Secondary Logo

Share this article on:

Letter to the Editor

An Affront to Scientific Inquiry Re

Moore, D. R. (2018) Editorial

Auditory Processing Disorder, Ear Hear, 39, 617–620

Iliadou, Vasiliki (Vivian)1; Chermak, Gail D.2; Bamiou, Doris-Eva3,4; Rawool, Vishakha Waman5; Ptok, Martin6; Purdy, Suzanne7; Jutras, Benoît8; Moncrieff, Deborah9; Stokkereit Mattsson, Tone10; Ferre, Jeanane M.11; Fox, Cydney12; Grech, Helen13; Geffner, Donna14; Hedjever, Mladen15; Bellis, Teri James16; Nimatoudis, Ioannis17; Eleftheriadis, Nikos18; Pedersen, Ellen Raben19; Weihing, Jeffrey20; Guillory, Lisa21; Madell, Jane R.22; Hurley, Annette23; Whitelaw, Gail M.24; Schochat, Eliane25; Spyridakou, Chrysa26; Sidiras, Christos1; Thai-Van, Hung27; Kostopoulou, Anastasia1; Veuillet, Evelyne27; Keith, Bill28; Mountjoy, Alyson29; Koohi, Nehzat30,31; Sirimanna, Tony32; Lau, Carol33; Cone, Barbara34; Kiese-Himmel, Christiane35; Abramson, Maria36; Raghunathrao, Rangasayee37; Musiek, Frank E.38

doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000644
Letters to the Editor
Related Article
Related Article II
Related Article III

1Clinical Psychoacoustics Lab, 3rd Department of Psychiatry, Neuroscience Sector, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece

2Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Elson S. Floyd College of Medicine, Washington State University Health Sciences Spokane, Spokane, Washington, USA

3Faculty of Brain Sciences, UCL Ear Institute, University College London, London, United Kingdom

4Biomedical Research Centre, National Institute for Health Research, London, United Kingdom

5Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders, West Virginia University, Morgantown, West Virginia, USA

6Department of Phoniatrics and Pediatric Audiology, Medizinische Hochschule Hannover (MHH), Hannover, Germany

7Centre for Brain Research, School of Psychology, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand

8Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal and CHU Sainte-Justine Research Center, Canada

9Department of Communication Science and Disorders, School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA

10Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Department of Neuromedicine and Movement Science, NTNU, Trondheim, Norway

11ASHA Special Interest Group 6, Oak Park, Illinois, USA

12International Guild of Auditory Processing Specialists, Los Angeles, California, USA

13University of Malta, Msida, Malta

14St. Johns University, Queens, New York, USA

15Faculty of Education and Rehabilitation Sciences, Speech Therapy Department, University of Zagreb, Zagreb, Croatia

16Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, USD Speech-Language-Hearing Clinics and Division of Basic Biomedical Sciences, University of South Dakota, Vermillion, South Dakota, USA

173rd Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece

18ENT & Audiology Private Practice, Thessaloniki, Greece

19The Maersk Mc-Kinney Moller Institute, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark

20Department of Otolaryngology – Head and Neck Surgery – and Communicative Disorders, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky, USA

21Truman VA Hospital, Columbia, Missouri, USA

22New York Eye and Ear Infirmary, New York, USA

23Department of Communication Disorders, School of Allied Health Professions, Louisiana State University, Health Sciences Center, New Orleans, Louisiana, USA

24Department of Speech and Hearing Science, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, USA

25Faculdade de Medicina FMUSP, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, SP, BR

26Nuffield Hearing and Speech Centre, London, United Kingdom

27CRNL and the Hospices Civils de Lyon, Lyon, France

28SoundSkills, Auckland, New Zealand

29APD Support UK

30National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London, United Kingdom

31UCL Ear Institute, University College London, London, United Kingdom

32Department of Audiology and Audiological Medicine, Great Ormond Street Hospital, London, United Kingdom

33Sound idEARS,Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

34Speech Language and Hearing Sciences, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, USA

35Phoniatric and Pediatric Audiological Psychology, University Medical Center Göttingen, Georg-August-University, Göttingen, Germany

36Hear Now at Abramson Audiology, Laguna Niguel, California, USA

37International Association of Logopedics and Phoniatrics

38Neuroaudiology Lab, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, USA.

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Back to Top | Article Outline

To the Editor:

It was with great concern that we read David Moore’s editorial on auditory processing disorder (APD; Moore 2018). Moore begins his editorial noting that in a “highly detailed critique” of a recent submission on early diagnosis of APD, two of three reviewers questioned whether an earlier diagnosis would be useful “because the status of APD is so controversial” or “difficult to verify.” Moore uses these anonymous reviewers’ comments to justify, based on a few selected papers, several of which are opinion papers rather than research or systematic reviews, that, effective immediately, “articles that either implicitly or explicitly assume APD as a single diagnostic characteristic of the auditory system likely will not be considered for publication.” This Editorial Board policy was adopted without presenting research (and perspectives) that present compelling counter-arguments that would refute such a policy. Moore states that this new Ear and Hearing policy is “endorsed by the whole Board;” however, the editorial is signed only by Moore. Moreover, as clinicians trained in diagnosis, we do not understand Moore’s conflation of the disorder of auditory processing and a “diagnostic characteristic.” According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association 2013), a disorder is defined as “a syndrome characterized by clinically significant disturbance in an individual’s cognition, emotion regulation, or behavior that reflects a dysfunction in the psychological, biological, or developmental processes underlying mental functioning”(p. 20). Thus, by definition, a disorder is not a “diagnostic characteristic.” We provide scientific evidence in this rebuttal that challenges a multitude of Moore’s statements and conclusions used to support his opinion and this indefensible editorial policy. We note the considerable adverse consequences of this policy for scientific inquiry and research, dissemination of scientific knowledge, and ultimately good patient care, and we conclude by urging the Editorial Board to reverse this biased policy.

