Cognitive Outcomes After Infant Spinal Anesthesia: The Other Side of the Coin : Anesthesia & Analgesia

Secondary Logo

Journal Logo

Editorials: Editorial

Cognitive Outcomes After Infant Spinal Anesthesia

The Other Side of the Coin

Nemergut, Michael E. MD, PhD*†; Crow, Sheri MD†; Flick, Randall P. MD, MPH*†

Author Information
Anesthesia & Analgesia 119(3):p 514-515, September 2014. | DOI: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000000343
  • Free

Few issues in pediatric anesthesia have garnered more attention from the research, clinical, and regulatory communities than the possibility that adverse neurocognitive outcomes result from the administration of general anesthetics to young children. While preclinical studies are nearly uniformly positive, data from human studies are far less consistent. Virtually all human studies conducted so far have been retrospective, rely on a database not designed to study the outcome of interest, and exhibit profound differences with regard to the specific outcome measured.1,2 Criticisms of these studies are numerous, appropriate, and frequently center on 2 fundamental issues: confounding secondary to comorbidity and the coincidence of anesthetic exposure with a surgical procedure. In essence, do these studies truly measure the effects of anesthesia or are they simply studies of cognitive outcome among those needing surgery or the effect of the surgical procedure itself?

Several studies from the Mayo Clinic, Columbia University Medical Center, and elsewhere have attempted to control for comorbidity.3–6 These studies suggest, but by no means prove, that medical comorbidity is inadequate as an explanation for the decrement in cognitive performance observed especially in those with multiple anesthetic and surgical exposures. If one accepts that comorbidity can be accounted for through adjustment or matching, the major known confounder remaining is that attributed to the effects of the surgical procedure.

Like opposite sides of the same coin, the effects of anesthetic and surgical exposure are extremely difficult to separate, particularly in a retrospective study. However, in this month’s issue of Anesthesia & Analgesia, Williams et al.7 have attempted to do just that and in doing so provide insight into one of the primary criticisms of the extant clinical literature.

The study by Williams et al.7 is the first to specifically examine whether a surgical procedure devoid of exposure to general anesthetics would produce the same cognitive effects as those procedures with general anesthetic exposure. The fundamental assumption of the study is, of course, that exposure to local anesthetics placed in the subarachnoid space would not be associated with subsequent neurodevelopmental injury. This assumption, while supported by preclinical data, has not been rigorously studied in the clinical setting and, as such, is plausible but still conjectural.8,9

In their study, Williams et al.7 sought to isolate the effect of surgery from that of general anesthesia by retrospectively comparing the cognitive outcomes of a cohort of infants who had undergone a brief surgical procedure under spinal anesthesia to normative data for the population of Vermont.7 The authors queried the Vermont Infant Spinal Registry database, a unique database composed of children who have undergone surgery by spinal anesthesia since 1979. They found 265 children who had received a solitary exposure to spinal anesthesia for 1 of 3 surgical procedures: circumcision, pyloromyotomy, or inguinal herniorrhaphy. These children were matched by age, gender, need for a free/reduced school lunch, and year of examination to unexposed children. Borrowing from the recent study by Block et al.,10 the primary outcome was children with very poor academic achievement (VPAA) defined as scoring below the fifth percentile on a group-administered test of achievement. Reading and math scores, as well as need for an individualized educational program, were evaluated as secondary outcomes. They found that children who had received a single exposure to spinal anesthetics during infancy did not differ significantly to unexposed children with regard to VPAA or need for an individualized educational program. Furthermore, standardized test scores were not negatively correlated with duration of surgery. These findings are in direct contrast to those of Block et al.,10 who found in a similarly designed study of exposure to general anesthetics and surgery an increase in VPAA among those exposed compared with the population at large.10 From these data, Williams et al.7 concluded that the provision of a single spinal anesthetic was not associated with VPAA and that there was no link between the duration of surgery and academic achievement scores.

The obvious implications of the Williams et al.7 study are that the observed decrement in performance associated with surgical/anesthetic in prior studies cannot be attributed to the surgery but must result from some other factor. Whether that factor is N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor and/or γ-aminobutyric acid receptor active agents that comprise virtually all our anesthetics remains to be determined. Interestingly, as the studies by Williams et al.7 and Block et al.10 together loosely resemble a retrospective version of the General Anesthesia Spinal Anesthesia (GAS) study now under way at several U.S. and international sites, it is tempting to ponder whether these 2 studies may or may not foreshadow the results of the GAS study. Indeed, while past editorials have cautioned against equating the relationship between general anesthesia and neurologic outcome as causal,2 we find ourselves stressing the opposite side of that coin. While Williams et al.7 did not find significant differences between exposed and unexposed children in their study, we caution against concluding that no difference exists. Studies evaluating single anesthetic/surgical exposures, as well as those using group-administered tests of achievement as outcome measures have commonly not shown differences in cognitive outcome in exposed children.5,6,11–14 The absence of a positive control (such as a group with exposure to general anesthesia) and the observation that the upper bound for the confidence intervals for VPAA for math and reading in this study (2.68 and 2.1, respectively) are similar to those reported from larger, positive studies2 suggest that the current study lacked the statistical power necessary to discriminate between exposed and unexposed children unless their cognitive differences were relatively large, likely larger than those reported in prior studies that evaluated children with multiple exposures to general anesthesia. As such, one must strongly caution against using the current data as justification for prematurely flipping the general/regional anesthetic coin in favor of regional techniques, the risks of which were not quantified in this study. Therefore, the critical difference between this study, the study by Block et al.,10 and other retrospective studies and the GAS study is that the GAS study is the only truly randomized controlled trial under way to evaluate the cognitive effects of general and spinal anesthesia. That study will for the most part be free of the confounding and bias that plagues this study and all others published to date.

