The Quest for Ecological Validity in Hearing Science: What It Is, Why It Matters, and How to Advance It : Ear and Hearing

Journal Logo

Eriksholm Workshop: Ecological Validity

The Quest for Ecological Validity in Hearing Science: What It Is, Why It Matters, and How to Advance It

Keidser, Gitte1,2; Naylor, Graham3; Brungart, Douglas S.4; Caduff, Andreas5; Campos, Jennifer6; Carlile, Simon7,8; Carpenter, Mark G.9; Grimm, Giso10; Hohmann, Volker10; Holube, Inga11; Launer, Stefan12; Lunner, Thomas1; Mehra, Ravish13; Rapport, Frances14; Slaney, Malcolm15; Smeds, Karolina16

Author Information
Ear and Hearing 41():p 5S-19S, November/December 2020. | DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000944
  • Open

Abstract

RATIONALE, SCOPE, AND APPROACH

Over the last few decades, hearing devices have evolved from straightforward amplifiers to highly sophisticated devices which respond to distinct environments to provide contextually relevant benefits to the wearer. Meanwhile, whilst many diagnostic and evaluation protocols have been computerized and automated, there has not been corresponding development in the procedures used for assessing a person’s hearing ability and evaluating the benefit of increasingly complex hearing-related interventions. As early as 1988, the Working Group on Speech Understanding and Aging (“Speech understanding and aging. Working Group on Speech Understanding and Aging. Committee on Hearing, Bioacoustics, and Biomechanics, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, National Research Council”, 1988) concluded that current audiometric tests were ineffective in determining a person’s real-life hearing problems and benefit with hearing devices, pointing to the need to: (1) deliver test environments more comparable with the dynamic and reverberant real-world environments; (2) capture comprehension; and (3) consider cognitive processes involved in speech understanding. As shown in Figure 1, some studies concerning the use of more realistic environments or tasks in the research design were published before and during the 1990s. However, the last decade has seen a steep increase in such publications. The rise coincides with digital hearing devices becoming fully established and with the growing number of device features addressing varied, everyday demands. These device developments have prompted industry-based and academic researchers to, once more, point out the lack of evolution of the tests that continued to be used clinically and in research laboratories (Edwards 2007; Jerger 2009). The underlying problem was again suggested to be a lack of realism afforded by traditional test setups and tasks. Around the same time, Neuhoff (2004) advocated for an “ecological psychoacoustics,” arguing that psychoacoustic investigations were traditionally limited to understanding the reaction of the auditory system to sounds, ignoring factors such as perception and cognition that would drive listening behaviors when detecting and recognizing sounds in the real world. Following this publication, a similar sharp increase in publications and conference presentations concerning “ecological validity” of the research setting, stimuli, or outcome can be seen (cf. Fig. 1).

F1
Fig. 1.:
Number of publications found on PubMed when using the combined search terms [(ecological OR ecologically) AND (valid OR validity) AND (hearing OR audiology)]—full line—and [(realistic) AND (environment OR task OR method) AND (hearing OR audiology)]—broken line—along a timeline showing 5-year intervals post 1990.

Publications on hearing-related research concerning ecological validity have primarily reacted to calls for a more real-life approach by either (1) introducing novel and sophisticated test environments and tasks into laboratory studies, thus aiming to better replicate real-life listening situations (e.g., Grimm et al. 2016; Weller et al. 2016; Coene et al. 2018; Devesse et al. 2020) or (2) introducing more context-sensitive forms of measurement into field studies (e.g., Gatehouse 1999; Wu et al. 2015; Wolters et al. 2016). While these studies have contributed greatly to moving the field forward in terms of making investigations more realistic in one way or another, they lack a shared conceptual basis, with the meaning of ecological validity and the purpose of striving for it in hearing-related research remaining unclear. For example, the term “ecological validity” has been used to indicate both that the experimental context was more naturalistic (e.g., Hadley et al. 2019; Zeni et al. 2020) and that the approach had more face validity; that is, provided the real-life information the researcher intended to obtain (e.g., Devesse et al. 2018; Decruy et al. 2019), and different motivations for adding more realism to research protocols have been proposed.

To promote a more unified and streamlined research effort in the future, the aims of the sixth Eriksholm Workshop on Ecologically Valid Assessments of Hearing and Hearing Devices were, in broad terms, to:

  • - define the term “ecological validity” as it applies to hearing-related research,
  • - outline purpose/s of striving for more ecological validity in this field,
  • - examine and discuss the variables and phenomena likely to affect the level of ecological validity in research studies assessing a person’s hearing ability (with/without hearing devices),
  • - summarize the current state of knowledge regarding the general level of ecological validity of various types of studies, and
  • - identify knowledge gaps and research priorities.

This article is structured as follows. The introductory sections motivate and present the consolidated definition of ecological validity and purposes of striving to improve it. Then, we examine and discuss ecological validity in different types of studies (laboratory, field, and hybrid), before presenting the state of the art in these domains, as we perceive it, and considering how one might evaluate the level of ecological validity in a study (published or in planning). After a brief discussion of the effect a holistic approach to research designs may have on ecological validity, the final section lists knowledge gaps and future research priorities.

DEFINITION OF ECOLOGICAL VALIDITY

To draw meaningful conclusions from any research study, it is important to consider the validity of the study results. Most researchers are familiar with the concepts of internal and external validity, with the former examining whether a research study was designed, conducted, and analyzed appropriately to answer its research questions and the latter examining whether findings of a research study can be generalized to other contexts. The concept of ecological validity is less familiar but has, in particular, a long history in the field of psychology, where it is widely considered a concept that examines to what extent the results of a research study are related to, or predict, outcomes in situations occurring in everyday life. In those terms, it is generally thought of as a type of external validity. In psychology, the degree of ecological validity of a study is assumed to be closely tied to three methodological dimensions; the nature of a study’s setting, types of stimuli implemented, and type of response used (Lewkowicz 2001). According to Schmuckler (2001), the early debate on ecological validity centered, in particular, around the impact the experimental setting or environment had on the research (Brunswik 1943; Lewin 1943). The debate resulted in the following classical, albeit narrow, definition of ecological validity: “ecological validity refers to the extent to which the environment experienced by the subjects in a scientific investigation has the properties it is supposed or assumed to have by the experimenter” (Bronfenbrenner 1977). Brunswik (1943) also implied the importance of making the stimuli and response more realistic. The former point was echoed by Gibson (1960), and elaborated on by Neisser (1976), who stressed that real-life inputs typically consist of information that is temporally and spatially extended as well as multimodal. As pointed out by Schmuckler (2001), these three dimensions (setting, stimuli, and response) do not constitute an exhaustive list of factors involved in increasing the ecological validity of research but offer a potential starting point in a discussion and definition of ecological validity.

Hearing loss is a chronic health condition and, as such, is often contextualized within the World Health Organization’s International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health (WHO-ICF) framework (WHO 2001) (e.g., Kiessling et al. 2003; Lind et al. 2016; Illum & Gradel 2017; Lersilp et al. 2018; Jaiswal et al. 2019; Manchaiah et al. 2019). Similarly, hearing research findings, management strategies, and outcome measures are increasingly interpreted within this framework (e.g., Psarros & Love 2016; Ali et al. 2017; Alfakir et al. 2019; Convery et al. 2019). The WHO-ICF framework categorizes the function and disability of a person into three interrelated levels referring to the body (structure or function), the whole person (activity), and the whole person in a social context (participation). Workshop participants felt that ecological validity in hearing research could therefore usefully be related to the WHO-ICF framework. So, inspired by the definition of ecological validity offered by closely related psychological sciences, while integrating the WHO-ICF framework that is well established within the healthcare domain, we offer the following definition of the ecological validity concept when applied to hearing-related research: “In hearing science, ecological validity refers to the degree to which research findings reflect real-life hearing-related function, activity, or participation.”

It is worth noting that ecological validity is not a binary phenomenon that is either present or absent from a research study, but each study presents a certain level of ecological validity. As with the concepts of external and internal validity, the assessment of a study’s level of ecological validity is ultimately based on subjective judgment, so the concept of ecological validity cannot be used to provide comprehensive objective criteria for experimental designs.