Back to Top | Article Outline

APD IS A WELL-ESTABLISHED DISORDER

We begin by emphasizing that the submission that caught Moore’s attention and was (presumably) rejected was done so not as a result of methodological design flaws, but rather because the reviewers considered the disorder controversial and difficult to verify. This opinion stands in stark contrast with the inclusion of APD (also known as CAPD or central auditory processing disorder) in both the International Classification of Disorders manual of the World Health Organization version 10 and 11 beta version (ICD-10 and ICD-11 beta version), in which APD/CAPD is recognized as a distinct diagnosis and disorder, as it is by many professional associations and societies throughout the world (e.g., American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 2005; International Bureau for Audiophonologie 2007; American Academy of Audiology 2010; Canadian Interorganizational Steering Group for Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology 2012 ; Danish Medical Audiological Society 2014 ; Esplin & Wright 2014; Deutsche Gesellschaft für Phoniatrie und Pädaudiologie (DGPP) 2015). Moore, thus, devalues the ICD and professional association position statements and guidelines and specifically dismisses those of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA; American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 2005) and the American Academy of Audiology (2010). Instead, he cites three papers to argue that “most if not all cases of childhood APD” really reflect learning or language disorders. To support that assertion, Moore cites de Wit et al.’s (2018) systematic review of 13 studies of moderate quality comparing the performance of children with suspected or diagnosed APD and children diagnosed with a different developmental disorder. This paper concluded that children with various developmental disorders performed similar on the outcome of 85 of 102 (sub)tests, including auditory tests and questionnaires and tests of intelligence, attention, memory, language, and reading. Further, de Wit et al. concluded that “listening difficulties experienced by children are multifaceted and that there is substantial overlap between various developmental disorders.” They quite astutely proposed “therefore, it is crucial that various professions work together and use a multidisciplinary approach not only in the assessment of children with listening complaints but also in the event of children who satisfy the diagnostic standards of specific language impairment, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and dyslexia” and, contrary to Moore’s editorial, concluded that “additional research is required to better understand the different profiles of children with various complaints or disorders.” If Ear and Hearing bans research on APD, presumably, neither review papers like de Wit et al.’s nor research papers like the ones de Wit et al. call for would be published. Obviously, these types of high-quality research papers are needed to advance science not only in the field of APD but also for developmental disorders in general.

Moore also cites DeBonis (2015) and Cameron et al. (2014) whose works actually support the APD diagnosis. While several of us published a rebuttal to DeBonis (2015) (Iliadou et al. 2016), DeBonis does note the adverse impact of speech recognition in noise deficits—well established as a characteristic processing disorder in APD (American Academy of Audiology 2010; American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 2005), and Cameron et al. focus on “spatial processing disorder,” which involves the interplay of speech recognition in noise with two other well-established auditory processes—localization and binaural interaction. Rather than refuting the existence of APD, these reports substantiate the diagnosis. There are 326 peer-reviewed APD papers in PubMed and over a thousand APD papers in the Scopus database: only a handful deny the existence of APD, and none of that handful provide conclusive evidence to support that assertion.

A recent European consensus on APD reached by professionals in 17 European countries (Iliadou et al. 2017b) noted that the two primary reasons why debate around APD lingers are (1) failure by critics to employ the currently available auditory processing test battery, with documented sensitivity and specificity, leading them to (2) draw conclusions regarding APD based on characteristics of individuals who are not diagnosed with APD and are so-called “suspected APD.” Interestingly, Moore ignores this multiauthored paper but selectively cites single-authored opinion papers, that are consistent with his perspective, rather than cite this European consensus paper or other opinion and research papers that counter his viewpoint (e.g., Chermak et al. 2017 ; Moncrieff (2017); Halliday et al. 2017 ; Iliadou et al. 2017a ; Iliadou & Kiese-Himmel 2017 ; Abramson et al. 2018) and other research articles presenting the current mainstream clinical diagnostic and treatment approach (Brenneman et al. 2017 ; Musiek et al. 2018). Moore references a single book focused on APD controversies but fails to cite the numerous books that focus on the auditory neuroscience underlying, and the practice of, diagnosing and treating APD (e.g., Bellis 1996 ; Chermak & Musiek 2014 ; Musiek & Chermak 2014 ; Rawool 2016 ; Geffner & Ross-Swain 2019).

Few if any professionals active in the field of audiology would deny the potential for cognitive and linguistic confounds when evaluating auditory processing or any task that requires co-operation and behavioral response on the part of the individual being tested, not least because the client with any developmental disorder is at a higher risk of having more than one disorder than the general population [e.g., in the case of ADHD (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence 2017) or autism (Mody & Belliveau 2013)]. There are, however, several tutorials on how to best minimize cognitive and linguistic confounds in diagnosing APD (e.g., Chermak et al. 2017a) with a goal to more fully inform individualized treatment and management for clients with complex and diverse needs.