The authors are to be commended for their contribution in assessing whether a surgical procedure may contribute to adverse cognitive outcome later in childhood. The Pediatric Anesthesia & Neurodevelopment Assessment (Columbia) and Mayo Anesthesia Safety in Kids (Mayo) studies, although not randomized controlled trials, will augment the insights gained from GAS. None, however, will report definitive outcomes for at least 2 or 3 years. In the meantime, we must content ourselves with very important but severely limited studies such as that published by Williams et al.7 in this issue of Anesthesia & Analgesia.7 He and his colleagues have provided us with a glimpse at the previously unseen flip side of the anesthesia-surgery coin.

DISCLOSURES

Name: Michael E. Nemergut, MD, PhD.

Contribution: This author helped design the editorial and write the manuscript.

Attestation: Michael E. Nemergut approved the final manuscript.

Name: Sheri Crow, MD.

Contribution: This author helped design the editorial and write the manuscript.

Attestation: Sheri Crow approved the final manuscript.

Name: Randall P. Flick, MD, MPH.

Contribution: This author helped design the editorial and write the manuscript.

Attestation: Randall P. Flick approved the final manuscript.

This manuscript was handled by: Peter J. Davis, MD.

REFERENCES

1. Nemergut ME, Aganga D, Flick RP. Anesthetic neurotoxicity: what to tell the parents? Paediatr Anaesth. 2014;24:120–6
2. Flick RP, Warner DO. A users’ guide to interpreting observational studies of pediatric anesthetic neurotoxicity: the lessons of Sir Bradford Hill. Anesthesiology. 2012;117:459–62
3. DiMaggio C, Sun LS, Kakavouli A, Byrne MW, Li G. A retrospective cohort study of the association of anesthesia and hernia repair surgery with behavioral and developmental disorders in young children. J Neurosurg Anesthesiol. 2009;21:286–91
4. DiMaggio C, Sun LS, Li G. Early childhood exposure to anesthesia and risk of developmental and behavioral disorders in a sibling birth cohort. Anesth Analg. 2011;113:1143–51
5. Flick RP, Katusic SK, Colligan RC, Wilder RT, Voigt RG, Olson MD, Sprung J, Weaver AL, Schroeder DR, Warner DO. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes after early exposure to anesthesia and surgery. Pediatrics. 2011;128:e1053–61
6. Sprung J, Flick RP, Katusic SK, Colligan RC, Barbaresi WJ, Bojanić K, Welch TL, Olson MD, Hanson AC, Schroeder DR, Wilder RT, Warner DO. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder after early exposure to procedures requiring general anesthesia. Mayo Clin Proc. 2012;87:120–9
7. Williams RK, Black IH, Howard DB, Adams DC, Matthews DM, Friend AF, Meyers HWB. Cognitive outcome after spinal anesthesia and surgery during infancy. Anesth Analg. 2014;119:651–60
8. Flick RP, Lee K, Hofer RE, Beinborn CW, Hambel EM, Klein MK, Gunn PW, Wilder RT, Katusic SK, Schroeder DR, Warner DO, Sprung J. Neuraxial labor analgesia for vaginal delivery and its effects on childhood learning disabilities. Anesth Analg. 2011;112:1424–31
9. Yahalom B, Athiraman U, Soriano SG, Zurakowski D, Carpino EA, Corfas G, Berde CB. Spinal anesthesia in infant rats: development of a model and assessment of neurologic outcomes. Anesthesiology. 2011;114:1325–35
10. Block RI, Thomas JJ, Bayman EO, Choi JY, Kimble KK, Todd MM. Are anesthesia and surgery during infancy associated with altered academic performance during childhood? Anesthesiology. 2012;117:494–503
11. Flick RP, Nemergut ME, Christensen K, Hansen TG. Anesthetic-related neurotoxicity in the young and outcome measures: the devil is in the details. Anesthesiology. 2014;120:1303–5
12. Ing CH, DiMaggio CJ, Malacova E, Whitehouse AJ, Hegarty MK, Feng T, Brady JE, von Ungern-Sternberg BS, Davidson AJ, Wall MM, Wood AJ, Li G, Sun LS. Comparative analysis of outcome measures used in examining neurodevelopmental effects of early childhood anesthesia exposure. Anesthesiology. 2014;120:1319–32
13. Hansen TG, Pedersen JK, Henneberg SW, Pedersen DA, Murray JC, Morton NS, Christensen K. Academic performance in adolescence after inguinal hernia repair in infancy: a nationwide cohort study. Anesthesiology. 2011;114:1076–85
14. Wilder RT, Flick RP, Sprung J, Katusic SK, Barbaresi WJ, Mickelson C, Gleich SJ, Schroeder DR, Weaver AL, Warner DO. Early exposure to anesthesia and learning disabilities in a population-based birth cohort. Anesthesiology. 2009;110:796–804
© 2014 International Anesthesia Research Society