PURPOSES OF STRIVING FOR MORE ECOLOGICAL VALIDITY, AND POTENTIAL BENEFICIARIES

Studies to date that have aimed at achieving more ecologically valid findings seem to have been driven mainly by the notion that many of the established methodologies used to assess hearing and hearing devices lack sufficient realism to produce adequately meaningful findings about a person’s hearing function, activity, or participation in real life. Such shortcomings in the state of the art could have implications for many stakeholders. Meanwhile, it is relatively rare to find explicit mention of why a lack of realism might be important. For these reasons, two subgoals of the workshop were:

  • - to obtain a clearer picture of why striving for greater ecological validity of research findings is desirable (i.e., the purpose(s)) and
  • - to identify who will benefit from those efforts (i.e., the beneficiaries).

Overall, four different purposes of striving for greater ecological validity in hearing-related research emerged that participants agreed on. They are:

A (Understanding): To better understand the role of hearing in everyday life.

B (Development): To support the development of improved hearing-related procedures and interventions.

C (Assessment): To facilitate improved methods for assessing and predicting the ability of people and systems to accomplish specific real-world hearing-related tasks.

D (Integration and Individualization): To enable more integrated and individualized hearing healthcare.

Purpose A expresses a need for a better understanding of how people use their hearing when undertaking everyday activities in their environments, and how this is affected by impaired body function. In the case of impaired hearing, this means understanding the kinds of activity limitations impaired hearing causes and how they manifest, as well as the behavior of people with hearing loss and their communication partners when challenged in everyday situations. Using the definition of fundamental, applied, and translational research suggested by Cooksey (2006), this need is in the domain of fundamental research.

Purpose B expresses a need for evaluation protocols that enable meaningful assessment of the individual’s real-life hearing ability and benefit from hearing interventions (including devices, communication strategies, design of built environments, and other means). This will enhance the quality of evidence used to support development of and justification for more advanced hearing-related diagnosis, rehabilitation and screening procedures as well as interventions, and lead to improved hearing-related quality of life for those with unmet hearing needs. This need is in the domain of applied (Cooksey 2006) research.

Purpose C expresses a need for more meaningful criteria to be established for the evaluation of, for example, eligibility for hearing-related benefit (e.g., subsidized intervention, insurance pay-outs), or ability to perform hearing-dependent tasks that are critical within certain professions (e.g., military, police force). Such criteria will help to maximize the ability of people with hearing impairment to participate in society. This need is in the domain of translational (Cooksey 2006) research.

Finally, Purpose D expresses a need for a more person-centered approach to understanding disability and needs, facilitating optimal intervention by considering the individual’s general health, social connectedness, healthcare environment, and other overarching factors. This implies a broad view of the ecosystem and how it affects the individual’s hearing health. This need is also in the domain of translational research, and concerns the removal of barriers to participation, but in terms of connected health and care systems beyond those solely related to hearing.

As for who will benefit from more ecologically valid assessments, the individuals and groups identified by workshop participants were consolidated into eight categories: (1) person with hearing needs (who may or may not have a hearing impairment); (2) people in immediate daily interaction with a person with hearing needs; (3) hearing-care professionals; (4) hearing researchers; (5) funders and policymakers; (6) product developers and marketers; (7) creators (e.g., film makers and designers of interactive games); and (8) designers of the built environment. Table 1 lists the categories of beneficiaries identified and to what extent each would likely benefit from the pursuit of greater ecological validity by way of each of the four purposes outlined earlier. Of note is that the pursuit of greater ecological validity for purposes A and B is beneficial for all groups. Also, the two most crucial groups, namely patients and clinicians, both stand to benefit from the pursuit of greater ecological validity for all four purposes. As exemplified by the articles in this supplement, it is our hope that future publications in this area will clearly state the purposes for which their work advances knowledge and development so that information and progress can more easily be consolidated and tracked, respectively.

TABLE 1. - The likely beneficiaries of pursuing more ecologically valid outcomes for each of the purposes A (Understanding), B (Development), C (Assessment), and D (Integration and individualization)
Beneficiary Purpose A Purpose B Purpose C Purpose D
Person with hearing need Yes Yes Yes Yes
People in immediate daily interaction with a person with hearing need Yes Yes Yes
Hearing-care professionals Yes Yes Yes Yes
Hearing researchers Yes Yes Yes
Funders and policymakers Yes Yes Yes
Product developers and marketers Yes Yes
Creators Yes Yes
Designers of built environments Yes Yes
See text for a full description of the four purposes.

At this point, we would like to emphasize that it is not the purpose of this article to suggest that all future studies should aim for a high level of ecological validity. There is nothing intrinsically superior about experiments that are more ecologically valid; rather, the methodological approach to a study should always be driven by the research objectives (see also Lewkowicz 2001).

DEFINITIONS OF PRIMARY CONCEPTS

Some concepts are referred to repeatedly throughout this article and across the articles making up this special issue. To promote comprehension and consistency, the most important concepts are collected and provided (in alphabetical order) with definitions in Table 2.

TABLE 2. - The definitions of primary concepts used frequently in this special issue
Term Description
Ecological validity The degree to which research findings reflect real-life hearing-related function, activity, or participation. See further description on pp. 7S.
Everyday life For a given individual, the subset of all real-life situations that are experienced with some significant frequency or have some significant importance.
Field study A study in which the principal data-collection environment is each individual participant’s everyday life, and in which primary stimuli and tasks are not controlled by an experimenter. See further description on pp. 8S.
Hybrid study A study in which the experimenter possesses control over one or more (but not all) of either the environment, the stimuli, or the participants’ task.
Laboratory study A study in which participants are removed from their everyday-life environment and placed in an artificial one for the purpose of exposing them to controlled environments, stimuli and/or tasks, and obtaining predetermined outcome measures. See further description on pp. 8S.
Outcome domain Any distinct aspect of function that could be assessed to determine whether an intervention has worked (Hall et al. 2018).
Outcome measure A measure, intended to reflect variation in a specified outcome domain, obtained through a replicable measurement procedure.
Participant task A goal that a study participant might try to achieve. The task may be explicitly instructed by the experimenter or assumed by the participant.
Real-life or real-world Situations that are not controlled by an experimenter.
Terms in italics are themselves defined elsewhere in the table.

TYPES OF STUDIES

It is customary to label research studies as either “laboratory” or “field” studies, and this distinction also inspired the structure of the workshop sessions. However, while workshop participants could agree that laboratory and field studies could be distinguished by their test environment (implemented versus real-world) and the level of control of variables of primary interest (high versus low), it proved impossible to reach agreement about what constituted a general border between the two types of studies. Instead it was decided to consider laboratory and field studies to fall, in their purest form, at opposite extremes on a continuum, with any study in between being referred to as a “hybrid” study. Within each type of study, data may be collected and interpreted using quantitative or qualitative methods, or a mixture of the two. Ecological validity in each of these three types of studies (laboratory, field, and hybrid) will be considered in the following sections of this article.

For this purpose, the “model” of a laboratory study assumed here is one in which:

  • - some situation in which human hearing function is believed to play a role is emulated in a laboratory
  • - participants are instructed to carry out some activity in the laboratory setup
  • - the activity encompasses a “task,” which may or may not be made explicit to the participant
  • - the experimenter controls all independent variables of primary importance in the experiment
  • - the experimenter is interested in one or more outcome domains and implements outcome measures accordingly (e.g., speech recognition score, task completion time, eye gaze statistics)
  • - if relevant, assessment of an intervention is assumed to be made by comparing outcome with the intervention versus without, or versus an alternative intervention.

Whereas the “model” of a field study assumed here is one in which:

  • - participants are instructed to carry out their normal daily activities in the usual manner, despite any additional burdens generated by participation in the study
  • - the only burdens imposed by the experimenter are for the purposes of monitoring activity or environment, or for eliciting participant impressions of situations and corresponding outcomes
  • - apart from selecting participants and setting design or intervention parameters (e.g., duration of field observation, hearing device operation), the experimenter controls no independent variables in the experiment
  • - the experimenter may attempt to monitor independent variables as they occur
  • - the experimenter is interested in one or more outcome domains and implements outcome measures accordingly (e.g., Ecological Momentary Assessment [EMA—Shiffman et al. 2008; Galvez et al. 2012], diaries, device usage statistics).