Back to Top | Article Outline

APD IS A DISORDER OF THE CENTRAL AUDITORY NERVOUS SYSTEM

Moore selectively recounts the history of APD, citing the observation by Myklebust (1954) of children with normal hearing sensitivity with listening difficulties. This observation should be viewed, however, within the historical context of work that began in Italy by a team of otorhinolaryngologists, led by Ettore Bocca, which confirmed that patients with central auditory nervous system (CANS) lesions presented significant deficits processing degraded speech, despite the presence of normal audiograms (Bocca et al. 1954). Bocca et al. provided strong evidence for the neurological origins of APD and established the cornerstone upon which APD is diagnosed today. The neurological subtypes of APD have since been extensively documented (e.g., Bamiou et al. 2012 ; Griffiths et al. 2012 ; Grube et al. 2016 ; Harris & Dubno 2017 ; Iliadou et al. 2017 ; Iliadou & Eleftheriadis 2017 ; Koohi et al. 2017 ; Murphy et al. 2017 ; Polanski et al. 2017 ; Szymaszek et al. 2017 ; Durante et al. 2018). Moreover, Boscariol et al. 2010 have demonstrated using neuroimaging that children with perisylvian polymicrogyria, which are indicative of functional cortical abnormalities, present with APD. Boscariol et al. 2011 documented a correlation between degree of auditory processing deficits and extent of cortical abnormality. The performance of children with perisylvian polymicrogyria on central auditory processing tests was, indeed, similar to the performance of children with “developmental” APD/CAPD (i.e., children with no hard evidence of CANS lesions) on these same central auditory processing measures (Boscariol et al. 2010 , 2011 ; Amaral et al. 2015). Boscariol et al’s work documents an anatomical correlate (polymicrogyria) to central auditory test findings. Further, evidence for a neurological underpinning of APD comes from the long course of myelin maturation that has been shown in children to correlate with their ability to perform on tests of dichotic listening (i.e., revealed as left ear deficits). The left ear deficit is a consistent and dependable measure reflecting interhemispheric transfer of acoustic information (Berlin et al. 1973 ; Musiek et al. 1984 ; Bellis et al. 2008 ; Boles et al. 2008 ; Westerhausen et al. 2015).

Back to Top | Article Outline

THE TEST BATTERY GOLD STANDARD

In a section that Moore titles “theory,” he presents a collection of one-sided opinion papers and reviews on listening difficulties, questioning the existence of a diagnostic APD gold standard. These papers define the gold-standard approach as requiring a single test to diagnose a disorder. However, clinicians know that best clinical practice requires that developmental disorders and the majority of neurological diseases require more than a single test to reach an accurate diagnosis. Moreover, it is widely recognized that due to the complexity of disorders there is no single test or symptom that proves a diagnosis, which is why a battery of tests should be used in clinical practice (Jerger & Hayes 1976 ; Kasper et al. 2015). Thus, Moore’s point that because there is no single test to definitively diagnose APD, there is no gold standard, is moot. A gold standard for any given diagnosis is considered the best available and widely used method (Farlex Partner Medical Dictionary 2012). Currently, the best available gold-standard approach is included in the American Academy of Audiology (2010) guidelines that are also endorsed by several audiological societies and APD professional groups worldwide (e.g. International Bureau for Audiophonologie 2007; Canadian Interorganizational Steering Group for Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology 2012 ; Danish Medical Audiological Society 2014 ; Esplin & Wright 2014; Deutsche Gesellschaft für Phoniatrie und Pädaudiologie (DGPP) 2015; European APD Study Group 2017). Going forward, we encourage research to refine the test battery, determine the most efficient test combinations (e.g., Burguetti & Carvallo 2008 ; Butler et al. 2011 ; Musiek et al. 2011 ; Allen & Allan 2014 ; Weihing et al. 2015b ; Barrozo et al. 2016), as well as develop innovative approaches, rather than ban scientific inquiry, as does Ear and Hearing’s new editorial policy.

Moore suggests that neurological underpinnings of APD are only or primarily seen in adults; however, he ignores the accumulating literature (some of which we cited above) that identify neuromorphological or frank neurological lesions underlying APD in some children (e.g., Bamiou et al. 2007 , Iliadou et al. 2008 ; Boscariol et al. 2011 ; Chermak & Musiek 2011 ; Koravand et al. 2017 and others cited above). Moore similarly omits several studies providing animal models for neurodevelopmental aspects of the CANS (Alexander et al. 2014a , b ; Anomal et al. 2015). He also injects the suggestion that attention is the key substrate of APD; however, attention deficits are neither a uniform nor a core supramodal deficit in the majority of children diagnosed with APD (Stavrinos et al. 2018). A number of basic and clinical research findings argue against attention as the primary driver of APD (e.g., Dawes & Bishop 2008 ; Banai et al. 2011 ; Moore et al. 2011 ; Gyldenkærne et al. 2014). Gyldenkaerne et al. (2014) reported that abnormal performance on central auditory tests often occurs despite sustained attention within normal limits, and Banai et al. (2011) reported that maturation rates for different auditory tasks are not correlated, as would be expected if a nonsensory factor (e.g., attention) had a uniform influence on performance.

