-ON 2012). For the present study, we used the hearing level classification of the World Health Organization (WHO 2020) as a reference. In babies who are HH or deaf, parents are given the option of early intervention for language/interaction and hearing via HAs or CIs (Boons et al. 2013). Early CI fitting, before the age of 12 months, provides children who are HH or deaf the ability to develop sufficient speech perception and recognition to achieve nearly age-appropriate spoken language development (Colletti et al. 2011; Leigh et al. 2013; Dettman et al. 2016). Currently, auditory access to spoken language in acoustic environments has improved due to early CI in children who are HH or deaf. This enables these children to acquire near age-appropriate language development. This level is comparable to that of children with moderate HL with HAs and, on average, is even better than that of children with severe HL with HAs (Yang et al. 2012; Leigh et al. 2016; Nekes 2016).
Children with severe HL are at risk for developmental language problems (Wake et al. 2005; Tomblin et al. 2015). Iwasaki et al. (2012) reported improved language outcomes in children who are HH or deaf with CIs compared to children with HAs. Several studies report delayed receptive and expressive language development in children with mild to severe HL with HAs (Borg et al. 2007; Koehlinger et al. 2013; Cupples et al. 2018). These language problems persist in adolescents with HL (Delage & Tuller 2007). Language and communication problems in children who are HH or deaf negatively interfere with social and emotional development (e.g., behavior issues, mental health, social skills, and interactions) (Wake et al. 2004; Barker et al. 2009; Stevenson et al. 2010; Gentili & Holwell 2011; Netten et al. 2015). Emotional and behavioral problems are therefore frequently reported in children with severe HL, profound HL or who are deaf with or without HAs (Mitchell & Quittner 1996; van Eldik et al. 2004; van Gent et al. 2007), and are more often reported in children with severe HL with HAs than in children who are HH or deaf with CIs (Theunissen et al. 2014; Theunissen et al. 2015; Wong et al. 2017). Children who are HH or deaf who receive CIs at a young age and achieve adequate communication seem to exhibit as few emotional and behavioral problems as their normal hearing peers (Theunissen et al. 2015; Boerrigter et al. 2019).
There is an ongoing debate about whether children with severe HL could also benefit from CIs over HAs (Lovett et al. 2015; Leigh et al. 2016; de Kleijn et al. 2018) to improve their relatively poor performance in a variety of domains. Parental preferences and clinical indication criteria for the choice of device (HA or a CI) for children with prelingual severe to profound HL are mostly based on the etiology and degree of hearing level. For optimal (early) intervention in children who are HH or deaf, this choice has to be made within the first year in life. However, in babies with severe HL, the choice between HAs or CIs is difficult when the consequences for various long-term developmental areas are not yet clear. The latest research shows surprisingly promising results in children who are HH or deaf who receive CIs at an early age; they achieve speech perception scores better than those of children with severe HL with HA (Yang et al. 2012; Leigh et al. 2016; Nekes 2016). Obviously, for children who are HH or deaf, speech perception through adequately fitted HAs or CIs is key to optimizing their full developmental potential. After all, the perception of speech is an important prerequisite for language development, reading, academic performance, and social and emotional well-being. Acquiring these abilities, however, is primarily taking place in informal, incidental daily situations. In typically developing (TD) children, more than 80% of language and general knowledge is acquired in incidental learning situations (Gillis & Schaerlaekens 2000). Incidental learning (learning without direct teaching) happens in many ways (e.g., through observation, repetition, social interaction, and problem solving). Children with poor listening abilities might have problems piecing together poorly heard information and, as a result, might not profit sufficiently from incidental learning. The inability to “overhear” spoken conversations limits the access of these children to many avenues of incidental learning, and therefore restricts their acquisition of knowledge of language, social interaction and how the world works, as well as stifling their development in many areas (Sarant 2012). Incidental learning is very important in acquiring complex linguistic components such as morphosyntax, reading comprehension, narrative abilities, and verbal reasoning skills. Incidental learning requires being able to recognize speech that is spoken at a distance (often to a third party) and that is spoken in noisy environments. Discourse is known to represent the primary language medium through which academic knowledge is conveyed and acquired, and verbal cognition has been shown to be an important indicator for children’s later academic performance and socioeconomic status (SES) also in deaf children (De Raeve 2015). In addition, a recent study showed that the perception of soft speech influences the magnitude and the timing of lexical access and competition in TD school-aged children (Hendrickson et al. 2021). This is in agreement with results in deaf children (Davidson 2011); lexical decision was poorer with stimuli at 50 dB SPL as compared to 70 dB SPL. Therefore, speech perception in adverse conditions, such as speech in noise or at soft loudness level is an important parameter. For indication purposes especially, it is proposed that speech perception measures used should reflect the listening challenges that individuals encounter in natural communication situations (Firszt et al. 2004).
Various studies have found that executive function (EF) is a good predictor of performance in everyday life. EF refers to a set of higher-order cognitive processes involved in the self-regulation of thoughts, actions, and emotions. These include mental processes such as planning, (verbal) working memory, inhibiting inappropriate responses, flexibility in adapting to changes, and decision-making, which are necessary for adaptive and goal-oriented behavior (P. Anderson 2002; V. Anderson 2002; Senn et al. 2004). EF is a good predictor of language development, academic achievement, and learning-related behaviors such as listening to instructions, following directions, and accomplishing tasks (Bull et al. 2008; Best et al. 2011; Neuenschwander et al. 2012), as well as for children’s social and emotional development (Riggs et al. 2006; Raaijmakers et al. 2008; Schoemaker et al. 2013).
In children who are HH or deaf, several EF tasks such as working memory, cognitive shifting, planning, and inhibition are poorly developed (Beer et al. 2011; Kronenberger et al. 2013; Kronenberger et al. 2014). In children who are HH or deaf, several factors can limit EF. First, this is probably due to poorer verbal communication, reduced access to social situations, and a higher cognitive load resulting from effortful listening (Lin et al. 2011; Botting et al. 2017). HL makes listening effortful, tiring, or stressful. Greater cognitive resources are needed to perceive sound and speech, with the consequence that this could be detrimental to the cognitive resources needed for other cognitive processes, such as EF (Lin et al. 2011; Pichora-Fuller et al. 2016). This makes long-term developmental outcomes for EF a relevant issue in the debate on CI indication criteria. Second, the language skills of children who are HH or deaf are associated with nonverbal EF tasks and even have predictive value (Botting et al. 2017; Jones et al. 2020). Poorer language levels thus negatively affect EF development.
Given the strong relationship among speech perception skills, language abilities, and EF, we argue that improving speech perception could lead to better language and EF development. If so, one might question whether children with severe HL with HAs are disadvantaged in terms of their developmental opportunities. It is important to investigate whether they would benefit more from CIs than from HAs.
