Secondary Logo

Share this article on:

Referred Muscle Pain: Basic and Clinical Findings

Arendt-Nielsen, Lars Dr.Med.Sci., Ph.D.; Svensson, Peter Dr.Odont, Ph.D.

The Clinical Journal of Pain: March 2001 - Volume 17 - Issue 1 - p 11-19
Special Topic Series: Musculoskeletal Pain

*Laboratory for Experimental Pain Research, Center for Sensory-Motor Interaction, Aalborg University, Aalborg, Denmark; †Department of Prosthetic Dentistry and Stomatognathic Physiology, Dental School, University of Aarhus, Aarhus, Denmark

Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Lars Arendt-Nielsen, Aalborg University, Center for Sensory-Motor Interaction, Laboratory for Experimental Pain Research, Fredrik Bajers Vej 7, D3, DK-9220 Aalborg, Denmark. Address electronic mail to LAN@smi.auc.dk

Acute pain and long-term pain originating from deep somatic structures represent a major part of pain complaints in many patients. Deep pain is a diagnostic and therapeutic problem, and further insights into the peripheral and central neurophysiologic mechanisms are necessary to improve diagnosis and therapy. Systematic studies of referred pain from muscles may help to reveal such mechanisms. The focus of this paper is discussion of the possible mechanisms behind pain referred from muscles.

Paradoxically, a large amount of experimental pain research has been obtained from studies of cutaneous pain. Cutaneous pain varies from deep pain in many ways. Typically, it is described as a localized sharp or burning pain and is rarely (if ever) referred to other somatic structures. Conversely, deep pain often is described as a diffuse, dull pain, with frequent referral to distant sites.1

Referred pain has been known and described for more than a century, and it has been used extensively as a diagnostic tool in the clinical setting. Head2 initially used the term "referred tenderness and pain" in 1893. However, other clinicians had reported the phenomenon previously (for a review, see Bonica1). Since then, it has been used to describe pain perceived at a site adjacent to or at a distance from the site of origin. The taxonomy committee of the International Association for the Study of Pain has not defined the term; however, several authors have defined it in different ways. In this paper, we will use the definition "pain felt at a site remote from the site of origin/stimulation."

Several neuroanatomic and physiologic theories regarding the appearance of referred pain have been suggested, and they state that nociceptive dorsal horn and brain stem neurons receive convergent inputs from various tissues; therefore, higher centres cannot identify correctly the actual input source. Most recently, the models have included newer theories in which plasticity of dorsal horn and brainstem neurons plays a central role. During the past decades, a systematic attempt to chart referred musculoskeletal pain areas in humans has been made.3 Some of these findings have been reproduced in experimental muscle pain studies in humans.4-15

Back to Top | Article Outline

BASIC ASPECTS

Clinical versus experimental studies regarding referred pain

Further basic research of all aspects of referred pain is needed to obtain a better understanding of pain pathologies related to deep somatic structures. Clinical research and, in particular, research of pain, often are confounded by many factors that make it difficult to look at specific aspects of the disease. Experimental models seem to be good alternatives.

Human experimental pain research classically involves two separate topics: (1) standardized activation of the nociceptive system and (2) measurements of the evoked responses (for a review, see Arendt-Nielsen16). The ultimate goal of advanced human experimental pain research is to obtain a better understanding of mechanisms involved in pain transduction, transmission, and perception under normal and pathophysiologic conditions. Hopefully, this can give more insight regarding the mechanisms underlying referred pain and provide better characterization, prevention, and management of pain. Experimental studies are useful in basic research because they can be standardized by using healthy individuals, allow a study with few confounding factors, and studies can be performed during very standardized conditions.16

Studies of clinical pain are limited by bias because of cognitive, emotional, and social aspects of the disease. Pain is a multidimensional and highly individualized perception that is difficult to quantify and to validate in the clinical setting. In experimental pain, the researchers have the possibility to control stimulus intensity, duration, and modality. Furthermore, the psychophysical-evoked responses can be assessed quantitatively (using, for example, visual analog scores) or qualitatively (using, for example, the McGill Pain Questionnaire). Stimulus-response relations, being of great value in, for example, pharmacologic research, can also be investigated. Disadvantages of experimental models are the short-lasting acute stimuli, which may not parallel long-term clinical pain. The psychological involvement may also be limited in experimental models; therefore, the stimuli may not mimic clinical pain sufficiently. Therefore, multimodality experimental pain stimuli may be recommended for assessment of pharmacologic interventions.16-18 A multimodal sensory test regime also should be used when hyper-/hypoalgesia is assessed in referred pain areas.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Muscle pain

Various methods can be used to induce experimental muscle pain. Usually, the methods are classified in two groups: (1) endogenous (without external stimuli); and (2) exogenous (external stimuli) methods.17

Human endogenous methods (e.g., ischemia and exercise) are characterized by high response rate and are suitable for studying general pain states. However, they have the disadvantage of involving several or all muscle groups within the region investigated, and often pain from other somatic tissues cannot be excluded.17,19 Finally, endogenous methods are not suitable to induce referred pain. Therefore, we will concentrate on exogenous models in this paper.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Referred muscle pain using algogenic substances