Moore criticizes the evidence underlying APD by lamenting that many publications on APD state opinions. Ironically, he cites several such publications throughout his editorial. For example, one of the opinion papers he cited suggested that a child with confirmed communication and/or listening difficulties should only be screened for hearing and not diagnostically assessed as is best clinical practice (DeBonis 2015). The arguments were based, however, on papers that involved groups of individuals suspected rather than diagnosed with APD. The problem with the “suspected APD” classification is underscored in a number of papers demonstrating that subjective report of listening problems is a poor indicator of actual performance-based auditory processing difficulties and fails to differentiate these from children with primary language disorders (e.g., Lam & Sanchez 2007 ; Ferguson et al. 2011 ; Barry et al. 2015). We would, thus, counter argue that if children with APD are not properly evaluated they might be erroneously labeled as having attention issues since the “diagnosis” would be based on questionnaires, thus depriving these children of appropriate auditory interventions.

Back to Top | Article Outline

APD TEST BATTERY NORMS

Moore criticizes studies that are based on original experimental data, arguing that they adhere to test batteries endorsed by professional organizations (e.g., AAA and ASHA) that he asserts, for the most part, do not have norms, and where they do are from sources “unavailable for verification.” There are, however, numerous publications describing norms either in the form of cutoffs for specific ages in children (i.e., Bellis 1996 ; Iliadou et al. 2009) or in the form of means and standard deviations (i.e. Shinn et al. 2009 ; Tomlin et al. 2014 ; Pedersen et al. 2017 ; Kiese-Himmel et al. 2018 ; Mattsson et al. 2018 ; Nickisch et al. 2018).

Back to Top | Article Outline

PREVALENCE OF APD

Moore’s section on “clinical prevalence, presentation, and evaluation” begins by citing his retrospective record review in which 1,113 children with normal audiograms had undergone a “central auditory processing evaluation” using the SCAN battery (Moore et al. 2018). Based on the SCAN, Moore concluded that only 14 of those children would be classified as qualifying for an APD diagnosis but that all but one presented other diagnoses (i.e., speech-language, attention disorders, or learning disorders) that provided an “alternative explanation of their listening difficulties.” Moore’s point is unclear because the SCAN is a screening rather than diagnostic battery and given its language content is prone to a strong language confound.

Moore also cites his personal unpublished, prospective study (which is not available for evaluation of its methodological rigor) that only 2 of 100 children “extensively examined” using “the most specific and sensitive tests of central function” presented any type of “auditory-based problem.” However, he does not identify the test battery used nor how the sensitivity and specificity of this battery were derived. His claim of 2 of 100 (2%) is thus no more than “anecdotal evidence.” Furthermore, the prevalence of 2% in his unpublished data set contradicts the 10% prevalence reported in a recent paper that he coauthored (Brewer et al. 2016) by a fivefold difference. Even if we assume that 2% of children more accurately reflects the prevalence of APD, it is unclear the point Moore is arguing. A 2% prevalence for any condition is significant, and even if APD was considered a rare disorder (with an occurrence of less than 1%), rare disorders are not to be ignored. Acoustic tumors, for example, occur far less than 2% of the time, but we certainly do not ignore them. No matter the prevalence rate, our professional ethics and our moral compass demand that each child diagnosed with APD receive the best intervention available to optimize academic social and community access and quality of life.

Moore concludes this section of his editorial stating that children in his prospective, unpublished study presented poor “speech in noise hearing,” which “correlated with slight to moderate impairment of cognitive skills [using the NIH Cognitive Toolbox,” citing Weintraub et al. (2013)]. However, Weintraub et al. state that the NIH-Toolbox Cognition Battery was “not developed as a clinical measure to either screen for cognitive impairment or substitute for a full, competent neurological evaluation.” (p. S61). Therefore, it is unclear how Moore determined slight to moderate impairment of cognitive skills in his sample using the NIH Cognition Toolbox. Clinical evaluations using standardized tools developed for research with undocumented efficiency for clinical diagnosis is yet another reason why research in APD (as well as in any other disorder) must be encouraged not restricted.

Back to Top | Article Outline

AVAILABILITY OF APD EVALUATION

Moore cites personal communication (not published and with unknown rigor) regarding a small informal survey to assert that there has been a decrease in the number of US children’s hospitals offering APD evaluation to imply that this is because APD is not well defined nor measurable. The authors of this rebuttal who practice in the United States would argue that if there is a diminution in services it is most likely due to (1) poor third-party reimbursement, (2) time required to complete a thorough evaluation, and (3) limitations in preparation in this area of practice in AUD programs. We reported these causative factors in a 2007 peer-reviewed publication (Chermak et al. 2007), and unfortunately, we are not aware of any evidence to suggest that these factors have changed substantially. However, the recent inclusion of CAPDs in the ICD-10 may provide the impetus for improved reimbursement by US insurance providers, as is the case in other countries around the world when those claims are coded consistent with the ICD-10.