A hybrid study is a study in which the experimenter has control of some, but not all experimental variables. Commonly that would mean either low control of task, environment, or stimuli in a study conducted in the laboratory, or high control of task, environment, or stimuli in a study conducted in the field. In reality, pure forms of laboratory and field studies are rarely carried out; most studies possess some characteristics of a hybrid study.

ECOLOGICAL VALIDITY IN LABORATORY, FIELD, AND HYBRID STUDIES

In classical reductionist experiment design, the goal of the researcher is to evaluate the influence of systematic changes in one or more carefully controlled independent variables on the values of one or more carefully measured dependent variables. To the greatest extent possible, any variability that might result from variables other than the independent variables should be eliminated from the experimental environment. However, the cost of eliminating this variability is the risk that the particular combination of values for the independent variables selected for the experiment might not generalize to the relevant real-life scenarios that served as the underlying motivation for conducting the experiment.

When considering the effect of independent variables on ecological validity, it is important to distinguish between outcome domains (e.g., speech intelligibility, listening effort, affective response, interactivity) and the outcome measures used to assess them (e.g., speech reception threshold, pupil dilation, self-report, behavioral synchrony). The level of control or variation in independent variables of the design may affect the phenomena elicited in the outcome domain of interest, and thereby the ecological validity of the experiment. In addition, the method of measuring in the outcome domain of interest may itself affect the phenomena elicited. In the following, we first examine each of these aspects separately in the context of laboratory studies as defined earlier. Then, we describe the phenomena affecting ecological validity in field studies as defined earlier, before discussing a few design features characteristic of hybrid studies.

How Independent Variables May Affect Ecological Validity in Laboratory Studies

In this section, we attempt to provide an overview of independent variables that may be important to consider in relation to their effect on the level of ecological validity of a laboratory study and consider how interactions between these variables and outcome domains may influence the ecological validity of study outcomes. Table 3 lists common independent variables, along with others identified during the workshop as being of some potential importance in hearing research. Brief examples or comments are provided to contextualize the variables, and the variables are categorized into five methodological dimensions. Working groups at the workshop initially produced widely differing categorizations. Subsequently, we attempted to rationalize and simplify these dimensions, specifically by referring to the three dimensions (setting, stimuli, and response) identified by Lewkowicz (2001). However, these three were not found to adequately encompass the diversity of variables identified for hearing research, nor to be entirely helpful distinctions. For example, individual variables frequently mentioned during workshop discussions were not clearly represented in the three dimensions identified by Lewkowicz. As another example, workshop discussions revealed that “setting” was too broad a term to adequately distinguish between variables related to the presentation of stimuli and the context of participation. The final dimensions of independent variables that participants reached a consensus on and their approximate mappings to those of Lewkowicz are:

TABLE 3. - A list of commonly used independent variables in hearing science, with explanatory notes, grouped into the methodological dimensions of Sources of stimuli, Environment, Context of participation, Task, and Individual
Methodological dimension Independent variables
Sources of stimuli Characteristics of stimulus sources; e.g., speech/other, diversity, familiarity, continuous vs. events
Characteristics of stimulus materials; e.g., monotonous, dynamic, neutral, emotional
For multimodal stimuli, which modalities are subjected to controlled manipulation; e.g., audio, visual, tactile
How other people are represented; disembodied voice -> real people axis. Includes potential for “uncanny valley” effects
Environment (presentation of stimuli) Acoustic field; e.g., levels, SNRs, spatial fidelity, size of eventual sweet spot
Interaction of environment and hearing devices; degree to which the reproduced field (sound or other signal modalities) provokes the same device behavior as the real field would
Incorporation of dynamic aspects; e.g., movement of sources
Modalities included; e.g., visual, inertial
Context of participation Participant preparation; e.g., instructions, explanation provided for the purpose of the experiment, familiarization/training sequence
Semantic associations of the situation being simulated for the participant; e.g., does the participant ever take part in such a situation, does the participant have negative associations with it (“I always fail”)
Motivation to take part; e.g., incentive, reimbursement, mode of recruitment
Familiarity with the lab and its people and/or methods; e.g., regular “semiprofessional” participants or patients in clinical routine
Psychological/physiological state at time of experiment; e.g., has the participant recently experienced a traumatic event or consumed psychoactive substances, does s/he have an important appointment later today
Task Nature of task; e.g., speech communication vs. environmental monitoring/detection
Nature of task if speech; e.g., repeat, recall, comprehend
Complexity; e.g., single vs. multiple tasks
Degree of constraint on route to task fulfillment; continuum from e.g., “press the button every time food is mentioned” to, e.g., “find out whether you have any acquaintances in common”.
Exploratory movement; degree to which body/head/eye movements by the participant (a) are allowed, and (b) produce realistic changes in the stimuli
Interaction; participant as observer/reporter vs. interactor
Predictability; e.g., limited response options, pattern of stimuli presentations
Distractors; e.g., visuals and audio unrelated to the explicit task
Individual Personality; e.g., open, agreeable, extroverted, neurotic
Hearing health; e.g., type, degree and configuration of hearing loss, tinnitus, hyperacusis
Sensory, cognitive, motor abilities; e.g., visual acuity, working memory, balance
Mental health; e.g., depressed or anxious.
Competency in task language; e.g., native vs. non-native, literacy level
Cultural background; e.g., ethnic, socioeconomic or religious factors affecting compliance, social desirability bias
Occupation/skillsets/training; educational and skill levels and educational attainment
Disease burden; e.g., frailty, multimorbidity
The labels used for each independent variable in future tables are shown in italics.

  • - Sources of stimuli [stimuli, setting];
  • - Environment (presentation of stimuli) [setting, stimuli];
  • - Context of participation [setting];
  • - Task [response];
  • - Individual [N/A].

The list of independent variables in Table 3, although long, is not exhaustive. Note that “Age” does not appear in the list. This is because age as such is not a variable that directly influences outcomes. Variables typically mediated by age (e.g., cognitive abilities, sensory abilities) are included explicitly, which is a more parsimonious approach, but requires one to make correct associations between age and its likely consequences. Similarly, “Demographics” is absent, as the most common components of this variable that are expected to directly affect the ecological validity of outcomes (e.g., education/occupation, cultural background, and disease burden) are explicitly included in the list.

In Table 4, we have for each independent variable and in relation to four example outcome domains (speech intelligibility, listening effort, affective response, and interactivity) indicated our best guess as to the extent to which the variable affects the ecological validity of outcomes in that outcome domain. The symbols in the interacting cells indicate: X = very likely (based on research or logic), ? = might, but not enough research to state, and o = probably not (mostly based on logic, not research).

TABLE 4. - For four example outcome domains, the independent variables are rated to show their suggested effect on the ecological validity of measures obtained in that domain
Independent variables Example outcome domains
Speech recognition Listening effort Interactivity Affective response
Sources of stimuli
 Stimulus sources X X X X
 Stimulus materials X X X X
 Multimodal stimuli ? ? X X
 Other people ? X X X
Environment
 Acoustic field X X ? ?
 Interaction of env. and hearing devices X X o ?
 Dynamic aspects X X X ?
 Modalities X X X X
Context of participation
 Participant preparation X X X X
 Semantic associations X X X X
 Motivation ? X X X
 Familiarity ? X X X
 Psychological/physiological state ? X X X
Task
 Nature of task N/A ? ? ?
 Nature of task if speech X X X X
 Complexity X X ? ?
 Degree of constraint ? X X X
 Exploratory movement X X X o
 Interaction ? X X X
 Predictability X X X X
 Distractors X X ? X
Individual
 Personality o X X ?
 Hearing health X X X X
 Sensory, cognitive, motor abilities X X X ?
 Mental health ? X X X
 Competency in task language X X X o
 Cultural background X X ? ?
 Occupation/skillsets/training ? X ? X
 Disease burden ? X ? X
X = very likely, ? = might, o = probably not.