A number of exogenous methods have been used to induce experimental human muscle pain. The most commonly used method is intramuscular infusion of hypertonic saline (6%). Kellgren and Lewis introduced the method in 1938,20,21 and intramuscular infusion of hypertonic saline subsequently has been used extensively.4-6,11-15,22-31 A variety of parameters have been shown to correlate with the infusion of hypertonic saline (e.g., saline concentration, infusion rate and pressure, and amount of saline infused).4,29,32 Nevertheless, the mechanisms responsible for the excitation of nociceptive activity shortly after the infusion are still unknown. A direct excitation of afferents because of osmotic difference has been proposed, although other mechanisms can not be excluded.32 Referred pain is felt in structures at a distance from the infusion site, and it appears with a delay of approximately 20 seconds in comparison with local pain5 (Fig. 1). This referred pain is characterized as being diffuse and unpleasant.4

FIG. 1

FIG. 1

Infusion of hypertonic saline has several advantages. It is easy and safe to use, and it induces local and referred muscle pain in most individuals (40-85%), depending on the actual muscle of injection.4-6,11,12,14 The disadvantage of this muscle pain model is the relatively long-lasting pain (several minutes) after a bolus infusion.4,5,6,10,17,31

In recent years, more potent algogenic substances have been tested as muscle pain models. Bradykinin,33-37 serotonin,35-37 capsaicin,38-40 and substance P35-37 have been used separately or in combination to induce muscle pain. This model combining different algogenic substances has been a promising model for deep tissue hyperalgesia. One study has shown referred pain after subsequent intramuscular injections of serotonin and bradykinin was used.35

Back to Top | Article Outline

Referred muscle pain using electrical stimulation

Intramuscular electrical stimulation (IMES) of muscle tissue has been used in various experimental and clinical settings. Intramuscular electrical stimulation offers an advantage in that it can induce referred muscle pain in an on-and-off manner. It is an easy method to use, and a high incidence of local (94%) and referred (78%) pain is induced.7 In our studies of IMES, we used 10-Hz stimulation for at least 10 seconds to generate referred muscle pain (Fig. 2).

FIG. 2

FIG. 2

Intramuscular electrical stimulation has been used to assess somatosensory sensibility by determining various thresholds (e.g., of sensation and of pain). Vecchiet et al.41 found a significantly lower pain threshold in muscle, subcutis, and skin of patients with chronic fatigue syndrome in comparison with healthy controls, which indicated hypersensitivity to painful stimulation in this group of patients. In a recent study, IMES was used to evaluate the effect of ketamine on muscle pain induced using single electrical stimulation in comparison with repeated (temporal summation) electrical stimulation for patients with fibromyalgia. A significant increase in the pain summation threshold to repeated IMES was found during the ketamine infusion.42

Referred pain after IMES appears with different delays in the various studies7-10 that range from immediately after the referred pain occurs to a delay of 43 seconds on average. A difference in stimulus intensities could account for the variances of referred pain onset.7 A more consistent delay of referred pain onset is characteristic for hypertonic saline experiments.6 The reason for the difference in time delay between the two models could be due to different excitation mechanisms of the nociceptive afferents and/or because of central mechanisms (temporal summation or hyperexcitability). However, IMES has a shortcoming in comparison with hypertonic saline in that it bypasses the sensory nerve endings, which makes investigations of receptor transduction mechanisms impossible.

Significantly higher stimulus intensity is necessary to elicit referred pain in comparison with local pain, and a significantly positive correlation has been found among the stimulus intensity and the local pain and referred pain intensity ratings.7 This is in accordance with previous experimental and clinical studies5,6,23,43,44 and studies that used direct intraneural electrical stimulation of muscle nociceptive afferents.38,43,45

Spatial summation is a well-described feature in many experimental pain models of cutaneous pain,46 deep pain,6,38,45 and visceral pain.47,48 The mechanism responsible for spatial summation observed most likely is an additional recruitment of nociceptor units,49 which results in an increased barrage to dorsal horn and brain-stem neurons and, consequentially, increased local pain and referred pain.

Significant correlations between the size of local pain and referred pain areas and the local sensation/pain and referred sensation/pain intensity ratings have been demonstrated.7 Similar observations have been detected in studies in which sequential infusions of hypertonic saline into a muscle resulted in an increasing number of individuals experiencing referred pain and increasing areas of referred pain,5 and in which intraneural electrical stimulation of muscle afferents at a constant frequency and intensity evoked an expansion of the projected pain area over time.38 Increased nociceptive input to the dorsal horn or brainstem neurons, which generates an expansion of receptive fields,50,51 may be responsible for the expansion of referred areas detected during increased intramuscular stimulation.7

Back to Top | Article Outline

Manifestation of referred muscle pain

Inman and Saunders systematically investigated the distribution of referred pain in relation to the activated muscle groups.52 Based on their observations, they suggested that referred pain followed the distribution of sclerotomes (muscle, fascia, and bone) more frequently than it followed the classical dermatomes.53

Sensory manifestations of clinical and experimental muscle pain are seen as diffuse aching pain in the muscle, pain referred to distant somatic structures, and modifications in superficial and deep tissue sensibility in the painful areas.1,6,20 These manifestations differ from cutaneous pain, which normally is superficial and localized around the injury and has a sharp and burning quality.1,4 Referred pain and sensibility changes in the painful structures have been known for many years,20,24 but the neural mechanisms responsible for these phenomena are not understood fully.