Therefore, we aimed to determine whether children with severe HL with HAs are disadvantaged in their perception of speech, language development, and EF compared to children who are HH or deaf with CIs and if they would benefit in the long-term from CIs over HAs. Our inclusion of long-term results contributes to the debate concerning CI indication criteria. We addressed the following research question to achieve this aim: Do children who are HH or deaf with CIs perform better than children with severe HL with HAs with respect to auditory speech perception, and subsequently receptive vocabulary and/or EF?
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
The participants are children with severe HL, profound HL or who are deaf with HAs or CIs. Children with HAs in this study had severe or profound HL according to the WHO classification (WHO 2020), the unaided best ear pure-tone average (PTA) of 60 dB HL or higher, averaged over the frequencies of 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz. The mean PTA was 69 dB HL (SD = 8.7), ranging from 60 to 97 dB HL. HAs were fitted according to the latest Desired Sensation Level prescriptive formula for children (Seewald et al. 2005). Children with CIs are HH or deaf, with an unaided best ear PTA >90 dB HL. CIs were all fully inserted. We included children from ages 8 to 15 with a nonverbal intelligence quotient of 80 or higher. We recruited children who are HH or deaf at Radboud University Medical Centre and four speech and hearing centers in the Netherlands. The CI group and HA group did not differ in language, educational setting, developmental problems, test age, SES, or nonverbal intelligence quotient. We measured SES regarding parents’ jobs on a 4-point scale. Children with additional developmental problems were diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, autism spectrum disorder, or had motor disabilities or learning problems such as dyslexia. The ages and characteristics of children who are TD varied per test norm group for specific assessments and are reported for each test separately in the assessment section below. Table 1 provides the characteristics of children with CIs and HAs.
TABLE 1. -
Descriptive statistics of the total study population
Demographics |
Total study population (N = 70) |
p
|
CI (n = 43) |
HA (n = 27) |
n (%) |
n (%) |
Sex |
|
|
0.894 |
Male |
23 (54) |
14 (52) |
|
Female |
20 (46) |
13 (48) |
|
Age at onset of hearing loss |
|
|
0.006*
|
<3 yr old |
36 (84) |
19 (70) |
|
≥3 yr old |
1 (2) |
7 (26) |
|
Unknown |
6 (14) |
1 (4) |
|
Etiology of hearing loss |
|
|
0.018*
|
Genetic |
28 (65) |
12 (44) |
|
Acquired |
11 (26) |
5 (19) |
|
Idiopathic |
4 (9) |
10 (37) |
|
Fitting |
|
|
0.000*
|
Unilateral |
17 (39) |
0 (0) |
|
Bilateral |
21 (49) |
27 (100) |
|
Bimodal |
5 (12) |
0 (0) |
|
Language |
|
|
0.409 |
Spoken |
27 (63) |
21 (78) |
|
Sign-supported spoken language |
7 (16) |
3 (11) |
|
Dutch Sign Language |
9 (21) |
3 (11) |
|
Educational setting |
|
|
0.466 |
Mainstream |
24 (56) |
19 (71) |
|
Special Hearing loss |
15 (35) |
6 (22) |
|
Special other |
4 (9) |
2 (7) |
|
Additional developmental problems |
|
|
0.757 |
Yes |
8 (19) |
4 (15) |
|
No |
35 (81) |
23 (85) |
|
|
M (SD) |
M (SD) |
p
|
Test age (yr) |
11.85 (1.87) |
11.01 (2.26) |
0.096 |
Age at detection of hearing loss (mo) |
4.70 (9.09) |
19.62 (23.30) |
0.000*
|
Age at hearing aids (mo) |
11.39 (9.90) |
27.96 (25.88) |
0.000*
|
Age at implantation (mo) |
34.35 (31.74) |
|
|
Duration of deafness or hearing loss before CI or HA (mo) |
27.30 (23.6) |
7.50 (10.07) |
0.002*
|
Socioeconomic status†
|
2.32 (1.09) |
2.29 (0.99) |
0.898 |
Nonverbal IQ |
102.94 (11.98) |
103.10 (15.69) |
0.973 |
*p < 0.05.
†Socioeconomic status score was measured by parental jobs, range 0–4.
CI, cochlear implant; HA, hearing aid; IQ, intelligence quotient.
Ethical Considerations
The reference number of the study is 2016–2611 (July 14, 2016). The research ethics committee of the Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre states that the abovementioned study does not fall within the remit of the Medical Research Involving Human Subjects Act (WMO) for children with CIs from Radboudumc. Further, for children with HAs from external audiological centers, all parents of participants and participants older than 12 provided written informed consent for the use of their data, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Procedure
We designed the test procedure to minimize the burden for the children and the professionals by adapting to the existing assessment protocols of the centers involved. We collected data for children with CIs during standard assessments at annual clinical follow-up sessions at the University Medical Centre, in which we determined speech perception in quiet and in noise, as well as language and cognitive abilities, for counseling purposes. For children with HAs, only speech perception data in quiet were collected annually. We gathered these data from the files. In addition, we collected data on language and cognition, especially for this study. Speech perception assessments were performed under the supervision of a clinical audiologist. Receptive vocabulary was measured by a speech and language pathologist, speech therapist, or child psychologist. EF tests were administered by a child psychologist. We randomized the order of the assessments for each participant. All children were tested in their preferred mode of communication (speech or sign-supported speech).
Materials and Methods
Speech Perception
We tested speech perception using the Dutch Audiology Society (NVA) hearing test for children, which contains meaningful consonant–vowel–consonant (CVC) words (Bosman & Smoorenburg 1995). This test was performed in a soundproof booth designed for audiometric testing. Stimuli were presented from a loudspeaker at one meter in front of the child in the free field in quiet. The presentation level of the stimuli was at the conversational level at 65 dB SPL (which is comparable with a person at 1 meter’s distance speaking with normal loudness) and the soft speech level at 45 dB SPL (which is comparable with a speaker at 3 to 5 meter’s distance speaking with normal loudness, such as a teacher in front of a classroom). The NVA children’s test consists of lists of 12 CVC words, in which a correct phoneme score is calculated over the last 11 CVC words. Scores are expressed as a percentage of correctly repeated phonemes.
Receptive Vocabulary
We assessed receptive vocabulary using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III-NL (Dunn & Dunn 2005). The PPVT is a standardized, validated test for those aged 2 years and 3 months to 90 years. The stimuli are spoken words that are presented live, audiovisually, to the child at the conversational level. Responses are manual in a closed-set task. The child has to identify the stimulus item out of four pictures. The score is expressed as a receptive vocabulary standard score. An average age-appropriate standard score is 100 (SD 15), and below-average scores of the PPVT are equivalent to a scaled score below 85. Normative data are based on a sample of 1746 children ranging in age from 2; 3–15; 11 years, and 1164 adults aged up to 90 years.
Executive Function
We used two distinctive tests to measure seven specific EF processes. We used the Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) battery (Delis et al. 2001) to gauge planning, inhibition, verbal cognitive flexibility, nonverbal cognitive flexibility, and problem-solving. To measure short-term and long-term verbal memory, we employed the Dutch Auditory Verbal Learning Test for children (Kingma & van den Burg 2008), a Dutch version of Ray’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Ray 1964).
The Delis Kaplan Executive Function System
The D-KEFS (Delis et al. 2001) is a comprehensive test battery of nine distinctive tests, of which five have been translated into Dutch. Each test can be used as a stand-alone measure of EF to gauge planning, inhibition, verbal cognitive flexibility, nonverbal cognitive flexibility, and problem-solving. Raw test scores are converted into scaled scores, with an age-appropriate mean score of 10 (SD 3). Normative data are based on a sample of 1750 children and adults ranging in age from 8 to 89 years. For this study, we used the five tests that are translated into Dutch, which are described below. Normative data are based on a sample of 700 children ranging in age from 6–15 years.
Color-Word Interference Test (Inhibition and Verbal Cognitive Flexibility).
The Color-Word Interference Test is a version of the Stroop test (Stroop 1935), which is used to assess inhibition and cognitive flexibility. The inhibition task requires the child to rapidly name the color in which words are printed while inhibiting the more automatic task of reading the words aloud. The cognitive flexibility task contains a shifting task in which naming the color of the word (instead of reading the word) alternates with reading the word that is framed. We used scaled completion time scores of both tasks as measures of inhibition and verbal cognitive flexibility.
Design Fluency Test (Nonverbal Cognitive Flexibility).
This test consists of three 1-minute tasks. The child is required to draw as many different designs on a sheet of paper with several boxes filled with dots, using only four lines. In the first task, the filled dots must be connected to each other. In the second task, the empty dots must be connected. In the third task, the child is required to alternate between the filled and empty dots. The number of correct designs in the third task is converted into a scaled score and is used as a measure of nonverbal cognitive flexibility.
Tower Test (Planning).
The child is required to build a target tower, which is shown in a picture, by moving disks of different sizes across three pegs in as few steps as possible as quickly as possible. The level of difficulty increases as the number of disks increases from 3 to 5. The child is not allowed to place a larger disk on top of a smaller disk or to move more than one disk at a time. The step-accuracy ratio is converted to a scaled score as a measure of planning.
20-Questions Test (Problem-Solving).
A sheet with 30 pictures is presented to the child. The assignment is to pose as few “yes-or-no” questions as possible to identify the target picture. The overall achievement scaled score is used as a measure of problem-solving.
Trail-Making Test (Verbal Cognitive Flexibility).
The Letter Number Switching task of the Trail-Making Test requires the child to quickly connect a series of letters and numbers randomly spaced on a paper by drawing a line alternating between the two (e.g., 1–A–2–B). Norm completion time scores are taken as a measure of verbal cognitive flexibility.
Dutch Auditory Verbal Learning Test
The Dutch version of the Ray Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Dutch RAVLT) for children aged 6 to 12 (Kingma & van den Burg 2008) consists of a list of 15 nouns that is read aloud in the same order five consecutive times. After each list, the child is asked to name the words he/she remembers (free recall). After a 25-minute interval, the child is asked again to recall the words (delayed recall). The total number of words of the free recall and the delayed recall are transformed to percentile scores with an age-appropriate mean score of 5.5 (SD 1.5) and used as a measure of short-term verbal memory and long-term verbal memory, respectively. Normative data are based on a sample of 225 Dutch children ranging in age from 6 to 12 years (van den Burg & Kingma 1999).
Statistical Analyses
We used IBM SPSS Statistics 25 for all statistical analyses. We employed nonparametric analyses since the assumption of normality, tested with the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, was violated for almost all study variables. First, we computed the median and interquartile range of all outcomes for the CI and HA groups separately. Second, we compared the group results for receptive vocabulary and EF of the HA and CI groups with the norm data of the TD group, and we performed a one-sample Chi-square test to examine the goodness of fit (p < 0.05). Differences between the distributions reflect below-average performance. Next, we contrasted the median scores for speech perception, receptive vocabulary, and EF between the CI and HA groups using the Mann-Whitney U test (p < 0.05). Finally, we employed Kendall’s tau-b (p < 0.05) to investigate any relationships between speech perception, receptive vocabulary, EF, and other study variables.
RESULTS
Comparison of Outcomes Between the CI and HA Groups
Speech Perception
Children with CIs reached a median speech perception score of 87% at 45 dB SPL. The children with HAs reached a median speech perception score of 68%. The Mann-Whitney U test indicated that speech perception scores at 45 dB SPL in the CI group (mean rank = 45.14, n = 43) were significantly higher than those in the HA group (mean rank = 20.15, n = 27), U = 995.00, z = 5.01, p < 0.01, two tailed. This effect can be described as a large effect (r = 0.60) and is illustrated in Figure 1. This means that children with CIs on average have better auditory perception at a soft speech level than children with HAs. There was no significant difference between children with CIs and HAs in speech perception performance at the conversation level. Both groups obtained ceiling scores. The median speech perception scores were 96% and 93%, respectively.
Fig. 1.: Distribution of speech perception scores on the speech perception test at 45 dB SPL in the CI and HA groups. CI, cochlear implant; HA, hearing aid.
In Table 2, the nonparametric descriptive statistics for receptive vocabulary and EF are presented for the CI and HA groups, as well as the expected results from their TD peers.
TABLE 2. -
The nonparametric descriptive of receptive vocabulary and EF results for the CI and HA groups and the expected results of the TD group
Materials |
Distributions: mean (–1 SD to +1 SD) |
% Below average |
TD norm group |
CI |
HA |
TD |
CI |
HA |
PPVT |
Receptive vocabulary |
100 (85–115) |
93 (78–107) |
94 (84–103) |
16 |
28 |
22 |
D-KEFS |
Color-Word Interference Test |
Inhibition |
10 (8–12) |
10 (8–12) |
10 (9–11) |
16 |
19 |
7 |
Verbal cognitive flexibility |
10 (8–12) |
10 (8–12) |
10 (9–12) |
16 |
24 |
11 |
Design fluency test |
Nonverbal cognitive flexibility |
10 (8–12) |
11 (9–13) |
12 (9–13) |
16 |
12 |
19 |
Tower test |
Planning |
10 (8–12) |
8 (6–9) |
9 (6–9) |
16 |
44 |
33 |
20-questions test |
Problem solving |
10 (8–12) |
11 (9–12) |
12 (9–14) |
16 |
7 |
11 |
Trail-making test*
|
Verbal cognitive flexibility |
10 (8–12) |
11 (8–12) |
11 (9–13) |
16 |
8 |
8 |
Dutch RAVLT |
Short-term verbal memory |
5.5 (4–7) |
4 (1–7) |
5 (4–8) |
16 |
47 |
23 |
Long-term verbal memory |
5.5 (4–7) |
4 (1–8) |
5.5 (2–7) |
16 |
44 |
35 |
*N = HA group: N = 25. CI group: N = 38.