Looking across the variables and their symbols listed for each dimension, it would appear that the ecological validity of outcomes in the domain of speech recognition would be particularly affected by variables related to the environment, whereas the ecological validity of outcomes in the domain of affective response would be particularly affected by variables related to sources of stimuli and context of participation. It would further seem that if a researcher wishes to manipulate and/or comment on the ecological validity of a study that aims to investigate listening effort, variables in all five methodological dimensions should be carefully considered. It can also be deduced from the table that there is still a lot to be learned about the potential effect of the context of participation on the ecological validity of outcomes in the domain of speech recognition and of environment in the domain of affective response. The outcome domains shown in Table 4 are given merely as examples to show how Table 3 may be used to facilitate the assessment of risks to ecological validity posed by any given study design, with any particular outcome domain.

How the Measurement of Dependent Variables May Affect Ecological Validity in Laboratory Studies

Here we examine the second set of interactions, namely between the method of outcome measurement (i.e., dependent variable) and the level of ecological validity of the phenomena elicited in the corresponding outcome domain. Note that we are not concerned here with the quality of a measurement (reliability, precision, bias, etc.) since that is an issue of internal validity, not external or ecological validity. Rather we aim to illustrate how the method of measurement itself may affect the phenomena one wishes to observe.

It is customary to categorize outcome measures according to the modality they make use of, namely behavior, physiology, and self-report. It may be possible in principle to measure any outcome domain via any of these three modalities; therefore, the observations made below are not related to any specific outcome domains.

Behavioral Measurement

Participant behavior intrinsic to the experimental task may be used as a direct source of data. Examples of this include:

  • - speaking in an unscripted conversation between two participants, where derived metrics (e.g., turn-taking statistics, dialog repair events, voice stress) are used as the dependent variables,
  • - gesture and body movement, when the participant is not instructed to move, but not prevented from doing so, may be used in the same way as described earlier for speech, and
  • - with an experimental task that is multi-faceted (e.g., in a virtual reality [VR] setup, crossing the road while conversing with a partner), trade-offs between subtasks may be used.

In itself, this type of measurement poses no threat to ecological validity. However, it is possible that participants who are aware that their behavior is being monitored may alter their behavior, whereby ecological validity would be compromised.

Behavior extrinsic to the task may also be utilized as an indirect source of data. Examples include:

  • - speaking to report task response (e.g., repeating the sentence heard, judging sense/nonsense of heard phrase, reporting direction of heard sound) and
  • - gesture and body movement to directly represent task response (e.g., pointing in the direction from which a voice was heard).

This type of measurement poses a substantial threat to ecological validity, as the participant is required to alternate between in-task (e.g., perception, reasoning, interrogation of environment) and out-of-task (reporting) cognition and behaviors.

Whether the measurement modality is intrinsic or extrinsic to the participant task, requirements for an intrusive technical apparatus can affect behavior. If that behavior is integral to performing the task, ecological validity may be compromised. For example, a body-worn apparatus that is weighty or constrictive may cause participants’ movement to be constrained and unrepresentative. On the other hand, if (for example) movement analysis is carried out via analysis of video recordings, no such problems occur.

Physiological Measurement

Physiological measures offer an attractive route to capturing participant responses with a high level of ecological validity, as they are considered fundamental indicators of body state and function. However, in most cases, substantial threats to ecological validity arise from the technical equipment needed to acquire the data. This is because intrusive equipment, such as mounted electrodes and wearable motion trackers, may well affect behavior, similarly to what was noted earlier for behavioral measurements. Likewise, in the well-known “white coat effect,” mere attendance at a physician’s office for a blood pressure test has the effect of raising one’s blood pressure (Parati & Mancia 2003). Similarly, “biofeedback” techniques demonstrate that (at least in the presence of feedback) a degree of volitional control is possible over physiological markers such as heart rate and blood pressure (Williamson & Blanchard 1979). All these types of effects may in principle reduce the ecological validity of physiological measurement outcomes. Meanwhile, note that the often massive moment-to-moment variation in physiological variables that occurs during natural behavior is not in itself a threat to ecological validity, but if behavior or circumstances are artificially constrained in order to minimize the resulting physiological “noise,” then the ecological validity of the resulting experiment is again at risk.

Self-Report

Here we regard self-report (e.g., “how easy was it to follow the talker?”) as an additional task, extrinsic to the experimental task (e.g., “try to follow the talker”). Adding an extra task like this implies a high risk to ecological validity. The exception is retrospective self-report, for instance, where a self-report is made after a block of trials or as part of a paired comparison paradigm, thereby effectively becoming a task of its own, distinct from the experimental task. However, even in such a design, the participant may, during execution of the task of interest, allocate resources to gathering impressions for use during the self-report task or take on an artificial “self-observation” behavior.

Factors That May Affect Ecological Validity in Field Studies

Field studies of hearing and hearing devices are universally justified on the basis that their results will reflect the everyday lived experience of the participants, or in other words, that they provide a high level of ecological validity. While it is undoubtedly the case that field studies are “born with” much greater ecological validity than laboratory studies, the workshop participants were unanimous in the view that studies are not guaranteed to possess adequate ecological validity merely by virtue of being carried out in participants’ everyday life and that significant risks to ecological validity are present in many forms of field study. In this section, we describe the sources and nature of such risks.

In contrast to the earlier discussion relating to laboratory studies, almost no threat to ecological validity arises from design aspects concerning the control of independent variables, because no attempt is made to control these variables, except insofar as participant selection represents experimenter control of “Individual” factors as defined in Table 3. Thus, except for selection bias (see later), all the threats to ecological validity in field studies arise from the requirements of measuring dependent variables and monitoring independent variables.

In the following, we list and describe phenomena that can occur in field studies and may pose a threat to a study’s ecological validity. These phenomena can be seen from two perspectives: one being distortions of behavior, the other being biased sampling of normal behavior patterns.

Distortions of Participant Behavior Within Everyday Life

  • - Reactivity, which refers to a change on an outcome measure of direct interest to the study, caused by a participant’s conscious engagement with the construct being measured. Note that this might also occur in laboratory studies, although it is less likely.
  • - Temporary avoidance of certain situations. This could be to avoid feeling socially awkward (e.g., staying home from a party due to carrying extra equipment or the need to divert attention away from the situation to execute self-reports) or to avoid feelings of noncompliance (e.g., skipping a swimming lesson because it coincides with a time the participant is expected to execute a self-report).
  • - Deliberately seeking out certain situations that are normally never (or less frequently) experienced by the participant in question. This may be done to “test out” the study apparatus, because these situations pose special problems for the participant, or because, having seen these situations listed in self-report outcome measures, the participant feels a duty to experience them.
  • - Temporary changes in behavior, within the participant’s everyday situations. This includes any behaviors that the participant believes are required to fulfill study tasks correctly.
  • - Sometimes the extra equipment (e.g., an external signal processing unit or an assistive listening device) and tasks carried by a participant elicit a positive form of curiosity from their acquaintances. This positive reinforcement may encourage abnormal behavior, for example, conversations about the study, or demonstrations of the equipment by the participant. While such conversations may be very similar to those normally occurring (e.g., showing a friend a new gadget), it is nevertheless a conversation that would not have taken place in the absence of the study.
  • - In the case of studies in which the researcher is placed in situ within the participant’s everyday life (e.g., Wildemuth 2017), the participant may exhibit abnormal (for that person) patterns of behavior and/or situations, due to a perceived need to conform to certain imagined norms.

Biased Sampling of Everyday Life

Sampling bias can take several forms:

  • - Undersampling of situations in which the participant is unable or unwilling to respond (e.g., situations of high cognitive or social load).
  • - Oversampling of situations in which responding is free of negative social consequences (e.g., self-initiated self-reports during quiet time alone). This may occur, for example, if participants perceive a requirement to achieve a certain number of responses per day.
  • - Oversampling of situations that participants judge to be “interesting,” based on their perception of the experimenter’s aims.
  • - Oversampling of situations in which participants find it easier to comply with study tasks because it is easier to make judgments (e.g., when a contrast in hearing device settings has a clear effect on successful communication).