Referred muscle pain probably involves a central neurobiological mechanism because it is possible to induce referred pain to limbs with complete sensory loss using an anesthetic block.24 However, the lack of peripheral input from the referred pain area seems to decrease the referred pain intensity,8 which suggests that the peripheral input from the referred pain area is involved but not a necessary condition for referred pain. Hypothetically, convergence of nociceptive afferents on dorsal horn neurons may mediate referred pain, but studies by Hoheisel and Mense54 showed a rare convergence of muscle afferents and other deep tissue afferents, such as muscle, although Sessle et al.55 showed an extensive convergence between both deep and superficial afferents in the craniofacial region. Central hyperexcitability may modulate the manifestation of referred pain. Animal studies have found a development of new receptive fields via noxious muscle stimuli.56-58 Recordings from a dorsal horn neuron with a receptive field located in the biceps femoris muscle indicated new receptive fields in the tibialis anterior muscle and in the paw after intramuscular injection of bradykinin into the tibialis anterior muscle.58 In the context of referred pain, revealing new receptive fields could be the mechanism behind referred pain because of central hyperexcitability.59 Forming of new receptive fields has been suggested to be the phenomenon of secondary hyperalgesia in deep tissue.59 Similar findings are shown in humans after intradermal injection of capsaicin in which a rapid development of central hyperexcitability (seen as secondary cutaneous hyperalgesia) is found. The time needed for revealing (in the range of seconds) may account for the time delay between local pain and the development of referred pain4 and for the increased number of individuals developing referred pain during repeated hypertonic saline infusions5 or tonic infusion.14

Several studies have found that the area of the referred pain correlated with the intensity6,7,52,60 and duration38 of the muscle pain, which parallels the observations for cutaneous secondary hyperalgesia. Chronic musculoskeletal pain has been shown to respond better to treatment using NMDA-receptor antagonists (ketamine) than to conventional morphine management,61 which indicates the role of central hyperexcitability in these patients, with the reason being that NMDA-antagonists in animal studies, in experimental studies, and in clinical studies are found to inhibit wind-up and hyperalgesia. Therefore, it is reasonable to propose that muscle pain conditions59 may evoke central hyperexcitability, which may play an important role in long-term musculoskeletal pain syndromes (e.g., whiplash62). The relation between temporal summation and central hyperexcitability may be shown by the progressive spread of pain during tonic intramuscular infusion of hypertonic saline.14 From studies on cutaneous hyperalgesia,47 central summation of nociceptive input from muscles and referred pain areas is expected to be exaggerated in musculoskeletal pain conditions if central hyperexcitability is involved. Infusions of hypertonic saline have shown larger referred pain areas in fibromyalgia patients than in controls, and also proximal referral of pain was found in the patients, but not in controls.61 This may reflect central hyperexcitability in fibromyalgia patients as hypertonic saline is infused into muscles with no clinical muscle pain.61 Moreover, the gain of temporal summation was increased in fibromyalgia patients as the pain threshold for repeated intramuscular electrical stimulation and not single stimulation was decreased in fibromyalgia patients compared to controls.61

In a recent study, a similar manifestation of enlarged referred pain areas to intramuscular injection of hypertonic saline was found in chronic pain patients after whiplash injuries.62 Preliminary data63 from temporomandibular pain patients show that such enlarged areas also can be manifested in the orofacial region. Similarly, enlarged referred pain areas also are found after visceral stimulation in patients with chronic visceral pain.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Hyperalgesia related to referred muscle pain

The somatosensory sensibility in the referred pain area may provide additional information about the mechanisms involved in generation of referred pain. It is accepted that muscle pain can result in hyperalgesia in the referred somatic structures.

The somatosensory sensibility is affected by saline-induced muscle pain in cutaneous and deep structures in the area of local and referred pain. During saline-induced pain, the deep tissue sensibility may increase,12,24,27,64 decrease,65 or remain unaffected5 in the local and referred muscle pain area.

Increased VAS response to electrical cutaneous stimulation and decreased sensibility to radiant heat stimulation have been reported in referred pain areas.5 This modality-specific somatosensory change found in the referred muscle area is similar to findings in secondary hyperalgesic areas of the skin.

The mechanisms of sensibility changes may be of peripheral origin or of central origin. Infiltration of the muscle tissue by anesthetics 30 minutes after injection of hypertonic saline completely reverses the cutaneous and muscular hyperalgesia.27 The effect of a peripheral block on the hyperalgesia27 suggests that the hyperalgesia is caused by maintained peripheral input which is also a necessary condition for referred pain.5,20 Alternatively, the mechanisms responsible for deep and cutaneous hyperalgesia after muscle pain may be caused by central hyperexcitability.