Back to Top | Article Outline

LISTENING DIFFICULTIES VERSUS APD

To our astonishment, Moore suggests that “listening difficulties” may be a better umbrella term to capture the range of deficits that ultimately impact listening—presumably including APD, attention, memory, and language—and could serve as a “diagnostic billing category.” In fact, listening difficulty is a symptom; APD is a disorder (ICD-10 and ICD-11 beta version). Listening difficulties may be seen in autism, hearing impairment, dyslexia, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and numerous other disorders, and as such listening difficulties is a nonspecific term that contributes little to differential diagnosis. As argued above, this is why questionnaires or self-report can lead to incorrect (over or under) diagnosis of APD. The overlap of symptoms across different disorders is particularly evident in (1) neurodevelopmental disorders and (2) those included in the DSM-5. We are stunned that Moore would suggest the term “listening difficulties” to replace APD when many children referred for APD evaluations because of “listening difficulties” actually perform quite well on central auditory processing measures (Sharma et al. 2009 ; Rosen et al. 2010).

A thorough evaluation relying on central auditory processing tests is mandatory to diagnose APD. APD is a complex disorder, and it is essential that the clinician (audiologist) responsible for diagnosing the disorder consult with other members of a multidisciplinary team to ensure accurate differential diagnosis and identify comorbid disorders to maximize efficient and effective intervention. Such treatment and management must be individualized and targeted to the deficit-specific processes identified by the diagnostic test battery (e.g., Cameron & Dillon 2011). Accumulating evidence has documented the success of auditory interventions following an APD test battery of documented sensitivity and specificity (see Weihing et al. 2015b and Filippini et al. 2019 for reviews).

Back to Top | Article Outline

WHAT WE CAN LEARN FROM THE DSM-5

One of the reasons the DSM was updated (DSM-5) was the realization that “the once plausible goal of identifying homogeneous populations for treatment and research resulted in narrow diagnostic categories that did not capture clinical reality, symptom heterogeneity within disorders, and significant sharing of symptoms across multiple disorders.” (DSM-5 introduction page 12, lines 31–34). The way forward is well stated in the DSM-5’s cautions that: “…a too-rigid categorical system does not capture clinical experience or important scientific observations” and that “… the boundaries between many disorder ‘categories’ are more fluid over the life course.” The DSM-5 argues that the fluidity of boundaries across disorders “should permit a more accurate description of patient presentations and increase the validity of a diagnosis” (DSM-5 page 5, 25–27, 29–30, 34–35). The authors of this rebuttal fully support the DSM-5 approach, an approach which is the antithesis of Ear and Hearing’s editorial decision to not publish research on APD due to its diverse clinical presentations, overlapping symptoms, and comorbidities.

Back to Top | Article Outline

CALL TO ACTION

Moore concludes his editorial by declaring again (as he did in his introduction) that the Ear and Hearing Editorial Board has decided that “articles that either implicitly or explicitly assume APD is a single diagnostic characteristic of the auditory system likely will not be considered for publication.” Because APD is included in the ICD-10 and the ICD-11 beta version as a specific type of hearing impairment/ear disease for both acquired and congenital APD—Ear and Hearing’s decision is indefensible. Ear and Hearing’s editorial decision attempts to restrict and diminish scientific inquiry and research and slow advancement of our understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of APD, by excluding publication of research based on a narrow and predetermined concept of what APD is or is not rather than on the merits of the research methodology (i.e., the collection, measurement, analysis, and clarity of presentation of data). This contradicts all tenets of the scientific process.

Carl Sagan once said that valid criticism does you a favor (Sagan 1995). We would hope that constructive criticism rather than a biased dismissal and banning of rigorous research would be fundamental policy of a journal that purports to serve the interests of the broad audiological community. Some of us have played a role in the development of Ear and Hearing from its inception. We have published and reviewed numerous articles for this journal. Over the years, we have advocated for Ear and Hearing in many ways. Given this history, we are indeed disappointed. We reject this editorial policy decision. We see no evidence that Moore’s editorial was properly vetted, with the thoroughness and fairness that has always been a hallmark of Ear and Hearing’s editorial values. It appears that this policy decision has been rendered without any discussion or input from those with different perspectives. If this editorial policy stands, it will be a serious affront to open, rigorous scientific debate and broad-based, cross-disciplinary representation that Ear and Hearing has championed since its first publication in July of 1975. Moore notes that he has reviewed hundreds of submissions on APD. We wonder how many scientifically worthy publications were rejected due to his bias.

On the basis of the foregoing, and unless and until this editorial policy is reversed, the coauthors of this rebuttal—composed of researchers and clinicians with considerable experience evaluating and treating individuals with APD—will no longer submit manuscripts to Ear and Hearing and will encourage others to refrain as well due to Ear and Hearing’s restriction on scientific inquiry.

Back to Top | Article Outline

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

V.(V).I., G.D.C., D.-E.B., and F.E.M. wrote the first draft that was then modified, edited, commented, and critically revised by all authors. All 39 authors contributed equally to the last draft that is currently submitted and have approved it.

Back to Top | Article Outline

REFERENCES

Abramson M., Moncrieff D., Chermak G.D., et al. Six points of audiological consensus on central auditory processing disorders (CAPD). Hear Rev, 2018). 25, 38–40.
Alexander M., Garbus H., Smith A. L., et al. Cell size anomalies in the auditory thalamus of rats with hypoxic-ischemic injury on postnatal day 3 or 7. Int J Dev Neurosci, 2014a). 33, 1–7.
Alexander M., Garbus H., Smith A. L., et al. Behavioral and histological outcomes following neonatal HI injury in a preterm (P3) and term (P7) rodent model. Behav Brain Res, 2014b). 259, 85–96.
Allen P., Allan C. Auditory processing disorders: Relationship to cognitive processes and underlying auditory neural integrity. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 2014). 78, 198–208.
Amaral M. I., Casali R. L., Boscariol M., et al. Temporal auditory processing and phonological awareness in children with benign epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Biomed Res Int, 2015). 2015, 256340.
American Academy of Audiology. Diagnosis, Treatment and Management of Children and Adults with Central Auditory Processing Disorder (2010). Available from: http://audiology-web.s3.amazonaws.com/migrated/CAPD%20Guidelines%208-2010.pdf_539952af956c79.73897613.pdf.
American Psychiatric Association (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (2013). 5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Anomal R. F., de Villers-Sidani E., Brandão J. A., et al. Impaired processing in the primary auditory cortex of an animal model of autism. Front Syst Neurosci, 2015). 9, 158.
Bamiou D. E., Free S. L., Sisodiya S. M., et al. Auditory interhemispheric transfer deficits, hearing difficulties, and brain magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities in children with congenital aniridia due to PAX6 mutations. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med, 2007). 161, 463–469.
Bamiou D. E., Werring D., Cox K., et al. Patient-reported auditory functions after stroke of the central auditory pathway. Stroke, 2012). 43, 1285–1289.
Banai K., Sabin A. T., Wright B. A. Separable developmental trajectories for the abilities to detect auditory amplitude and frequency modulation. Hear Res, 2011). 280, 219–227.
Barrozo T. F., Pagan-Neves L. d. e. O., Vilela N., et al. The influence of (central) auditory processing disorder in speech sound disorders. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol, 2016). 82, 56–64.
Barry J. G., Tomlin D., Moore D. R., et al. Use of questionnaire-based measures in the assessment of listening difficulties in school-aged children. Ear Hear, 2015). 36, e300–e313.
Bellis T.J. Assessment and Management of Central Auditory Processing Disorders in the Educational Setting: From Science to Practice. 1996). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing.
Bellis T. J., Billiet C., Ross J. Hemispheric lateralization of bilaterally presented homologous visual and auditory stimuli in normal adults, normal children, and children with central auditory dysfunction. Brain Cogn, 2008). 66, 280–289.
Berlin C. I., Hughes L. F., Lowe-Bell S. S., et al. Dichotic right ear advantage in children 5 to 13. Cortex, 1973). 9, 394–402.
Bocca E., Calearo C., Cassinari V. A new method for testing hearing in temporal lobe tumours; preliminary report. Acta Otolaryngol, 1954). 44, 219–221.
Boles D. B., Barth J. M., Merrill E. C. Asymmetry and performance: Toward a neurodevelopmental theory. Brain Cogn, 2008). 66, 124–139.
Boscariol M., Garcia V. L., Guimarães C. A., et al. Auditory processing disorder in perisylvian syndrome. Brain Dev, 2010). 32, 299–304.
Boscariol M., Guimarães C.A., de Vasconcellos Hage S.R., et al. Auditory processing disorder in patients with language-learning impairment and correlation with malformation of cortical development. Brain Dev, 2011). 33, 824–831.
Brenneman L., Cash E., Chermak G. D., et al. The relationship between central auditory processing, language, and cognition in children Being evaluated for central auditory processing disorder. J Am Acad Audiol, 2017). 28, 758–769.
Brewer C. C., Zalewski C. K., King K. A., et al. Heritability of non-speech auditory processing skills. Eur J Hum Genet, 2016). 24, 1137–1144.
Burguetti F.A.R., Carvallo R.M.M. Efferent auditory system: Its effect on auditory processing. [Sistema auditivo eferente: Efeito no processamento auditivo] Braz J Otorhinolaryngol, 2008). 74, 737–745.
Butler B. E., Purcell D. W., Allen P. Contralateral inhibition of distortion product otoacoustic emissions in children with auditory processing disorders. Int J Audiol, 2011). 50, 530–539.
Cameron S., Dillon H. Development and evaluation of the LiSN & learn auditory training software for deficit-specific remediation of binaural processing deficits in children: Preliminary findings. J Am Acad Audiol, 2011). 22, 678–696.
Cameron S., Dillon H., Glyde H., et al. Prevalence and remediation of spatial processing disorder (SPD) in indigenous children in regional Australia. Int J Audiol, 2014). 53, 326–335.
Canadian Interorganizational Steering Group for Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology. Canadian Guidelines on Auditory Processing Disorder in Children and Adults: Assessment & Intervention (2012). Retrieved from http://www.ooaq.qc.ca/publications/doc-documents/Canadian_Guidelines_EN.pdf.
Chermak G. D., Bamiou D. E., Vivian Iliadou V., et al. Practical guidelines to minimise language and cognitive confounds in the diagnosis of CAPD: A brief tutorial. Int J Audiol, 2017a). 56, 499–506.