Selection bias is primarily driven by issues of who is willing to take part in and able to comply with such studies (willing to bear whatever burden is imposed, and able to behave and respond appropriately), that is, a self-selection bias. This means the situations being sampled may not be representative of the intended target population, even if the ecological validity is high at the level of individual participants. Of course, selection bias is also present for laboratory studies, but probably operates to generate differently biased samples of participants.

Ecological Validity in Hybrid Studies

As defined earlier, hybrid studies combine design features of pure laboratory and field studies. Hence a combination of the threats to ecological validity discussed in the previous sections would apply to these studies. For illustrative purposes, in this section, we present and discuss several study design features that are common in hearing science literature and that would be categorized as “hybrid” according to the present scheme.

One design feature concerns directed behavior in the field; that is, when a test participant is asked to do very specific tasks in their everyday environments and/or to perform tasks only in predefined everyday environments. An example would be a study in which participants are instructed to perform systematic comparisons of different devices or device modes during their normal daily activities. Although data is collected in participants’ everyday environments, such a study does not qualify as a pure field study because the participant is required by the experimenter to consciously manipulate the study’s independent variables. This is an issue that could only be overcome if the hearing device itself was programmed to enter different modes at different times, without informing the user. In this example, the level of ecological validity of outcome measures can be affected both by distortions of participant behavior within their everyday life, and by interaction with independent variables in the methodological dimensions of “task” and “context of participation” (Table 3).

Another general design feature of hybrid studies involves participants making retrospective reports of experiences from the real world while situated in a research milieu (lab, office, or clinic). Data may be collected via questionnaires, interviews, or focus groups. Although the data typically refer to unrestricted behaviors in the field, they do not qualify as pure field data as participants are removed from the real-life environments at the time they report their experiences. The data are not pure laboratory data either as the experimenter did not have the necessary control of any independent variables that could be interacting with the experiences referred to. In such designs, the level of ecological validity of outcome measures can be affected by biased sampling of everyday life situations and the outcome method measurement (self-report). In addition, retrospection biases can in this case negatively affect the ecological validity of outcome measures.

A final design feature that deserves a mention in this section is the special case of making self-reports with reference to hypothetical situations. Many popular questionnaires, whether self-administered at home or administered by the experimenter in a research milieu, ask test participants to imagine how they would perform in hypothetical real-life situations. In this case, participants may during data collection be dislocated from the environment in question not only in a physical sense but also mentally if the situation is unfamiliar to them. The extra level of “dislocation” is a further threat to ecological validity of outcome measures in this case, beside the factors mentioned with the previous example.

ECOLOGICAL VALIDITY IN HEARING RESEARCH: STATE OF THE ART

In this section, we aim to evaluate the level of ecological validity currently achieved in state-of-the-art test scenarios. Before going into detail, the question is considered from a high-level perspective, namely the WHO ICF categories of Body function, Activity, and Participation (WHO 2001). We propose that laboratory studies conforming to the model described earlier are best suited to studying the Activity category (e.g., behavior). Such studies are not likely to be efficient for probing the “biological” end of the Body function category (e.g., hair cell status and function), nor to provide meaningful insights about the “societal” end of the Participation category (e.g., quality of life), although that might be a plausible future goal. Field studies of the type discussed here are also unlikely to be efficient for probing the “biological” end of the Body function category, but they are probably the best vehicle for providing meaningful insights about the “societal” end of the Participation category. With careful design, field studies may also be effective for illuminating aspects of Activity.

In the previous sections, we have discussed various factors and phenomena that likely affect the level of ecological validity in laboratory, field, and hybrid studies. In the process, we have in Table 3 introduced a list of independent variables considered of potential interest across several outcome domains when conducting hearing research.

In this section, we take a closer look at the same independent variables, with the aim of evaluating the level of ecological validity broadly achieved for each variable when implemented in pure laboratory and field studies. Hybrid studies are not considered here, as the independent variables in those studies are either controlled, as would occur in laboratory studies, or not, as would occur in field studies. While diagnostic testing as it is carried out in the clinic was not a focus of the workshop, we included it as a type of “study” for the sake of comparison. This exercise provides a glimpse of the current state of the art in hearing research and clinical practice and helps to highlight future research priorities. By “current state of the art,” we mean the highest level of ecological validity that can be achieved with established equipment and procedures, even if this level is only achieved by a few research laboratories. The exercise is necessarily crude due to the numerous and varied outcome domains and measures in use, as well as subjective, since judging the level of ecological validity of a study is not an exact science.

Table 5 repeats in the first column the independent variables presented in Table 3; except here we have bundled the range of personality and demographic variables under the “Individual” dimension into one row, as the achieved level of ecological validity was judged to be the same across these variables. In the second column, we have for each variable provided some examples of design features that are likely to support a high level of ecological validity of a study. In the final columns, we have indicated whether we judge the variable to have a “low,” “medium,” or “high” level of representation of the real world in the best contemporary standard-of-care clinical, laboratory, and field settings.

TABLE 5. - The independent variables from Table 3 with examples of design features applicable to each variable that are considered likely to support a high level of ecological validity of a study, and the rating of how well this is currently and generally achieved in clinical and research settings
Independent variables Examples of design features that presumably support a high level of ecological validity Current state of the art in the
Clinic Laboratory Field
Sources of stimuli
 Stimulus sources The inclusion of varied natural sound sources; nonevent speech; different talkers (e.g., male/female, adult/child, native/accent); familiar talkers. Low Medium High
 Stimulus materials The inclusion of context-dependent cues such as Lombard effects; variation in speed; disfluencies; interjections, and/or emotion. Low Medium High
 Multimodal stimuli Multiple modalities carry manipulations that are consistent and natural for the intended real-world scenario. Low Medium High
 Other people Other people are represented in a manner (e.g., modalities, behavior) that is consistent with the level of realism in other aspects of the scenario’s presentation. Low High High
Environment
 Acoustic field The presentation of realistic sound levels; spatial relationships; reverberation. Medium High High
 Interaction of environment and hearing devices The acoustic field (including direct and reflected sound) is picked up by the device’s microphone/s in a natural manner. Low Medium High
 Dynamic aspects The presentation of moving sources is realistic for the intended real-world scenario. Low Medium High
 Modalities The presentation includes visual cues (e.g., AV speech cues, nonverbal background cues); tactile cues in interferer stimuli; inertia in the environment. Low Medium High
Context of participation
 Participant preparation Clear instructions and familiarization of study tasks are provided. Medium Medium Medium
 Semantic associations The situations are familiar and relevant to the participant. Medium Low High
 Motivation The scenario and task elicit appropriate engagement and motivation. Medium Medium High
 Familiarity The participant feels comfortable with physical aspects of the experiment. Medium Medium High
 Psych/physiological state The participant is not abnormally stressed or anxious due to factors beyond the study design. Low Low High
Task
 Nature of task The tasks included are appropriate for the intended real-world scenario. Medium Medium High
 Nature of task if speech The speech tasks included resemble those that might occur in the intended real-life scenario. Low Low High
 Complexity Any additional tasks included stimulate natural mental processes as they might occur in the intended real-world scenario. Low Medium High
 Degree of constraint The participant is free to perform the task in whatever ways feel natural in the intended real-world scenario. Low Low Medium
 Exploratory movement The participant is allowed freedom of gaze, head movement, and/or body movement similar to that they would have in the intended real-world scenario, and such movements produce realistic changes in the stimuli. Low Medium High
 Interaction Interaction with other persons represented or actually present elicits plausible behaviors from all involved. Low Medium High
 Predictability The task possesses predictability similar to what would be present in real life. Medium Medium High
 Distractors Any distractors are plausible for the intended real-world scenario. Low Low High
Individual
 Variety of personality and demographic factors Participant recruitment includes stratification or registration of those personal and demographic variables believed to have potential influence. High Low Low

Looking across the rows of Table 5, it can be seen that in agreement with our earlier hypothesis, field testing would seem to present a higher level of ecological validity than both laboratory and clinical testing, suggesting that if ecological validity has high priority in a study, the more time intensive and less controlled field tests currently offer the optimal form of data collection. However, if field studies are to achieve analytical power, they need to be equipped with greater abilities to monitor the values of their uncontrolled variables. Of particular note is that clinical testing is lagging behind in the “Sources of stimuli,” “Environment,” and “Task” dimensions, whereas for laboratory testing there is some scope for improvement in the “Context of participation” dimension. Variables in the “Individual” dimension naturally have the highest level of ecological validity in clinical settings, where individual factors of the specific patient are automatically operating as they should. In research settings, there is a substantial risk that individual variables unaccounted for (or deliberately excluded) reduce the level of ecological validity.