Central hyperexcitability of dorsal horn and brainstem neurons initiated by nociceptive activity from muscles may explain the expansion of pain with referral to other areas, and it probably also explains hyperalgesia in these areas. However, facilitated neurons do not account for the decreased sensation to certain sensory stimuli in the referred area. Descending inhibitory control of the dorsal horn neurons may explain the decreased response to additional noxious stimuli in the referred pain area. Recently, we found that saline-induced muscle pain resulted in deep-tissue hypoalgesia in extra segmental areas (including the area of referred pain) distant from the pain focus.13,66 In addition, segmental inhibition at the spinal cord or brainstem level may contribute to the decreased sensibility.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Modulation of referred pain

During the past century, several theories on the origin of referred pain have been suggested (see later in this paper). To illuminate possible mechanisms of referred pain, a number of case reports and experiments regarding the effect of anesthetizing the referred pain area have been published. Often, when referred visceral pain has been investigated, contradictory results have been shown. Weiss and Davies67 published the first large study. They found that patients with various diseases (e.g., angina pectoris, pleuritis, stomach ulcer, chronic cholecystitis, salpingitis, and kidney stones) experienced pain at structures (most often the skin) located at a distance from the affected organ(s), which could have been partially and, in some cases, completely abolished by infiltrating the area using a local anesthetic.

Conversely, Wollard et al.68 found minor or no changes of referred pain intensity in an anesthetized skin area. Furthermore, Kellgren20 did not see a decrease in referred pain intensity when he anesthetized areas to which saline-induced muscle pain was referred.

Several explanations regarding the divergent results obtained when an area of referred pain is anesthetized have been offered: (1) the variation in the number of structures (skin, subcutis, fascia, muscle, tendons, ligaments, and bone) anesthetized. This is likely a major bias because referred pain areas and, especially visceral referred pain, tend to be located in the deep tissues in which complete anesthesia of a referred pain area is difficult; (2) the duration and level of local pain; (3) the site of the local pain (skin, viscera, and deep structures); (4) whether sensory changes (hypersensitivity) occur at the referred pain site.

The IMES technique recently has been used to investigate systematically the effect of anesthetizing the referred areas. In a placebo-controlled experiment,8 an eutectic mixture of local anesthetics was applied to the skin lying over the referred pain area. Reduction of the referred pain intensity by 22.7% was shown in the local anesthetic group in comparison with the placebo group.

A similar result has been reported when ethyl chloride was sprayed onto the saline-induced referred pain area, which greatly reduced referred pain.69 This suggests that referred pain to some degree is dependent on spontaneous input from cutaneous receptors. Although cutaneous nociceptors do not exert resting activity,70 a reduction of activity from other skin receptors (e.g., thermal receptors and possibly low-threshold mechano-receptors) could explain the finding.

To completely block all afferents from the referred pain area two techniques have been used: (1) differential nerve blocking with an inflated tourniquet between the site of stimulation and the corresponding distal referred area;9 and (2) intravenous regional analgesia (IVRA).9

Interestingly, the referred pain intensity was reduced by 40.2% while myelinated nerve fiber function was impaired, which suggests that referred pain has a peripheral component associated with intact myelinated nerve fiber function. When the nerve fiber function was blocked completely, referred pain still occurred.

Other studies in which the afferent input from the referred area has been completely blocked, have reported similar findings.24,71 Therefore, evidence seems to suggest that referred pain, to some degree, depends on an intact peripheral nervous system, with some spontaneous input.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Neurophysiological mechanisms for referred pain

The mechanisms responsible for referred pain referral to adjacent anatomical segment are not known in detail. Several theories have been suggested and will be summarized briefly. These theories have been developed for visceral pain, for muscle pain or for both, and, as such, are very general.

Back to Top | Article Outline

The convergent-projection theory

Based on the ideas of Sturge72 and Ross,73 Ruch74 proposed that afferent fibers from different tissues converge onto common spinal neurons (Fig. 3A). The foundation of this suggestion is that the nociceptive activity from the spinal cord is misinterpreted as originating from other structures. This could explain the segmental nature of referred muscle pain and the increased referred pain intensity recorded when local muscle pain was intensified.6,7,16,48 However, it does not adequately explain the apparent delay in the development of referred pain after local pain.4,5,7,23 Also, referred pain has not been shown to be a stereotypical bidirectional phenomenon (e.g., muscle pain in the anterior tibial muscle produces pain in the ventral part of the ankle, but the opposite condition has not been shown). However, jaw muscle pain can be referred to the teeth, and tooth pain can be referred to the muscles. Finally, the threshold for eliciting local and referred muscle pain is different.7,23,43,44,75

FIG. 3

FIG. 3

Back to Top | Article Outline

The convergence-facilitation theory

MacKenzie76 was also inspired by the ideas of Sturge72 and Ross.73 He believed that viscera were totally insensitive and that nonnociceptive afferent input to the spinal cord created an irritable focus in the spinal cord (Fig 3B). This focus would make other somatic inputs appear in an abnormal fashion and, in some cases, be perceived to be referred pain. The theory was not recognized, mainly because it did not accept true visceral pain. In recent years, however, MacKenzie's simple idea of an irritable focus has reclaimed awareness under another term-central sensitization. The somatosensory sensibility changes reported in referred pain areas could in part be explained by similar mechanisms in the dorsal horn and brainstem neurons and the delay in appearance of referred pain shown in various studies6,7 could also be explained because the creation of central sensitization may require time.