CI, cochlear implant; D-KEFS, Delis Kaplan Executive Function System; EF, executive function; HA, hearing aid; PPVT, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; Dutch RAVLT, Ray Auditory Verbal Learning Test; TD, typically developing.
Receptive Vocabulary
There was no significant difference in receptive vocabulary scores between children with CIs and HAs.
Executive Function
The Mann-Whitney U test showed no significant differences in EF domain scores between the children with CIs and HAs. Distributions of the scaled scores of both groups on the D-KEFS tests are presented in Figure 2. Figure 3 displays the distributions of the CI and HA groups for the Dutch RAVLT.
Fig. 2.: Distributions of scaled scores on the D-KEFS tests in the CI and HA groups. CI, cochlear implant; D-KEFS, Delis Kaplan Executive Function System; HA, hearing aid.
Fig. 3.: Distributions of scaled scores on the Dutch RAVLT in the CI and HA groups. CI, cochlear implant; HA, hearing aid; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test.
Comparison of Outcomes of CI and HA Groups With the Normative Data of the TD Group
Receptive Vocabulary
The Chi-square test for goodness of fit indicates significant differences between observed and expected distributions between the children with CIs and the norm group data of the TD children (p = 0.033, with a medium effect size ω = 0.32); 28% of the CI group obtained below-average receptive vocabulary scores. We did not detect any significant differences in receptive vocabulary between the HA group and the norm group data of the TD children.
Executive Function
The Chi-square test for goodness of fit demonstrated, for the CI group, significant differences between observed and expected distributions based on the norm group data of the children who are TD on the Tower test (planning) (p = 0.004, medium effect size ω = 0.44) of the D-KEFS. We did not observe a significant difference on the other tests of the D-KEFS. Short-term and long-term verbal memory of the Dutch RAVLT also revealed significantly different outcomes in the CI group. Regarding differences between observed and expected distributions of short-term verbal memory, p = 0.002, with a large effect size, and ω = 0.51; for long-term verbal memory, p = 0.007, with a medium effect size, ω = 0.45. Forty-four percent of the CI group obtained below-average scores for planning, 47% on the short-term verbal memory tasks, and 44% on the long-term verbal memory tasks.
In the HA group, we only found a significant difference between the observed and expected distributions of the TD norm group on the Tower test (planning) (p = 0.014, medium effect size ω = 0.47) of the D-KEFS and the long-term verbal memory tasks of the Dutch RAVLT p = 0.01, with a large effect size, ω = 0.50. For planning, 33% of the HA group obtained below-average scores, and 35% obtained below-average scores for long-term verbal memory.
Associations Between Speech Perception, Receptive Vocabulary, EF, and Other Study Variables for the CI and HA Groups Separately
We used Kendall’s tau-b to investigate relationships among speech perception, receptive vocabulary, and EF. Looking at the relationship among speech perception, receptive vocabulary, and EF, we noted a significant, medium positive association in the HA group between speech perception at 45 dB SPL and the receptive vocabulary standard score (τ = 0.284, p = 0.042, two-tailed, n = 27), and a significant, medium, positive association (τ = 0.307, p = 0.038, two-tailed, n = 27) between speech perception at 45 dB SPL and the Tower test (planning). No significant relationships were found among speech perception, receptive vocabulary, and EF in the CI group. A visual representation of the significant relationships with speech perception at 45 dB SPL is presented in Figure 4. Other significant relationships are portrayed in Table 3.
TABLE 3. -
Kendall’s tau-b correlations between study variables and
executive function for CI (gray) and HA (white) groups
Study variables |
1. |
2. |
3. |
4. |
5. |
6. |
7. |
8. |
9. |
10. |
11. |
12. |
13. |
14. |
15. |
1. Age at onset of hearing loss (HL) |
— |
0.276
*
|
— |
0.120 |
0.222 |
–0.233 |
–0.041 |
0.130 |
0.102 |
–0.023 |
0.007 |
0.257 |
–0.210 |
0.095 |
0.058 |
2. Age at implantation |
— |
— |
— |
–0.169 |
0.037 |
–0.129 |
–0.065 |
0.085 |
0.250
*
|
0.203 |
–0.025 |
–0.138 |
0.018 |
0.026 |
0.044 |
3. pure-tone average (PTA) best ear |
–0.146 |
— |
— |
— |
— |
— |
— |
— |
— |
— |
— |
— |
— |
— |
— |
4. Socioeconomic status |
0.016 |
— |
–0.162 |
— |
0.012 |
0.052 |
0.233 |
0.233 |
0.097 |
0.096 |
0.022 |
–0.093 |
0.273 |
0.259 |
0.067 |
5. Nonverbal IQ |
–0.479
*
|
— |
–0.087 |
0.345
*
|
— |
0.417
*
|
0.169 |
0.045 |
0.531
*
|
0.327 |
0.497
*
|
0.258 |
0.163 |
–0.025 |
0.163 |
6. Speech perception at 45 dB SPL |
–0.295
*
|
— |
–0.410
*
|
0.104 |
0.284 |
— |
0.319*
|
0.037 |
0.099 |
0.191 |
0.177 |
–0.065 |
0.173 |
0.078 |
0.103 |
7. Speech perception at 65 dB SPL |
–0.326
*
|
— |
0.088 |
0.211 |
0.387
*
|
0.358
*
|
— |
–0.045 |
0.134 |
0.224 |
0.233
*
|
–0.090 |
0.133 |
0.162 |
0.113 |
8. Receptive vocabulary |
–0.188 |
— |
0.045 |
0.139 |
0.477
*
|
0.284
*
|
0.191 |
— |
0.121 |
0.001 |
0.134 |
0.072 |
0.030 |
0.067 |
0.007 |
9. Color-Word Interference Test—Inhibition |
0.118 |
— |
–0.088 |
0.291 |
0.119 |
–0.019 |
0.107 |
––0.099 |
— |
0.564
†
|
0.291
*
|
–0.14 |
0.157 |
0.160 |
0.182 |
10. Color-Word Interference Test—Verbal cognitive flexibility |
0.044 |
— |
0.053 |
0.094 |
0.344
*
|
–0.071 |
0.039 |
0.213 |
0.340
*
|
— |
0.256*
|
0.019 |
0.208 |
0.255
*
|
0.216 |
11. Design fluency test—Nonverbal cognitive flexibility |
–0.022 |
— |
–0.056 |
0.417
*
|
0.178 |
0.049 |
0.080 |
0.088 |
0.283 |
0.171 |
— |
0.147 |
0.142 |
0.183 |
0.372
*
|
12. Tower test—Planning |
–0.027 |
— |
–0.091 |
0.000 |
–0.096 |
0.307
*
|
0.080 |
–0.019 |
–0.13 |
–0.066 |
0.030 |
— |
–0.187 |
–0.065 |
–0.175 |
13. 20-questions test—Problem solving |
–0.047 |
— |
0.061 |
0.075 |
0.297 |
0.076 |
0.073 |
0.445
*
|
–0.006 |
0.205 |
0.094 |
–0.013 |
— |
0.209 |
0.406
*
|
14. RAVLT short-term verbal memory |
–0.024 |
— |
0.167 |
0.338
*
|
0.158 |
–0.039 |
–0.212 |
0.013 |
0.080 |
0.000 |
0.251 |
0.014 |
0.094 |
— |
0.605
†
|
15. RAVLT long-term verbal memory |
0.020 |
— |
0.054 |
0.109 |
–0.138 |
0.206 |
–0.038 |
–0.135 |
0.095 |
–0.169 |
0.251 |
0.386
*
|
–0.48 |
0.319
*
|
— |
*p < 0.05.
†p < 0.001.
CI, cochlear implant; HA, hearing aid; IQ, intelligence quotient.
Fig. 4.: Scatterplots of the Relation of Speech Perception scores with Receptive vocabulary and with planning. A and B, Scatterplot of the positive linear correlation between speech perception at 45 dB SPL and receptive vocabulary, and between speech perception at 45 dB SPL and planning in the HA group. CI, cochlear implant; HA, hearing aid.
DISCUSSION
We aimed to determine whether children with severe HL with HAs are disadvantaged in their perception of speech, receptive vocabulary, and EF compared to children who are HH or deaf with CIs and if they would benefit in the long-term from CIs over HAs. The research question is as follows: Do children who are HH or deaf with CIs perform better than children with severe HL with HAs with respect to auditory speech perception, and subsequently receptive vocabulary and/or EF?
First, we made a comparison between the outcomes of the CI and HA groups on speech perception, receptive vocabulary, and EF. We found no difference in the perception of speech at the conversational level (65 dB SPL) between children with CIs and HAs. This is in contrast to the results of Leigh et al. (2016), who reported that children who are HH or deaf with CIs reached higher speech perception scores than children with severe HL with HAs (PTA>66 dB HL) for speech perception at 65 dB SPL. The difference in the outcomes of Leigh et al. (2016) compared to our study can be attributed to the age of CI. The children with CIs in the study of Leigh et al. (2016) all received their CIs before 3 years of age. The mean age at CI in our study was 2 years and 10 months, with a SD of 2 years and 8 months.
As well established, speech perception in adverse listening situations is an important factor for the linguistic, cognitive, and socioemotional development. Therefore, it is necessary to assess at lower intensity levels that reflect real-life acoustic conditions. In our study, the children with severe HL with HAs demonstrated significantly poorer perception of soft speech (45 dB SPL) compared to children who are HH or deaf with CIs, which is in line with our hypothesis. Thus, children with HAs exhibit less favorable auditory perception of soft speech. This implies that even state of the art technology does not provide a consistently optimal signal.
Yang et al. (2012) found that children with CIs had better speech in noise perception scores than children with severe HL (PTA > 70 dB HL) with HAs. This indicates that perception of speech in more challenging acoustic situations is better among children with CIs than in children with severe HL with HAs.
We compared the outcomes of the CI and HA groups to the norm group of children who are TD and computed associations between variables. Next, we discuss these findings. The receptive vocabulary of both groups was age-appropriate. This is in line with earlier reports, such as the work of Langereis & Vermeulen (2013), who indicated that phoneme scores >85% at the conversational level (65 dB SPL) enable children to develop sufficient receptive vocabulary. There was no association between the perception of speech at 65 dB SPL and receptive vocabulary probably due to the ceiling effect. Nonetheless, we found an association between the perception of soft speech at 45 dB SPL and receptive vocabulary in the HA group. Accordingly, De Raeve et al. (2015) reported a positive correlation between the perception of soft speech and verbal cognition in children with CIs. These findings suggest that increasing perception of soft speech in children with severe HL with HAs could contribute to improved language development abilities. In contrast, we did not observe a relationship between the perception of soft speech and receptive vocabulary in children who are HH or deaf with CIs. The absence of this relationship in the CI group is likely due to the distribution of speech perception scores at 45 dB SPL of children who are HH or deaf with CIs. Approximately 80% of these children achieved soft speech perception scores of 75% or higher. The relationship between speech perception and language development has already been demonstrated (Tsao et al. 2004; Leigh et al. 2013; Dettman et al. 2016). In our study, a small group of children who are HH or deaf with CIs exhibited relatively good speech perception levels but nevertheless had poor receptive vocabulary levels. One explanation could be that 39% of the children in the CI group were fitted with a unilateral CI. Research by De Raeve et al. (2015) and Jacobs et al. (2016) implies that bilateral CI users have better language and verbal cognitive outcomes than unilateral CI users. Thirty-nine percent of the CI group in our study received unilateral implants, whereas currently, in the Netherlands, children who are HH or deaf almost always receive bilateral CIs; hence, improved developmental outcomes are expected in these children (De Raeve et al. 2015; Jacobs et al. 2016; Di Stadio et al. 2018). Another explanation for the children with CI who exhibited relatively good speech perception levels but had poor receptive vocabulary levels could be due to the relatively old age at intervention in our CI group (our study group was implanted at a relatively old age; M = 34 months, SD = 32). As stated before, earlier implantation (before 12 months) leads to better auditory and language outcomes (Dettman et al. 2016; Kral et al. 2016; McKinney 2017). However, we did not observe a correlation between the age of implantation and receptive vocabulary. A third explanation could be related to the etiology of deafness. A frequent diagnosis in children with HL is cytomegalovirus or other etiologies of severe disabilities. Children with cytomegalovirus vary widely in their audiometric, language, and cognitive outcomes (Ciorba et al. 2009; Fletcher et al. 2018). In our study, the etiology of a large share of the HA group was unknown. We thus did not include etiology in the analyses.
Our results indicate that to obtain age-appropriate levels of receptive vocabulary and EF, the perception of soft speech is a necessary but insufficient prerequisite. For children with severe HL with HAs an association between the perception of soft speech and receptive vocabulary and planning was found. For children who are HH or deaf with CIs, poor planning and verbal memory abilities cannot be associated to deficits in the perception of soft speech. Interestingly, children with CI who have poor speech perception abilities show poor vocabulary and planning skills, but children with CI who have good speech perception show a very large variability. Assessment of speech perception in challenging listening conditions, such as the perception of soft speech or speech in noise, could be relevant for CI indications. Several studies suggest broadening the auditory threshold inclusion criteria for pediatric CI to a PTA of 80 dB HL (de Kleijn et al. 2018) or even 60 dB HL (Leigh et al. 2016). In Belgium, the auditory criteria for CI were recently revised to a PTA of 70 dB HL (De Raeve et al. 2020). In line with these results, our findings suggest that it is important to determine whether auditory gain with a CI is associated with other developmental areas.
The analyses of EF outcomes, which were focused on a comparison of the CI and HA groups with the norm group, revealed a difference between the distributions of children with CIs and children who are TD in three domains: (1) planning and (2) short- and (3) long-term verbal memory tasks. Large numbers of children with CIs exhibited below-average scores compared to the TD norm group data on these tasks. The finding that children who are HH or deaf with CIs demonstrate short-term memory problems is in line with other studies. AuBuchon et al. (2015) and Watson et al. (2007) showed that children who are HH or deaf with CIs demonstrated short-term verbal memory developmental problems compared to their hearing peers. This could be attributed to the large number of children with a unilateral CI (39%) compared to the children with HAs, who all have bilateral HA fittings. Di Stadio et al. (2018) found that children with unilateral HL exhibited increased memory problems compared to children with bilateral hearing devices or their normal hearing peers. Stiles et al. (2012) indicated that children with mild or severe HL with HAs showed resilience in verbal short-term memory, with the important caveat that all children who used HAs in their study used oral communication and were enrolled in oral classrooms. It could be that the verbal/oral learning environment in the mainstream educational setting contributes to a better verbal memory system. Therefore, the educational setting could partly explain the observed differences in verbal memory between the HA and CI groups. In our study, the HA group, 71% of children were enrolled in a mainstream educational setting compared to 56% of the CI group.
Our study demonstrates that a considerable proportion of children with HL experience problems in planning. A possible explanation for this might be that inner speech is reduced in children with severe or profound HL. Inner speech has an important role in self-regulation, cognition and behavior (Alderson-Day & Fernyhough 2015), and interferes with EF. Wallace et al. (2017) investigated the role of verbal mediation in a tower test task in a hearing population, demonstrating that efficient planning relies on inner speech, which seems to be related to language development (Alarcon-Rubio et al. 2013). The fact that we did not observe a direct relationship between language and planning in our study could be because we examine receptive vocabulary only. According to Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development, inner speech comprises a transformation from external social speech to private speech to inner speech. This implies that expressive language should also be explored. Jones et al. (2020) found that expressive vocabulary affects EF development. However, inner speech is a complex, abstract level of language, containing more aspects such as thoughts, feelings, and knowledge. It is a difficult concept to measure and cannot be captured by standardized language tests (Alderson-Day & Fernyhough 2015). Further research should be undertaken to determine the role of language and inner speech in EF in children with HL with CIs and HAs. We did identify a correlation between the perception of soft speech and planning in children with severe HL with HAs.
For children with HL who obtained deviant planning or verbal memory scores, intensive computer-based EF training could be beneficial. EF computer training seems beneficial for children with CIs children with specific memory and language problems (Kronenberger et al. 2011). Perhaps this could also be helpful for children with HAs. It remains unclear, however, how these improved skills would generalize across diverse situations and behaviors in daily life. It is therefore recommended that EF computer-based training be combined with EF training that takes place in daily life situations in which the child is typically situated, such as at school, at home, or in sports or music training (Hsu et al. 2014; Blair 2017).
CONCLUSIONS
Children with severe HL with HAs exhibit less favorable auditory perception of soft speech, but not at a conversational level, compared to children who are HH or deaf with CIs. More children with severe HL with HAs and children who are HH or deaf with CIs exhibit problems in planning and verbal memory but not in other EF compared to the norm groups of TD children. The results indicate that to obtain age-appropriate levels of receptive vocabulary and EF, the perception of soft speech is a necessary but not sufficient prerequisite.
LIMITATIONS
There are several limitations regarding the participants in our study. With respect to children who are HH or deaf with CIs, the mean age at implantation was relatively late (34 months). This may have affected language and EF results because early implantation leads to better outcomes (Dettman et al. 2016; Kral et al. 2016; McKinney 2017). Further, this group consisted of 51% unilateral CI users in contrast to the children with severe HL with HAs, who had all bilateral HAs. Research shows that bilateral CI users have better language and cognitive outcomes than unilateral CI users (De Raeve et al. 2015). Currently, children who are HH or deaf in the Netherlands almost always receive bilateral CIs and are implanted earlier in life, and improved developmental outcomes are expected in these children. With regard to children with severe HL with HAs in the study, only four children had a PTA higher than 75 dB HL. The vast majority of the group had a PTA between 60 dB and 75 dB HL. Seventy-one percent of these children were enrolled in mainstream educational settings. Children with severe HL with HAs who were enrolled in special educational settings often declined to participate. The results of children with severe HL with HAs in this study group may be an overestimation. Further research with more matching research groups is needed to answer the question of which levels of hearing threshold CI could add to speech perception ability, resulting in better receptive vocabulary and EF performance in children with severe HL.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
M.S.B., H.A.M.M., E.A.M.M., A.M.V and M.C.L. received funding from Cochlear Benelux. The remaining authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
REFERENCES
Alarcon-Rubio D., Sanchez-Medina J. A., Winsler A. (2013). Private speech in illiterate adults: Cognitive functions, task difficulty, and literacy. J Adult Dev, 20, 100–111.
Alderson-Day B., Fernyhough C. (2015). Inner speech: Development, cognitive functions, phenomenology, and neurobiology. Psychol Bull, 141, 931–965.
Anderson P. (2002). Assessment and development of
executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol, 8, 71–82.
Anderson V. (2002).
Executive function in children: Introduction. Child Neuropsychol, 8, 69–70.
AuBuchon A. M., Pisoni D. B., Kronenberger W. G. (2015). Short-term and working memory impairments in early-implanted, long-term cochlear implant users are independent of audibility and speech production. Ear Hear, 36, 733–737.
Barker D. H., Quittner A. L., Fink N. E., Eisenberg L. S., Tobey E. A., Niparko J. K.; CDaCI Investigative Team. (2009). Predicting behavior problems in deaf and hearing children: The influences of
language, attention, and parent-child communication. Dev Psychopathol, 21, 373–392.
Beer J., Kronenberger W. G., Pisoni D. B. (2011).
Executive function in everyday life: Implications for young cochlear implant users.
Cochlear Implants Int, 12(Suppl 1), S89–S91.
Best J. R., Miller P. H., Naglieri J. A. (2011). Relations between
executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learn Individ Differ, 21, 327–336.
Blair C. (2017). Educating
executive function. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci, 8, 10.1002/wcs.1403.
Boerrigter M., Vermeulen A., Marres H., Mylanus E., Langereis M. (2019). Frequencies of behavioral problems reported by parents and teachers of hearing-impaired children with
cochlear implants. Front Psychol, 10, 1591.
Boons T., Brokx J., Frijns J., Philips B., Vermeulen A., Wouters J., van Wieringen A. (2013). Newborn hearing screening and cochlear implantation: Impact on spoken
language development. B-ENT, 21, 91–98.
Borg E., Edquist G., Reinholdson A. C., Risberg A., McAllister B. (2007). Speech and
language development in a population of Swedish hearing-impaired pre-school children, a cross-sectional study. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 71, 1061–1077.
Bosman A. J., Smoorenburg G. F. (1995). Intelligibility of Dutch CVC syllables and sentences for listeners with normal hearing and with three types of hearing impairment. Audiology, 34, 260–284.
Botting N., Jones A., Marshall C., Denmark T., Atkinson J., Morgan G. (2017). Nonverbal
executive function is mediated by
language: A study of deaf and hearing children. Child Dev, 88, 1689–1700.
Bull R., Espy K. A., Wiebe S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Dev Neuropsychol, 33, 205–228.
Ciorba A., Bovo R., Trevisi P., Bianchini C., Arboretti R., Martini A. (2009). Rehabilitation and outcome of severe profound deafness in a group of 16 infants affected by congenital cytomegalovirus infection. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol, 266, 1539–1546.
Colletti L., Mandalà M., Zoccante L., Shannon R. V., Colletti V. (2011). Infants versus older children fitted with
cochlear implants: Performance over 10 years. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 75, 504–509.
Cupples L., Ching T. Y., Button L., Seeto M., Zhang V., Whitfield J., Gunnourie M., Martin L., Marnane V. (2018). Spoken
language and everyday functioning in 5-year-old children using
hearing aids or
cochlear implants. Int J Audiol, 57(Suppl 2), S55–S69.
Davidson L. S., Geers A. E., Blamey P. J., Tobey E. A., Brenner C. A. (2011). Factors contributing to speech perception scores in long-term pediatric cochlear implant users. Ear Hear, 32(1 Suppl), 19S–26S.
de Kleijn J. L., van Kalmthout L. W. M., van der Vossen M. J. B., Vonck B. M. D., Topsakal V., Bruijnzeel H. (2018). Identification of pure-tone audiologic thresholds for pediatric cochlear implant candidacy: A systematic review. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg, 144, 630–638.
De Raeve L., Archbold S., Lehnhardt-Goriany M., Kemp T. (2020). Prevalence of
cochlear implants in Europe: Trend between 2010 and 2016.
Cochlear Implants Int, 21, 275–280.
De Raeve L., Vermeulen A., Snik A. (2015). Verbal cognition in deaf children using
cochlear implants: Effect of unilateral and bilateral stimulation. Audiol Neurootol, 20, 261–266.
Delage H., Tuller L. (2007).
Language development and mild-to-moderate hearing loss: Does
language normalize with age? J Speech Lang Hear Res, 50, 1300–1313.
Delis D. C., Kaplan E., Kramer J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan
Executive Function System: Examiners Manual. Psychological Corporation.
Dettman S. J., Dowell R. C., Choo D., Arnott W., Abrahams Y., Davis A., Dornan D., Leigh J., Constantinescu G., Cowan R., Briggs R. J. (2016). Long-term communication outcomes for children receiving
cochlear implants younger than 12 months: A multicenter study. Otol Neurotol, 37, e82–e95.
Di Stadio A., Dipietro L., Toffano R., Burgio F., De Lucia A., Ippolito V., Garofalo S., Ricci G., Martines F., Trabalzini F., Della Volpe A. (2018). Working memory function in children with single side deafness using a bone-anchored hearing implant: A case-control study. Audiol Neurootol, 23, 238–244.
Dunn L. M., Dunn L. M. (2005). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III-NL (Dutch Version). Pearson.
Firszt J. B., Holden L. K., Skinner M. W., Tobey E. A., Peterson A., Gaggl W., Runge-Samuelson C. L., Wackym P. A. (2004). Recognition of speech presented at soft to loud levels by adult cochlear implant recipients of three cochlear implant systems. Ear Hear, 25, 375–387.
Fletcher K. T., Horrell E. M. W., Ayugi J., Irungu C., Muthoka M., Creel L. M., Lester C., Bush M. L. (2018). The natural history and rehabilitative outcomes of hearing loss in congenital cytomegalovirus: A systematic review. Otol Neurotol, 39, 854–864.
Gentili N., Holwell A. (2011). Mental health in children with severe hearing impairment. Adv Psychiatr Treat, 17, 54–62.
Gillis S., Schaerlaekens A. (2000). Kindertaalverwerving: Een handboek voor het Nederlands. Martinus Nijhoff.
Hendrickson K., Oleson J., Walker E. (2021). School-age children adapt the dynamics of lexical competition in suboptimal listening conditions. Child Dev, 92, 638–649.
Hsu N. S., Novick J. M., Jaeggi S. M. (2014). The development and malleability of executive control abilities. Front Behav Neurosci, 8, 221.
Iwasaki S., Nishio S., Moteki H., Takumi Y., Fukushima K., Kasai N., Usami S. (2012).
Language development in Japanese children who receive cochlear implant and/or hearing aid. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 76, 433–438.
Jacobs E., Langereis M. C., Frijns J. H., Free R. H., Goedegebure A., Smits C., Stokroos R. J., Ariens-Meijer S. A., Mylanus E. A., Vermeulen A. M. (2016). Benefits of simultaneous bilateral cochlear implantation on verbal reasoning skills in prelingually deaf children. Res Dev Disabil, 58, 104–113.
Jones A., Atkinson J., Marshall C., Botting N., St Clair M. C., Morgan G. (2020). Expressive vocabulary predicts nonverbal
executive function: A 2-year longitudinal study of deaf and hearing children. Child Dev, 91, e400–e414.
Kingma A., van den Burg W. (2008). Drie parallelversies van de 15-woordentest voor kinderen. Universitair Medisch Centrum Groningen.
Koehlinger K. M., Van Horne A. J., Moeller M. P. (2013). Grammatical outcomes of 3- and 6-year-old children who are hard of hearing. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 56, 1701–1714.
Kral A., Kronenberger W. G., Pisoni D. B., O’Donoghue G. M. (2016). Neurocognitive factors in sensory restoration of early deafness: A connectome model. Lancet Neurol, 15, 610–621.
Kronenberger W. G., Beer J., Castellanos I., Pisoni D. B., Miyamoto R. T. (2014). Neurocognitive risk in children with
cochlear implants. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg, 140, 608–615.
Kronenberger W. G., Pisoni D. B., Henning S. C., Colson B. G. (2013). Executive functioning skills in long-term users of
cochlear implants: A case control study. J Pediatr Psychol, 38, 902–914.
Kronenberger W. G., Pisoni D. B., Henning S. C., Colson B. G., Hazzard L. M. (2011). Working memory training for children with
cochlear implants: A pilot study. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 54, 1182–1196.
Langereis M., Vermeulen A. (2013). Duidelijke meerwaarde van Cochleaire Implantatie – Het effect van CI op de taalontwikkeling, schoolse ontwikkeling en het welbevinden van ernstig slechthorende en dove kinderen [Added value of cochlear implantation - The effect of CI on
language development, school development and the well-being of severely hearing-impaired and deaf children]. Nederlands tijdschrift voor logopedie, 85, 16–24.
Leigh J., Dettman S., Dowell R., Briggs R. (2013). Communication development in children who receive a cochlear implant by 12 months of age. Otol Neurotol, 34, 443–450.
Leigh J. R., Dettman S. J., Dowell R. C. (2016). Evidence-based guidelines for recommending cochlear implantation for young children: Audiological criteria and optimizing age at implantation. Int J Audiol, 55(Suppl 2), S9–S18.
Lin F. R., Ferrucci L., Metter E. J., An Y., Zonderman A. B., Resnick S. M. (2011). Hearing loss and cognition in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neuropsychology, 25, 763–770.
Lovett R. E., Vickers D. A., Summerfield A. Q. (2015). Bilateral cochlear implantation for hearing-impaired children: Criterion of candidacy derived from an observational study. Ear Hear, 36, 14–23.
McKinney S. (2017). Cochlear implantation in children under 12 months of age. Curr Opin Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg, 25, 400–404.
Mitchell T. V., Quittner A. L. (1996). Multimethod study of attention and behavior problems in hearing-impaired children. J Clin Child Psychol, 25, 83–96.
Moon C., Lagercrantz H., Kuhl P. K. (2013).
Language experienced in utero affects vowel perception after birth: A two-country study. Acta Paediatr, 102, 156–160.
Nekes S. (2016). Equivalent Hearing Loss in Children (Dissertation). Köln.
Netten A. P., Rieffe C., Theunissen S. C., Soede W., Dirks E., Korver A. M., Konings S., Oudesluys-Murphy A. M., Dekker F. W., Frijns J. H.; DECIBEL Collaborative study group. (2015). Early identification:
Language skills and social functioning in deaf and hard of hearing preschool children. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 79, 2221–2226.
Neuenschwander R., Röthlisberger M., Cimeli P., Roebers C. M. (2012). How do different aspects of self-regulation predict successful adaptation to school? J Exp Child Psychol, 113, 353–371.
Pichora-Fuller M. K., Kramer S. E., Eckert M. A., Edwards B., Hornsby B. W., Humes L. E., Lemke U., Lunner T., Matthen M., Mackersie C. L., Naylor G., Phillips N. A., Richter M., Rudner M., Sommers M. S., Tremblay K. L., Wingfield A. (2016). Hearing impairment and cognitive energy: The Framework for Understanding Effortful Listening (FUEL). Ear Hear, 37(Suppl 1), 5S–27S.
Raaijmakers M. A., Smidts D. P., Sergeant J. A., Maassen G. H., Posthumus J. A., van Engeland H., Matthys W. (2008). Executive functions in preschool children with aggressive behavior: Impairments in inhibitory control. J Abnorm Child Psychol, 36, 1097–1107.
Ray A. (1964). L’examen clinique en psychologie [The Clinical Psychological Examination]. Presses Universitaires de France.
Riggs N. R., Jahromi L. B., Razza R. P., Dillworth-Bart J. E., Mueller U. (2006).
Executive function and the promotion of social-emotional competence. J Appl Dev Psychol, 27, 300–309.
Sarant J. (2012).
Cochlear implants in children: A review. InTech (online serial).
http:// www.intechopen.com/books/hearing-loss/cochlear-implants-inchildren-a-review.
Schoemaker K., Mulder H., Deković M., Matthys W. (2013). Executive functions in preschool children with externalizing behavior problems: A meta-analysis. J Abnorm Child Psychol, 41, 457–471.
Seewald R., Moodie S., Scollie S., Bagatto M. (2005). The DSL method for pediatric hearing instrument fitting: Historical perspective and current issues. Trends Amplif, 9, 145–157.
Senn T. E., Espy K. A., Kaufmann P. M. (2004). Using path analysis to understand
executive function organization in preschool children. Dev Neuropsychol, 26, 445–464.
Sharma A., Nash A. A., Dorman M. (2009). Cortical development, plasticity and re-organization in children with
cochlear implants. J Commun Disord, 42, 272–279.
Stevenson J., McCann D., Watkin P., Worsfold S., Kennedy C.; Hearing Outcomes Study Team. (2010). The relationship between
language development and behaviour problems in children with hearing loss. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 51, 77–83.
Stiles D. J., McGregor K. K., Bentler R. A. (2012). Vocabulary and working memory in children fit with
hearing aids. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 55, 154–167.
Stroop J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol, 18, 643–662.
Theunissen S. C., Rieffe C., Kouwenberg M., De Raeve L. J., Soede W., Briaire J. J., Frijns J. H. (2014). Behavioral problems in school-aged hearing-impaired children: The influence of sociodemographic, linguistic, and medical factors. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 23, 187–196.
Theunissen S. C., Rieffe C., Soede W., Briaire J. J., Ketelaar L., Kouwenberg M., Frijns J. H. (2015). Symptoms of psychopathology in hearing-impaired children. Ear Hear, 36, e190–e198.
Tomblin J. B., Harrison M., Ambrose S. E., Walker E. A., Oleson J. J., Moeller M. P. (2015).
Language outcomes in young children with mild to severe hearing loss. Ear Hear, 36(Suppl 1), 76S–91S.
Tsao F. M., Liu H. M., Kuhl P. K. (2004). Speech perception in infancy predicts
language development in the second year of life: A longitudinal study. Child Dev, 75, 1067–1084.
van den Burg W., Kingma A. (1999). Performance of 225 Dutch school children on Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT): Parallel test-retest reliabilities with an interval of 3 months and normative data. Arch Clin Neuropsychol, 14, 545–559.
van Eldik T., Treffers P. D., Veerman J. W., Verhulst F. C. (2004). Mental health problems of deaf Dutch children as indicated by parents’ responses to the child behavior checklist. Am Ann Deaf, 148, 390–395.
van Gent T., Goedhart A. W., Hindley P. A., Treffers P. D. (2007). Prevalence and correlates of psychopathology in a sample of deaf adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 48, 950–958.
Wake M., Hughes E. K., Poulakis Z., Collins C., Rickards F. W. (2004). Outcomes of children with mild-profound congenital hearing loss at 7 to 8 years: A population study. Ear Hear, 25, 1–8.
Wake M., Poulakis Z., Hughes E. K., Carey-Sargeant C., Rickards F. W. (2005). Hearing impairment: A population study of age at diagnosis, severity, and
language outcomes at 7-8 years. Arch Dis Child, 90, 238–244.
Wallace G. L., Peng C. S., Williams D. (2017). Interfering with inner speech selectively disrupts problem solving and is linked with real-world executive functioning. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 60, 3456–3460.
Watson D. R., Titterington J., Henry A., Toner J. G. (2007). Auditory sensory memory and working memory processes in children with normal hearing and
cochlear implants. Audiol Neurootol, 12, 65–76.
WHO. (2020). Prevention of Blindness and Deafness - Grades of Hearing Impairment. World Health Organization.
Wong C. L., Ching T. Y. C., Cupples L., Button L., Leigh G., Marnane V., Whitfield J., Gunnourie M., Martin L. (2017). Psychosocial development in 5-year-old children with hearing loss using
hearing aids or
cochlear implants. Trends Hear, 21, 2331216517710373.
Yang H. M., Hsieh Y. J., Wu J. L. (2012). Speech recognition performance under noisy conditions of children with hearing loss. Clin Exp Otorhinolaryngol, 5(Suppl 1), S73–S75.