The design features listed in the second column of Table 5 suggest some potential strategies for supporting a high level of ecological validity, especially in laboratory studies. We note that many of these strategies are further detailed and discussed in the selection of articles found in this supplement (e.g., Brungart et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 68S-78S; Carlile & Keidser 2020, this issue, pp. 56S-67S; Grimm et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 48S-55S; Hohmann et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 31S-38S; Lunner et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 39S-47S; Smeds et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 20S-30S) and are still to be formally verified. It should also be noted that some variables are likely to affect the level of ecological validity of a study more than others. A valuable future exercise could thus be to use the information provided in the accompanying articles to prioritize both the methodological dimensions of variables, as well as the variables within each dimension, in terms of their importance when the goal is to achieve a high level of ecological validity of a study. It is likely that the outcome domain of interest will influence which variables are most important.

EVALUATING THE LEVEL OF ECOLOGICAL VALIDITY OF HEARING RESEARCH STUDIES

As noted earlier, ecological validity is not a relevant criterion by which to evaluate all studies. However, for those studies where it is relevant for a given purpose; for example, purpose A (Understanding), B (Development), C (Assessment), or D (Integration and Individualization), we attempt in this section to distil the products of the workshop into some practical recommendations.

As also noted earlier, ecological validity is not a binary concept, and even studies conducted entirely in the real world (field tests) are subject to phenomena that can threaten the ecological validity of research findings. In other words, researchers should be mindful that a study is not either ecologically valid or not, but that each study presents a certain level of ecological validity and that in reality it is probably impossible to carry out a research study that is free of all threats to ecological validity. At the moment, there are no formal guidelines for how to determine the level of ecological validity a study presents nor any set of objective criteria for how to quantify a study design as more or less ecologically valid. Defining such guidelines or criteria would foremost require some agreed understanding of what constitutes the ultimate benchmark for each test variable to maximize ecological validity of a study. As is evident from many of the articles in this supplement, our knowledge of how far we can push variables and phenomena of interest to increase the ecological validity of a study is growing. But efforts are still in their infancy, and the knowledge and ideas exemplified in the articles in this special issue need to be formally consolidated. In the meantime, it is our hope that the thoughts presented in this article (although necessarily subjective and descriptive) will be helpful to researchers in assessing the level of ecological validity in studies in which ecological validity is stated to be relevant. Furthermore, we encourage researchers, when publishing their own work of this type, to disclose if and how specific efforts were made to obtain a high level of ecological validity in their study. Each of the five methodological dimensions of independent variables (Sources of stimuli, Environment, Context of participation, Task, and Individual—cf. Table 3) should be considered, as applicable. Authors should further disclose if and how specific efforts were made to reduce the effect of potential threats to ecological validity identified for the chosen modality of data collection (behavioral, physiological, or self-report), and, if applicable, disclose if and how distortion of participant behavior and biased sampling of everyday life in the field were managed.

As an example, Jensen et al. (2019) conducted an EMA study aiming to obtain information about the auditory reality of hearing aid users. In this study, experienced hearing aid users were equipped with an EMA system that was used to collect information about participants’ experience with two hearing aid programs in the field. The EMA system was programmed to prompt participants to answer a set of questions every 2 hr, but participants could choose to delay or reject the task at such times, they could disengage the prompt alarm when they thought it was inappropriate to be interrupted, and they could also elect to answer the questions unprompted at any time. Because participants manipulated the hearing aid settings as they went about in their everyday environments, this study classifies as a hybrid study, and as such the level of ecological validity of the study is affected by both the interaction with independent variables listed in Table 3 and the phenomena related to field studies. For this study, the assessment of the level of ecological validity would be something like this: As no restrictions were imposed regarding the listening environments in which hearing aid settings could be evaluated, and participants could control when to do the task, the study overall had a high level of ecological validity in terms of “Sources of stimuli,” “Environment,” and “Context of participation.” The fact that participants consciously had to switch between hearing aid programs when in different listening environments reduces the level of ecological validity in the “Task” dimension. Because participants were selected from established pools of presumably high-functioning and healthy test volunteers, the level of ecological validity in the “Individual” category is considered low. The high risk to ecological validity naturally posed by the use of self-reports was partly managed by enabling participants to answer questions about the listening situation and experience in situ. However, the need to read questions and response options and to use a touch screen to perform the task, as well as the option of delaying the completion of the questionnaire discounted some of this gain. The objective measures collected of the environment posed no threat to ecological validity of the study. Finally, there appears to have been no attempt made to alleviate potential threats to ecological validity of the study caused by distortions of participant behavior within everyday life. On the other hand, sampling bias of normal behavior patterns was partly controlled by prompting the participant for responses with set intervals. On balance, taking the aim of the study into account, the ecological validity of the study is judged to be between medium and high. It should be emphasized that the assessment of ecological validity in any specific study is dependent on the study’s aim as well as its experimental design.

ECOLOGICAL VALIDITY AND HOLISM

Holism refers to the treatment of a person as a whole, taking into account the individual’s cognitive and physical well-being, their social network, and environment, and not just the symptoms of their disease. It is a concept that is beginning to make inroads into audiology (Bray 2018), amidst growing appreciation of comorbidities between hearing loss and other chronic health conditions (Besser et al. 2008). Campos and Launer (2020, this issue, pp. 99S-106S) present an in-depth discussion of holism in hearing healthcare. While the potential benefit of a holistic approach to treatment of any health condition seems self-evident, two questions in relation to integrating holism in hearing research were discussed during the workshop:

  • - Would it increase the ecological validity of a study?
  • - Would it influence how the variables outlined in previous sections affect the ecological validity of a study?

Following a discussion of reasons for and against embracing holism in hearing research, workshop participants agreed that the positives (e.g., give hearing health better context) were more likely to increase the face validity than the ecological validity of a study and that the negatives (e.g., add complexity to the study design) could be minimized by organizing programs of research that address confounds through sequential and parallel studies. The general consensus was that integrating holism into a study design may or may not increase its ecological validity.

On the second question, the general consensus was that integrating holism into a study design does not pose any specific threat to how the variables and phenomena discussed in previous sections affect the ecological validity of a study.

KNOWLEDGE GAPS AND RESEARCH PRIORITIES

It is apparent from the results of this Eriksholm Workshop, that in those areas of hearing research that are concerned with how auditory abilities are put to use in everyday life, improvements in our understanding depend upon diverse and increasingly sophisticated methods of measurement (which includes methods of experimental and statistical control). In this section, we summarize key areas where knowledge is still limited and list some priorities for research to move our field forward in terms of achieving more ecologically valid research findings. The list is by no means exhaustive or suggestive of order of importance. Further research recommendations are found in the accompanying articles of this special issue, where many of the research priorities listed here are also discussed in more depth.

Understanding the Processes of Hearing and Communication in Real Life

Many variables and phenomena that are considered to affect the ecological validity of a study have been presented in preceding sections. They are selected based on the experience of researchers working in the field of hearing science for many years. However, it was agreed during the workshop that we possess incomplete understanding of the processes of hearing in real life and of the factors that challenge people with hearing problems in everyday situations. We believe that a more refined conceptual understanding of these issues would be helpful for designing and evaluating studies where high ecological validity is relevant, as well as for developing new assessment tests for clinical applications. The development of such understanding would benefit from the use of qualitative methods involving people with hearing loss and their families (Rapport & Hughes 2020, this issue, pp. 91S-98S). In particular, further work is needed that goes beyond traditional qualitative techniques to include novel methods that capture the transient ephemeral nature of listening. For example, real-time data capture and mobile methods (also known as “walking interviews”—Kinney 2017), allow for qualitative assessments that take place in situ.