Back to Top | Article Outline

The axon-reflex theory

Bifurcation of afferents from two different tissues has been suggested as an explanation of referred pain75 (Fig. 3C). Although bifurcation of nociceptive afferents from different tissues (muscle and skin77 and intervertebral discs and skin78) exists, generally it is agreed that these types of neurons are rare.79 Moreover, a time delay in the appearance of referred pain, different thresholds for eliciting local and referred muscle pain, and somatosensory sensibility changes in referred pain area cannot be explained by this theory.

Back to Top | Article Outline

The hyperexcitability theory

Numerous experimental and clinical studies (see previous section) have documented some effects of anesthetizing the referred pain area; therefore, referred pain may likely not be explained solely by a central mechanism, although the central component is assumed to be the most predominant.

The aforementioned theories lack some of the referred pain characteristics previously described in this article. Recently, Mense59 suggested an interesting theory, especially from a perspective regarding referred muscle pain, that is known as the central-hyperexcitability theory (Fig. 3D).

Recordings from a dorsal horn neuron in animals have revealed that noxious stimuli to a receptive field in a muscle generated within minutes at a distance from the original receptive field.3,54,58 The appearance of two new receptive fields could indicate that latent convergent afferents on the dorsal horn neuron may be opened by noxious stimuli arising from muscle tissue,59 and this facilitation of latent convergence connections could appear as referred pain. Recent observations from the same group have shown that substance P released from the terminal ends of primary afferents plays a role in the connectivity in the dorsal horn.80 Furthermore, an expansion of the receptive fields proximal to the normal receptive field was found in a study in which experimental myositis was induced, and, subsequently, application of antagonists to three different neurokinin receptors is effective in preventing the induced hyperexcitability.81 The idea of this theory is in line with several of the characteristics of referred muscle pain (dependency on stimulus and a delay in appearance of referred pain in comparison with local pain). The proximal appearance of receptive fields, thought of as referred pain, is in contrast to the reports from a majority of the experimentally referred pain studies, including healthy individuals.4-10,20,52,65 Clinical studies regarding the spread of experimentally induced referred pain in patients with whiplash syndrome and fibromyalgia have shown proximal and distal referral of pain.42,62 In only one study have we seen proximal spread of referred muscle pain in a few healthy volunteers after intramuscular injection of capsaicin. A possible explanation of the divergence in these observations could be that an already ongoing pain is necessary to have a massive barrage or to induce a state of hyperexcitability in the spinal cord. This results in proximal and distal referral in comparison with the predominant distal referral in healthy individuals.

The hyperexcitability theory3,58,59,82 is based on animal studies in which receptive fields appeared within minutes. This does not fit exactly with the development of referred pain in humans, which occurs within seconds. However, we think that the idea of latent connections between dorsal horn neurons is convincing. To explain the referred pain, which could not be anesthetized, supraspinal mechanisms that could mimic the mechanisms seen in the dorsal horn or brainstem regions can not be excluded. If the processing of local and referred muscle pain is not performed in the same supraspinal pathways and centers, neuroimaging techniques (positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonant imaging) might be possible to visualize the underlying nociceptive processing responsible for referred pain in humans.

Back to Top | Article Outline

The thalamic-convergence theory

Theobald83 suggested that referred pain appeared as a summation of input from the injured area and the referred pain area within neurons in the brain, and not in the spinal cord (Fig. 3E). A recent study of referred pain in monkeys that applied computer simulations has shown several pathways that converge on different subcortical and cortical neurons.84

Back to Top | Article Outline

CONCLUSION

Referred muscle pain has some fundamental features.

1. The size of referred pain is related to the intensity and duration of ongoing/evoked pain.

2. Temporal summation is a potent mechanism for generation of referred muscle pain.

3. Central hyperexcitability is important for the extent of referred pain.

4. Patients with chronic musculoskeletal pains have enlarged referred pain areas to experimental stimuli. Proximal spread of referred muscle pain is seen in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain and very seldom seen in healthy individuals.

5. Modality specific somatosensory changes occur in referred areas, which emphasizes the importance of using a multimodal sensory test regime for assessment.

Human experimental pain research has provided new possibilities to study referred pain quantitatively in volunteers and patients. Clinical studies and pharmacologic modulation of experimentally induced referred pain may contribute with additional information regarding the underlying mechanisms. Better characterization and understanding of referred pain mechanisms and related hyperalgesia may help to optimize and to rationalize pain management and clinical diagnosis.

Acknowledgments: The authors thank The Danish National Research Foundation, Copenhagen, Denmark, for supporting the time spent to write this paper, and Dr. Thomas Graven-Nielsen, Center for Sensory-Motor Interaction, Aalborg University, Aalborg, Denmark, for his contribution.