Chermak G.D., Musiek F.E. Neurological substrate of central auditory processing deficits in children. Cur Pediatr Rev, 2011). 7, 241–251.
Chermak G. D., Musiek F. E. Handbook of Central Auditory Processing Disorder: Vol. 2. Comprehensive Intervention (2014). 2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing.
    Chermak G.D., Musiek F.E., Weihing J. Beyond controversies: The science behind central auditory processing disorder. Hear Rev, 2017b). 24, 20–24.
    Chermak G. D., Silva M. E., Nye J., et al. An update on professional education and clinical practices in central auditory processing. J Am Acad Audiol, 2007). 18, 428–452; quiz 455.
    Danish Medical Audiological Society. (2014). Retrieved from http://dmasaud.dk/onewebmedia/DSOHH-KKR-APD.pdf.
    Dawes P., Bishop D. V. Maturation of visual and auditory temporal processing in school-aged children. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 2008). 51, 1002–1015.
    de Wit E., van Dijk P., Hanekamp S., et al. Same or different: The overlap between children with auditory processing disorders and children with other developmental disorders: A systematic review. Ear Hear, 2018). 39, 1–19.
    Deutsche Gesellschaft für Phoniatrie und Pädaudiologie (DGPP). Leitlinie Auditive Verarbeitungs- und Wahrnehmungsstörungen (2015). Retrieved from http://www.awmf.org/leitlinien/detail/ll/049-012.html.
    Durante A. S., Mariano S., Pachi P. R. Auditory processing abilities in prematurely born children. Early Hum Dev, 2018). 120, 26–30.
    Esplin J., Wright C. Auditory Processing Disorder: New Zealand Review. A Report Prepared for the Ministry of Health and Ministry of Education. 2014). Retrieved from https://www.health.govt.nz/system/files/documents/publications/auditory_processing_disorder.pdf.
    Farlex Partner Medical Dictionary. (2012). Retrieved from http://medical-dictionary.thefreedictionary.com/gold+standard.
    Ferguson M. A., Hall R. L., Riley A., et al. Communication, listening, cognitive and speech perception skills in children with auditory processing disorder (APD) or Specific Language Impairment (SLI). J Speech Lang Hear Res, 2011). 54, 211–227.
    Filippini R., Weihing J., Chermak G.D., et al. Geffner D, Swain D. Current issues in the diagnosis and treatment of central auditory processing disorder in children. In Auditory Processing Disorders: Assessment, Management, and Treatment. (2019). 3rd ed., pp. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.3–36).
    Geffner D., Ross-Swain D. Auditory Processing Disorders: Assessment, Management, and Treatment. (2019). 3rd ed.). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.
    Griffiths T.D., Bamiou D.E., Warren J.D. Palmer A. R, Rees A. Disorders of the auditory brain. In The Oxford Handbook of Auditory Science the Auditory Brain (pp. 2012). United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.509–542).
    Grube M., Bruffaerts R., Schaeverbeke J., et al. Core auditory processing deficits in primary progressive aphasia. Brain, 2016). 139(pt 6), 1817–1829.
    Gyldenkærne P., Dillon H., Sharma M., et al. Attend to this: The relationship between auditory processing disorders and attention deficits. J Am Acad Audiol, 2014). 25, 676–687; quiz 706.
    Halliday L. F., Tuomainen O., Rosen S. Auditory processing deficits are sometimes necessary and sometimes sufficient for language difficulties in children: Evidence from mild to moderate sensorineural hearing loss. Cognition, 2017). 166, 139–151.
    Harris K. C., Dubno J. R. Age-related deficits in auditory temporal processing: Unique contributions of neural dyssynchrony and slowed neuronal processing. Neurobiol Aging, 2017). 53, 150–158.
    Iliadou V., Bamiou D. E., Kaprinis S., et al. Auditory processing disorder and brain pathology in a preterm child with learning disabilities. J Am Acad Audiol, 2008). 19, 557–563.
    Iliadou V., Bamiou D. E., Kaprinis S., et al. Auditory processing disorders in children suspected of learning disabilities–A need for screening? Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 2009). 73, 1029–1034.
    Iliadou V. V., Bamiou D. E., Sidiras C., et al. The use of the gaps-in-noise test as an index of the enhanced left temporal cortical thinning associated with the transition between mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Acad Audiol, 2017a). 28, 463–471.
    Iliadou V. V., Eleftheriadis N. Auditory processing disorder as the sole manifestation of a cerebellopontine and internal auditory canal lesion. J Am Acad Audiol, 2017). 28, 91–101.
    Iliadou V., Kiese-Himmel C. Common misconceptions regarding pediatric auditory processing disorder. Front Neurol, 2017). 8, 732.
    Iliadou V. V., Ptok M., Grech H., et al. A European perspective on auditory processing disorder-Current knowledge and future research focus. Front Neurol, 2017b). 8, 622.
      Iliadou V. V., Sirimanna T., Bamiou D. E. CAPD is classified in ICD-10 as H93.25 and hearing evaluation-Not screening-should be implemented in children with verified communication and/or listening deficits. Am J Audiol, 2016). 25, 368–370.
      Jerger J. F., Hayes D. The cross-check principle in pediatric audiometry. Arch Otolaryngol, 1976). 102, 614–620.
      Kasper D. L., Hauser S. L., Jameson J. L., et al. Harrison’s Principles of Internal Medicine. 2015). 19th ed. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Medical Publishing Division.
      Kiese-Himmel C., Nickisch A., Werner F. [Auditory processing disorder: Is the diagnosis already possible in first graders?]. Laryngorhinootologie, 2018). 97, 37–43.
      Koohi N., Vickers D. A., Lakshmanan R., et al. Hearing characteristics of stroke patients: Prevalence and characteristics of hearing impairment and auditory processing disorders in stroke patients. J Am Acad Audiol, 2017). 28, 491–505.
      Koravand A., Jutras B., Lassonde M. Abnormalities in cortical auditory responses in children with central auditory processing disorder. Neuroscience, 2017). 346, 135–148.
      Lam E., Sanchez L. Evaluation of screening instruments for auditory processing disorder (APD) in a sample of referred children. Aust N Z J Audiol, 2007). 29, 26–39.
      Mattsson T. S., Follestad T., Andersson S., et al. Normative data for diagnosing auditory processing disorder in Norwegian children aged 7-12 years. Int J Audiol, 2018). 57, 10–20.
      Mody M., Belliveau J. W. Speech and language impairments in autism: Insights from behavior and neuroimaging. N Am J Med Sci (Boston), 2013). 5, 157–161.
      Moore D. R. Guest Editorial: Auditory processing disorder, Ear Hear, 2018). 39, 617–620.
      Moore D. R., Cowan J. A., Riley A., et al. Development of auditory processing in 6- to 11-yr-old children. Ear Hear, 2011). 32, 269–285.
      Moncrieff D. Response to de Wit et al., 2016, “characteristics of auditory processing disorders: A systematic review”. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 2017). 60, 1448–1450.
      Murphy C. F. B., Stavrinos G., Chong K., et al. Auditory processing after early left hemisphere injury: A case report. Front Neurol, 2017). 8, 226.
        Musiek F. E., Chermak G. D., Bamiou D. E., et al. CAPD: The most common ‘hidden hearing loss’ central auditory processing disorder-and not cochlear synaptopathy-is the most likely source of difficulty understanding speech in noise (despite normal audiograms). ASHA Lead, 2018). 23, 6.
        Musiek F. E., Chermak G. D., Weihing J., et al. Diagnostic accuracy of established central auditory processing test batteries in patients with documented brain lesions. J Am Acad Audiol, 2011). 22, 342–358.
        Musiek F. E., Chermak G. D. Handbook of Central Auditory Processing Disorder: Vol. 1. Auditory Neuroscience and Diagnosis (2014). 2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing.
        Musiek F. E., Gollegly K. M., Baran J. A. Myelination of the corpus callosum and auditory processing problems in children: Theoretical and clinical correlates. Semin Hear, 1984). 5, 231–240.
        Myklebust HR. Auditory Disorders for children: A manual for differential diagnosis. 1954) New York: Grune & Stratton.
        National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. (Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Diagnosis and Management, NICE Guideline: Short Version. 2017). Retrieved from https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng87/documents/short-version-of-draft-guideline.
        Nickisch A., Werner F., Kiese-Himmel C. APD in first graders. Which tests discriminate between normal and impaired children? [Diagnostik von AVWS bei Erstklässlern: Welche Tests trennen auffällige von unauffälligen Kindern?] Laryngorhinootologie, 2018). Feb 15.
        Pedersen E. R., Dahl-Hansen B., Christensen-Dalsgaard J., et al. Implementation and evaluation of a Danish test battery for auditory processing disorder in children. Int J Audiol, 2017). 56, 538–549.
        Polanski J. F., Soares A. D., Pereira L. D., et al. The effect of citalopram versus a placebo on central auditory processing in the elderly. Otol Neurotol, 2017). 38, 1233–1239.
        Rawool V. Auditory Processing Deficits: Assessment and Intervention. 2016). New York, NY: Thieme.
        Rosen S., Cohen M., Vanniasegaram I. Auditory and cognitive abilities of children suspected of auditory processing disorder (APD). Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 2010). 74, 594–600.
        Sagan C. Wonder and Skepticism, Skeptical Inquirer Volume 19.1. 1995). Retrieved from https://www.csicop.org/si/show/wonder_and_skepticism.
        Sharma M., Purdy S. C., Kelly A. S. Comorbidity of auditory processing, language, and reading disorders. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 2009). 52, 706–722.
        Shinn J. B., Chermak G. D., Musiek F. E. GIN (gaps-in-noise) performance in the pediatric population. J Am Acad Audiol, 2009). 20, 229–238.
        Stavrinos G., Iliadou V. M., Edwards L., et al. The relationship between types of attention and auditory processing skills: Reconsidering auditory processing disorder diagnosis. Front Psychol, 2018). 9, 34.
        Szymaszek A., Wolak T., Szelag E. The treatment based on temporal information processing reduces speech comprehension deficits in aphasic subjects. Front Aging Neurosci, 2017). 9, 98.
        Tomlin D., Dillon H., Kelly A. S. Allowing for asymmetric distributions when comparing auditory processing test percentage scores with normative data. J Am Acad Audiol, 2014). 25, 541–548.
        Weihing J., Chermak G. D., Musiek F. E. Auditory training for central auditory processing disorder. Semin Hear, 2015a). 36, 199–215.
        Weihing J., Guenette L., Chermak G., et al. Characteristics of pediatric performance on a test battery commonly used in the diagnosis of central auditory processing disorder. J Am Acad Audiol, 2015b). 26, 652–669.
        Weintraub S., Dikmen S. S., Heaton R. K., et al. Cognition assessment using the NIH toolbox. Neurology, 2013). 80(Suppl 3), S54–S64.
        Westerhausen R., Bless J., Kompus K. Behavioral laterality and aging: The free-recall dichotic-listening right-ear advantage increases with age. Dev Neuropsychol, 2015). 40, 313–327.
        Copyright © 2018 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.