Another area deserving of special attention is understanding the processes of hearing in interactive communication situations. It is generally believed that communication difficulty is the most disabling consequence of living with a hearing problem. As is evident from several publications in this special issue, assessments that provide information of high ecological validity about a person’s communication ability are of particular interest in hearing science (e.g., Brungart et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 68S-78S; Carlile & Keidser 2020, this issue, pp. 56S-67S; Grimm et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 48S-55S; Lunner et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 39S-47S). Specifically, we agreed it is of high priority to steer away from traditional unidirectional test paradigms and develop new bidirectional assessment paradigms that encompass the interactive nature of everyday communications. Workshop participants agreed that to achieve this, apart from applying qualitative methods to refine our understanding of the processes of communication in real life, further work is in particular needed to understand: (1) the behaviors (e.g., body language, vocal effort, turn-taking) of interlocutors that lead to communication success and how to effectively measure and characterize such behaviors in multi-person scenarios; (2) the acoustic qualities of sounds, beyond the signal-to-noise ratio, that challenge participation in everyday communication situations; and (3) what imaging data (e.g., Electroencephalography, Magnetoencephalography, Functional near-infrared spectroscopy) and other physiological measures may provide in terms of metrics of communication success and its underlying processes.

Unified and Extended Methodologies

Technological advances are continuously affecting how we collect data. As discussed in several articles in this special issue, new technologies are expected to enhance our ability to obtain more ecologically valid outcome measures, both in the laboratory and in the field (e.g., Caduff et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 120S-130S; Mehra et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 140S-146S; Slaney et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 131S-139S). While the prospects are exciting, knowledge of how best to utilize these new technologies and integrate them with established test methods is lacking. This means that data of potential high importance are being collected using novel, unique, and incompatible methodologies, making it difficult to consolidate findings across research groups.

One technology that was discussed during the workshop is the EMA approach, which is rapidly spreading as a means to collect data in the field (Holube et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 79S-90S; Smeds et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 20S-30S). EMA systems can capture a diverse range of objective and subjective data, and it was agreed that identifying and developing a core data set that could form a common minimal basis for future EMA data collection, where practical, should have high priority. Such a unified minimal set could consist of one or a combination of the following components: (1) objectively measured acoustic characteristics; (2) questions and response options; and (3) temporal structure of assessment intervals.

Another accelerating technology that was discussed is the VR method (Hohmann et al. 2020, this issue, pp. 31S-38S). Historically, VR techniques have been used to assess the impact that hearing impairment has on extremely complicated operational tasks, like engaging in combat in a tank or flying a helicopter. In recent years, VR technology has become more affordable and more capable of simulating unconstrained everyday environments. Amongst other potential advantages, VR is anticipated to make it possible to simulate acoustical, visual, and inertial components of everyday communication situations and multitasking demands with greater control than can be achieved in the field. This technological advance calls for parallel research to develop new task paradigms able to meaningfully reproduce the communicative and cognitive complexities of situations involving two or more people. Also, the inclusion of hearing devices into VR setups is not trivial, and more research is needed to establish criteria for, and methods to achieve, adequate acoustical veracity of sound fields and trade-offs against (for example) freedom of participant movement.

Strategies for Increasing and Evaluating Ecological Validity of Studies

Strategies that are thought to support a higher level of ecological validity in hearing science studies are listed in Table 5. At present, any proposed strategies for increasing ecological validity must be regarded as speculative, as there currently exists no evidence for: (1) what variables and phenomena are most important and reliable for supporting a high level of ecological validity; (2) whether this depends on the outcome domain of interest; and (3) what constitutes the ultimate benchmark for each test variable to maximize the ecological validity. A high priority was identified for research addressing these questions and leading to the development of verifiable recommendations for how to increase the ecological validity of measures obtained in different outcome domains and a set of benchmarks to strive for. In this context, a need was expressed for a tool or metric, which could be used to assess the level of ecological validity of a study. Presently, field studies are believed to be the best approach to obtain outcomes with a high level of ecological validity. However, there is a pressing need for examinations into how newer technology may be utilized to better monitor the values of uncontrolled variables in field studies. Field studies may then attain sufficient analytical power that their results can inform concrete progress.

Ecological Validity and Holism

Holism is a relatively new concept in hearing science, and little is known about how integrating the concept into research designs affects the ecological validity of a study. Workshop participants recognized that to expand the knowledge base in this area, established methodologies used in hearing research would need to be advanced to enable a more integrative approach to measure hearing together with other health, social, and environmental factors, and that this would require knowledge from other and unfamiliar disciplines. For some examples, see Campos and Launer (2020, this issue, pp. 99S-106S) and Carpenter and Campos (2020, this issue, pp. 107S-119S).

CONCLUSION

The sixth Eriksholm Workshop on applying the ecological validity concept in hearing science reached consensus on: (1) a definition of ecological validity; (2) four broad purposes of striving for ecological validity in hearing research and their beneficiaries; (3) the main variables and phenomena that threaten the ecological validity of research findings in laboratory, field, and hybrid studies; (4) strategies that, based on current knowledge, are expected to support a high level of ecological validity of a study; and (5) a range of knowledge gaps that would benefit from future attention. It further developed some thoughts on how to evaluate the level of ecological validity of a study, and on the effect of integrating holism on the ecological validity of a study.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This consensus paper is based on intensive discussions on ecological validity in hearing research that took place at the 6th Eriksholm Workshop in August 2019. G.K. and G.N. convened and facilitated the workshop and have contributed equally to the preparation of the consensus paper. All authors contributed equally to the discussions during the workshop and have reviewed and provided critical feedback to preliminary drafts of the paper. In addition, the following participants contributed substantially to the development of the sections on laboratory studies (J.C.), field studies (I.H. and K.S.), and state of the art (D.B.). The authors further thank the William Demant Foundation for funding the workshop that made it possible to meet in person and work together on developing the consensus views presented here. Contribution to this work was further supported by the Medical Research Council [grant number MR/S003576/1]; and the Chief Scientist Office of the Scottish Government (G.N.); the USA Government (D.B.); and the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation)—Project-ID 352015383—SFB 1330 B1 (G.G. and V.H.).