Back to Top | Article Outline

REFERENCES

1. Bonica JJ. General considerations of acute pain. In: Bonica JJ, ed. The Management of Pain, Vol 2. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger; 1990;7:159-79.
2. Head H. On disturbances of sensation with especial reference to the pain of visceral disease. Brain 1896;19:11-276.
3. Simons, D.G., Travell, J. Glossary. In: Simons DG, Travell JG, Simons L, eds. Myofascial pain and dysfunction. The trigger point manual. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins; 1998;2:1-10.
4. Graven-Nielsen T, Arendt-Nielsen L, Svensson P, et al. Quantification of local and referred muscle pain in humans after sequential i.m. injections of hypertonic saline. Pain 1997;69:111-7.
5. Graven-Nielsen T, Arendt-Nielsen L, Svensson P, et al. Stimulus-response functions in areas with experimentally induced referred muscle pain-a psychophysical study. Brain Res 1997;744:121-8.
6. Graven-Nielsen T, McArdle A, Phoenix J, et al. In vivo model of muscle pain: quantification of intramuscular chemical, electrical, and pressure changes associated with saline-induced muscle pain in humans. Pain 1997;69:137-43.
7. Laursen RJ, Graven-Nielsen T, Jensen TS, et al. Quantification of local and referred pain in humans induced by intramuscular electrical stimulation. Eur J Pain 1997;1:105-13.
8. Laursen RJ, Graven-Nielsen T., Jensen TS, et al. Referred pain is dependent on sensory input from the periphery: a psycophysical study. Eur J Pain 1998;1:261-9.
9. Laursen R, Graven-Nielsen T, Jensen TS, et al. The effect of compression and regional anaesthetic block on referred pain intensity in humans. Pain 1999;80:257-63.
10. Laursen RJ, Graven-Nielsen T, Jensen TS, Arendt-Nielsen L. The effect of differential and complete nerve block on experimental muscle pain in humans. Muscle Nerve 1999;22:1564-70.
11. Svensson P, Arendt-Nielsen L, Houe L. Sensory-motor interactions of human experimental unilateral jaw muscle pain: a quantitative analysis. Pain 1995;64:241-9.
12. Svensson P, Arendt-Nielsen L, Nielsen H, Larsen JK. Effect of chronic and experimental jaw muscle pain on pain-pressure thresholds and stimulus-response curves. J Orofacial Pain 1995;9:347-56.
13. Svensson P, Graven-Nielsen T, Arendt-Nielsen L. Mechanical hyperesthesia of human facial skin induced by tonic painful stimulation of jaw-muscles. Pain 1997;74:93-100.
14. Svensson P, Arendt-Nielsen L, Houe L. Muscle pain modulates mastication: an experimental study in man. J. Orofacial Pain 1998;12:7-16.
15. Svensson P, McMillan AS, Graven-Nielsen T, Wang K, Arendt-Nielsen L. Modulation of an inhibitory reflex in single motor units in human masseter by tonic painful stimulation. Pain 1999;83:441-446.
16. Arendt-Nielsen L. Induction and assessment of experimental pain from human skin, muscle and viscera. In: Jensen TS, Turner JA, Wiesenfeld-Hallin Z, eds. Proceedings of the 8th World Congress on Pain, Vancouver, Canada. Seattle: IASP Press;1997:393-425.
17. Poulsen L, Arendt-Nielsen L, Brøsen K, et al. The hypoalgesic effect of imipramine in different human experimental pain models. Pain 1995;60:287-93
18. Brennum J, Petersen KL, Horn A, et al. Quantitative sensory examination of epidural anaesthesia and analgesia in man: dose response effect of bupivacaine. Pain 1994;56:315-26.
19. Newham DJ, Edwards RHT, Mills KR, et al. Skeletal muscle pain. In: Wall PD, Melzack R, eds. Textbook of Pain, 3rd ed. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone; 1994;423-40.
20. Kellgren JH. Observation on referred pain arising from muscle. Clin Sci 1938;3:175-90.
21. Lewis T. Suggestions relating to the study of somatic pain, BMJ 1938;1:321-5.
22. Kellgren JH. On the distribution of pain arising from deep somatic structures with charts of segmental pain areas. Clin Sci 1939;4:35-46.
23. Inman VT, Saunders JBCM. Referred pain from skeletal structures. J Nerv Ment Dis 1944;99:660-7.
24. Feinstein B, Langton JNK, Jameson RM, et al. Experiments on pain referred from deep somatic tissues. J Bone Joint Surg 1954;36:981-97.
25. Hockaday JM, Whitty CWM. Patterns of referred pain in the normal subject. Brain 1967;90:481-96.
26. Meadows JC. Observations on muscle pain in man, with particular reference to pain during needle electromyography. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1970;33:519-23.
27. Vecchiet L, Galletti R, Giamberardino MA, et al. Modifications of cutaneous, subcutaneous, and muscular sensory and pain thresholds after the induction of an experimental focus in the skeletal muscle. Clin J Pain 1988;4:55-9.
28. Vecchiet L, Giamberardino MA, Dragani L, et al. Latent myofascial trigger points: changes in muscular and subcutaneous pain thresholds at trigger point and target level. J Man Med 1990;5:151-4.
29. Vecchiet L, Dragani L, De Bigontina P, et al Experimental referred pain and hyperalgesia from muscles in humans. In: Vecchiet L, Albe-Fessard D, Lindblom U, eds. New trends in referred pain and hyperalgesia. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers BV; 1993:239-49.
30. Graven-Nielsen T, Fenger-Gron LS, Svensson P, et al. Quantification of deep and superficial sensibility in saline-induced muscle pain-a psychophysical study. Somatosens Mot Res 1998;5:46-53.
31. Madeleine P, Lundager B, Voigt M, et al. Shoulder muscle coordination under chronic and acute neck-shoulder pain. An occupational pain study. Eur J Appl Physiol 1998;79:127-40.
32. Graven-Nielsen T, McArdle A, Phoenix J, et al. In vivo model of muscle pain: quantification of intramuscular chemical, electrical, and pressure changes associated with saline-induced muscle pain in humans. Pain 1997;69:137-43.
33. Kantor TG, Jarvik ME, Wolff BB. Bradykinin as a mediator of human pain. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 1967;126:505-7.
34. Pedersen-Bjergaard U, Nielsen LB, Jensen K, et al. Algesia and local responses induced by neurokinin A and substance P in human skin and temporal muscle. Peptides 1989;10:1147-52.
35. Jensen K, Tuxen C, Pedersen-Bjergaard U, et al. Pain and tenderness in human temporal muscle induced by bradykinin and 5-hydroxytryptamine. Peptides 1990;11:1127-32.
36. Babenko V, Graven-Nielsen T, Svensson P, et al. Experimental human muscle pain and muscular hyperalgesia induced by combinations of serotonin and bradykinin. Pain 1999;82:1-8.
37. Babenko V, Graven-Nielsen T, Svensson P, et al. Experimental human muscle pain induced by intramuscular injections of bradykinin, serotonin, and substance P. Eur J Pain 1999;3:93-102.
38. Marchettini P, Simone DA, Caputi G, et al. Pain from excitation of identified muscle nociceptors in humans. Brain Res 1996;740:109-16.
39. Witting N, Svensson P, Gottrup H, et al. Intramuscular and intradermal injection of capsaicin a comparison of local and referred pain. Pain 2000;84:407-412.
40. Arima T, Svensson P, Arendt-Nielsen L. Capsaicin-induced muscle hyperalgesia in exercised and non-exercised human masseter. J Orofacial Pain 2000;14:213-23.
41. Vecchiet L, Montanari, G, Pizzigallo, E, et al. Sensory characterization of somatic parietal tissues in humans with chronic fatigue syndrome. Neurosci Lett 1996;208:117-20.
42. Graven-Nielsen T, Aspegren-Kendall S, Henriksson KG, et al. Ketamine attenuates experimental referred muscle pain and temporal summation in fibromyalgia patients In: Proceedings of 9th World Congress on Pain, Vienna, Austria. Seattle: IASP Press; 1999:516.
43. Torebjörk HE, Ochoa JL, Schady W. Referred pain from intraneural stimulation of muscle fascicles in the median nerve. Pain 1984;18:145-56.
44. Hong CZ, Chen YN, Twehous D, et al. Pressure threshold for referred pain on the trigger point and adjacent areas. J Musculos Pain 1996;4:61-79.
45. Simone DA, Marchettini P, Caputi G, et al. Identification of muscle afferents subserving sensation of deep pain in humans. J Neurophysiol 1994;72:883-9.
46. Nielsen J, Arendt-Nielsen L. Spatial summation of heat induced pain within and between dermatomes. Somatosens Mot Res 1997;14:119-25.
47. Arendt-Nielsen L, Andersen OK, Jensen TS. Brief, prolonged and repeated stimuli applied to hyperalgesic skin areas: a psychophysical study Brain Res 1996;712:165-7.
48. Drewes AM, Arendt-Nielsen L., Jensen JH, et al. Experimental pain in the stomach: a model based on electrical stimulation guided by gastroscopy. Gut 1997;41:753-7.
49. Handwerker HO, Kobal G. Psychophysiology of experimentally induced pain. Physiol Rev 1998;73:639-71.
50. Hoheisel U, Mense S. Long-term changes in discharge behaviour of cat dorsal horn neurones following noxious stimulation of deep tissues. Pain 1989;36:239-47.
51. Hu JW, Sessle BJ, Raboisson P, et al. Stimulation of craniofacial muscle afferents induces prolonged facilitatory effects in trigeminal nociceptive brain-stem neurones. Pain 1992;48:53-60.
52. Inman VT, Saunders JBCM. Referred pain from skeletal structures. J Nerv Ment Dis 1944;99:660-7.
53. Foerster O. The dermatomes in man. Brain 1933;56:1-39.
54. Hoheisel U, Mense S. Response behaviour of cat dorsal horn neurones receiving input from skeletal muscle and other deep somatic tissues. J Physiol (Lond) 1990;426:265-80.
55. Sessle BJ, Hu JW, Amano N, Zhong G. Convergence of cutaneous, tooth pulp, visceral, neck and muscle afferents onto nociceptive and non-nociceptive neurones in trigeminal subnucleus caudalis (medullary dorsal horn) and its implication for referred pain. Pain 1986;27:219-35.
56. Wall PD, Woolf CJ. Muscle but not cutaneous C-afferent input produces prolonged increase in the excitability of the flexion reflex in the rat. J Physio 1984;356:443-58.
57. Cook AJ, Woolf CJ, Wall PD, et al. Dynamic receptive field plasticity in rat spinal cord dorsal horn following C-primary afferent input. Nature 1987;325:151-3.
58. Hoheisel U, Mense S, Simons DG, et al. Appearance of new receptive fields in rat dorsal horn neurons following noxious stimulation of skeletal muscle: a model for referral of muscle pain? Neurosci Lett 1993;153:9-12.
59. Mense S. Referral of muscle pain. American Pain Society Journal 1994;3:1-9.
60. Stohler CS, Lund JP. Psychophysical and orofacial motor response to muscle pain-validation and utility of an experimental model. In: Morimoto T, Matsuya T, Takada K, eds. Brain and oral functions. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science B.V.; 1995:227-37.
61. Sörensen J, Graven-Nielsen T, Henriksson KG, et al. Hyperexcitability in fibromyalgia. J Rheumatol 1998;25:152-5.
62. Johansen MK, Graven-Nielsen T, Olesen AS, et al. Generalised muscular hyperalgesia in chronic whiplash syndrome. Pain 1999;83:229-34.
63. Svensson P, List T, Hector G. Analysis of stimulus-evoked pain in patients with myofascial temporomandibular pain disorders. Pain 2001. In press.
64. Jensen K, Norup M Experimental pain in human temporal muscle induced by hypertonic saline, potassium and acidity. Cephalalgia 1992;12:101-6.
65. Arendt-Nielsen L, Graven-Nielsen T, Svensson P, et al. Temporal summation in muscles and referred pain areas: an experimental human study. Muscle Nerve 1997;20:1311-3.
66. Graven-Nielsen T, Babenko V, Svensson P, et al. Experimentally induced muscle pain induces hypoalgesia in heterotopic deep tissues, but not in homotopic deep tissues. Brain Res 1998;787:203-10.
67. Weiss S, Davis D. The significance of the afferent impulses from the skin in the mechanism of visceral pain. Skin infiltration as a useful therapeutic measure. Am J Med Sci 1928;176:517-36.
68. Woollard HH, Roberts JEH, Carmichael EA. An inquiry into referred pain. Lancet 1932;1:337-8.
69. Whitty CWM, Willison RG. Some aspects of referred pain. Lancet 1958;2:226-31.
70. Campbell JN, Meyer RA. Cutaneous nociceptors. In: Belmonte C, Cervero F, eds. Neurobiology of nociceptors. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1996:117-45.
71. Jones CM. Pain from the digestive tract. Proc Ass Res Nerv Ment Dis 1943;23:274-89.
72. Sturge WA. The phenomena of angina pectoris and their bearing upon the theory of counter-irritation. Brain 1883;5:492-510.
73. Ross J. On the segmental distribution of sensory disorder. Brain 1888;10:333-61.
74. Ruch TC. Pathophysiology of pain. In: Ruch TC, Patton HD, Woodbury JW, eds. Neurophysiology. Philadelphia: WB Saunders;1961:350-68.
75. Sinclair DC, Weddell G, Feindel WH. Referred pain and associated phenomena. Brain 1948;71:184-211.
76. MacKenzie J. Some points bearing on the association of sensory disorders and visceral disease. Brain 1893;16:321-53.
77. Mense S, Light AR, Perl ER. Spinal terminations of subcutaneous high-threshold mechanoreceptors. In: Brown AG, Rèthelyi M, eds. Spinal cord sensations. Edinburgh: Scottish Academic Press;1981:79-84.
78. Takahashi Y, Sato A, Nakamura SI, et al. Regional correspondence between the ventral portion of the lumbar intervertebral disc and the groin mediated by a spinal reflex. A possible basis of discogenic referred pain. Spine 1998;23:1853-9.
79. McMahon SB. Mechanisms of cutaneous, deep and visceral pain. In: Wall PD, Melzack R, eds. Textbook of pain. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone;1994:129-51.
80. Hoheisel U, Mense S, Ratkai M. Effects of spinal cord superfusion with substance P on the excitability of rat dorsal horn neurons processing input from deep tissues. J Musculoskel Pain 1995;3:23-44.
81. Hoheisel U, Sander B, Mense S. Myositis-induced functional reorganisation of the rat dorsal horn: effects of spinal superfusion with antagonists to neurokinin and glutamate receptors. Pain 1997;69:219-30.
82. Mense S. Nociception from skeletal muscle in relation to clinical muscle pain. Pain 1993;54:241-89.
83. Theobald GW. The role of the cerebral cortex in the apperception of pain. Lancet 1949;257:41-7.
84. Apkarian AV, Brüggemann J, Shi T, et al A thalamic model for true and referred visceral pain. In: Gebhart GF, ed. Visceral pain, Progress in Pain Research and Management. Seattle: IASP Press, 1995;5:217-59.
85. Selzer M, Spencer WA. Convergence of visceral and cutaneous afferent pathways in the lumbar spinal cord. Brain Res 1969;14:331-48.
Keywords:

Muscle Pain; Referred pain; Experimental; Hyperexcitability

Copyright © 2001 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.