REFERENCES

Alfakir R., van Leeuwen L. M., Pronk M., Kramer S. E., Zapala D. A. Comparing the International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health Core Sets for Hearing Loss and Otorhinolaryngology/Audiology intake documentation at Mayo Clinic. Ear Hear, (2019). 40, 858–869
Ali A., Hickson L., Meyer C. Audiological management of adults with hearing impairment in Malaysia. Int J Audiol, (2017). 56, 408–416
Besser J., Stropahl M., Urry E., Launer S. Comorbidities of hearing loss and the implications of multimorbidity for audiological care. Hear Res, (2008). 369314
Bray V. A holistic approach to managing hearing loss and its comorbidities. Hear J, (2018). 71, 14,16,17
Bronfenbrenner U. Toward an experimental ecology of human development. Am Psychol, (1977). 32, 513–531
Brungart D. S., Barrett M. E., Cohen J. I., Fodor C., Yancey C., Gordon-Salant S. Objective assessment of speech intelligibility in crowded public spaces. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 68S–78S
Brunswik E. Organismic achievement and environmental probability. Psychol Rev, (1943). 50, 255–272
Caduff A., Feldman Y., Ishai P. B., Launer S. Physiological monitoring and hearing loss: Towards a more integrated and ecologically validated health mapping. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 120S–130S
Campos J. L., Launer S. (From healthy hearing to healthy living: A holistic approach. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 99S–106S.
Carlile S., Keidser G. (Conversational interaction is the brain in action: Implications for the evaluation of hearing and hearing interventions. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 56S–67S.
Carpenter M. G., Campos J. L. (The effects of hearing loss on balance: A critical review. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 107S–119S.
Coene M., Krijger S., van Knijff E., Meeuws M., De Ceulaer G., Govaerts P. J. LiCoS: A new linguistically controlled sentences test to assess functional hearing performance. Folia Phoniatr Logop, (2018). 70, 90–99
Convery E., Hickson L., Meyer C., Keidser G. Predictors of hearing loss self-management in older adults. Disabil Rehabil, (2019). 41, 2026–2035
Cooksey D. A Review of UK Health Research Funding, (2006). The Stationery Office.
Decruy L., Vanthornhout J., Francart T. Evidence for enhanced neural tracking of the speech envelope underlying age-related speech-in-noise difficulties. J Neurophysiol, (2019). 122, 601–615
Devesse A., Dudek A., van Wieringen A., Wouters J. Speech intelligibility of virtual humans. Int J Audiol, (2018). 57, 908–916
Devesse A., van Wieringen A., Wouters J. AVATAR assesses speech understanding and multitask costs in ecologically relevant listening situations. Ear Hear, (2020). 41, 521–531
Edwards B. The future of hearing aid technology. Trends Amplif, (2007). 11, 31–45
Galvez G., Turbin M. B., Thielman E. J., Istvan J. A., Andrews J. A., Henry J. A. Feasibility of ecological momentary assessment of hearing difficulties encountered by hearing aid users. Ear Hear, (2012). 33, 497–507
Grimm G., Hendrikse M., Hohmann V. (Survey of self motion in the context of hearing and hearing device research. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 48S–55S.
Gibson J. J. The concept of the stimulus in psychology. Am Psychol, (1960). 15, 694–703
Grimm G., Kollmeier B., Hohmann V. Spatial acoustic scenarios in multichannel loudspeaker systems for hearing aid evaluation. J Am Acad Audiol, (2016). 27, 557–566
Hadley L. V., Brimijoin W. O., Whitmer W. M. Speech, movement, and gaze behaviours during dyadic conversation in noise. Sci Rep, (2019). 9, 10451
Hall D. A., Smith H., Hibbert A., Colley V., Haider H. F., Horobin A., Londero A., Mazurek B., Thacker B., Fackrell K; Core Outcome Measures in Tinnitus (COMiT) initiative. The COMiT’ID Study: Developing core outcome domains sets for clinical trials of sound-, psychology-, and pharmacology-based interventions for chronic subjective tinnitus in adults. Trends Hear, (2018). 22, 2331216518814384
    Hohmann V., Paluch R., Krueger M., Meis M., Grimm G. The Virtual Lab: Realization and application of virtual sound environments. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 31S–38S
    Holube I., von Gablenz P., Bitzer J. (Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in audiology: Current state, challenges, and future directions. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 79S–90S.
    Illum N. O., Gradel K. O. Parents’ assessments of disability in their children using World Health Organization International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health, Child and Youth Version joined body functions and activity codes related to everyday life. Clin Med Insights Pediatr, (2017). 11, 1179556517715037
    Jaiswal A., Aldersey H. M., Wittich W., Mirza M., Finlayson M. Using the ICF to identify contextual factors that influence participation of persons with deafblindness. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, (2019). 100, 2324–2333
    Jensen N. S., Hau O., Lelic D., Herrlin P., Wolters F., Smeds K. Evaluation of auditory reality and hearing aids using an ecological momentary assessment (EMA) approach. 2019). Proceedings of the 23rd International Congress on Acoustics, Aachen, Germany, Berlin: German Acoustical Society. pp. 6545–6552
    Jerger J. Ecologically valid measures of hearing aid performance. Starkey Audiol Ser, (2009). 1, 1–4
    Kiessling J., Pichora-Fuller M. K., Gatehouse S., Stephens D., Arlinger S., Chisolm T., Davis A. C., Erber N. P., Hickson L., Holmes A., Rosenhall U., von Wedel H. Candidature for and delivery of audiological services: Special needs of older people. Int J Audiol, (2003). 42Suppl 2)2S92–2S101
    Kinney P. Walking interviews. Soc Res Update, (2017). 67, 1–4
    Lersilp S., Putthinoi S., Lersilp T. Facilitators and barriers of assistive technology and learning environment for children with special needs. Occup Ther Int, (2018). 2018, 3705946
    Lewin K. Defining the “field at a given time.”. Psychol Rev, (1943). 40, 292–310
    Lewkowicz D. J. The concept of ecological validity: What are its limitations and is it bad to be invalid?. Infancy, (2001). 2, 437–450
    Lind C., Meyer C., Young J. Hearing and cognitive impairment and the role of the international classification of functioning, disability and health as a rehabilitation framework. Semin Hear, (2016). 37, 200–215
    Lunner T., Alickovic E., Graversen C., Ng E.H.N., Wendt D., Keidser G. (Three new outcome measures that tap into cognitive processes required for real-life communication. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 39S–47S.
    Manchaiah V., Granberg S., Grover V., Saunders G. H., Ann Hall D. Content validity and readability of patient-reported questionnaire instruments of hearing disability. Int J Audiol, (2019). 58, 565–575
    Mehra R., Brimijoin O., Robinson P., Lunner T. Potential of augmented reality platforms to improve individual hearing aids. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 140S–146S
    Neisser U. Cognition and Reality, (1976). Freeman.
    Neuhoff J. G. Neuhoff J. G. Ecological psychoacoustics: Introduction and history. Ecological Psychoacoustics, (2004). Elsevier Academic Press. pp. 1–13
    Parati G., Mancia G. White coat effect: Semantics, assessment and pathophysiological implications. J Hypertens, (2003). 21, 481–486
    Psarros C., Love S. The role of the World Health Organization’s International Classification of Functioning, Health and Disability in models of infant cochlear implant management. Semin Hear, (2016). 37, 272–290
    Rapport F., Hughes S. (Frameworks for change in hearing research: Valuing qualitative methods in the real world. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 91S–98S.
    Schmuckler M. A. What is ecological validity? A dimensional analysis. Infancy, (2001). 2, 419–436
    Shiffman S., Stone A. A., Hufford M. R. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol, (2008). 4, 1–32
    Slaney M., Lyon R. F., Garcia R., Kemler B., Gnegy C., Wilson K., Kanevsky D., Savla S., Cerf V. (Auditory measures for the next billion users. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 131S–139S
    Smeds K., Gotowiec S., Wolters F., Herrlin P., Larsson J., Dahlquist M. Selecting scenarios for hearing-related laboratory testing. Ear Hear, (2020). 41(Suppl 1), 20S–30S
    Speech understanding and aging. Working Group on Speech Understanding and Aging. Committee on Hearing, Bioacoustics, and Biomechanics, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, National Research Council. J Acoust Soc Am, (1988). 83, 859–895
    Weller T., Best V., Buchholz J. M., Young T. A method for assessing auditory spatial analysis in reverberant multitalker environments. J Am Acad Audiol, (2016). 27, 601–611
    Wildemuth B. M (Ed.). (Applications of Social Research Methods to Questions in Information and Library Science (pp. (2017) Libraries Unlimited. 209–218
    Williamson D. A., Blanchard E. B. Heart rate and blood pressure biofeedback: II. A review and integration of recent theoretical models. Biofeedback Self Regul, (1979). 4, 35–50
    Wolters F., Smeds K., Schmidt E., Christensen E. K., Norup C. Common sound scenarios: A context-driven categorization of everyday sound environments for application in hearing-device research. J Am Acad Audiol, (2016). 27, 527–540
    World Health OrganizationInternational Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF), (2001). WHO. https://www.who.int/classifications/icf/en/
    Wu Y. H., Stangl E., Zhang X., Bentler R. A. Construct validity of the ecological momentary assessment in audiology research. J Am Acad Audiol, (2015). 26, 872–884
    Zeni S., Laudanna I., Baruffaldi F., Heimler B., Melcher D., Pavani F. Increased overt attention to objects in early deaf adults: An eye-tracking study of complex naturalistic scenes. Cognition, (2020). 194, 104061
    Keywords:

    Amplification; Ecological validity; Field study; Hearing; Hearing science; Hybrid study; Laboratory study; Outcome domains; Research; Test variables

    Copyright © 2020 The Authors. Ear & Hearing is published on behalf of the American Auditory Society, by Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc.