Anesthetic Effects on the Progression of Parkinson Disease in the Rat DJ-1 Model : Anesthesia & Analgesia

Secondary Logo

Journal Logo

Featured Articles: Original Laboratory Research Report

Anesthetic Effects on the Progression of Parkinson Disease in the Rat DJ-1 Model

Xu, Daniel A. BS; DeYoung, Timothy P. MSN, RN; Kondoleon, Nicholas P. MD; Eckenhoff, Roderic G. MD; Eckenhoff, Maryellen F. PhD

Author Information
Anesthesia & Analgesia 133(5):p 1140-1151, November 2021. | DOI: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000005665

Abstract

See Article, p 1138

KEY POINTS

  • Question: Does anesthesia accelerate the onset of Parkinson disease symptoms and pathology in an individual at risk for Parkinson disease?
  • Findings: Isoflurane transiently enhanced dopaminergic neurodegeneration in the substantia nigra without inducing detectable long-term functional deficits.
  • Meaning: In this animal model, isoflurane did not accelerate the onset of Parkinson disease.

Parkinson disease (PD) is the second most common neurodegenerative disorder, preceded only by Alzheimer disease. Anesthetics have been associated with the acceleration of neurodegenerative disorders in preclinical models, including Alzheimer disease1–9 and Huntington disease.10 Despite similar biophysical features of the underlying proteinopathy, less is known about an association of anesthetics with PD. This is important especially among older individuals, who are also by far the most likely to be exposed to general anesthetics and surgery. A case report described the onset of frank Parkinsonian symptoms in a 60-year-old patient immediately after surgery, which resolved during the postoperative period.11 However, 1 year later, the same patient was diagnosed with PD, suggesting that the perioperative period either transiently unmasked PD symptoms or actually accelerated PD pathogenesis. In addition, a study reported a significant increase in mortality due to PD in anesthesiologists compared to internists.12 In that study, the anesthesiologists would have been practicing in less well-scavenged operating rooms than today. Consistent with this is a case report of a 59-year-old anesthesiologist who developed PD, with evidence of poor anesthetic gas scavenging at his hospital.13 A knockout (KO) rat model of PD (DJ-1 protein, encoded by the Park7 gene [DJ-1; PARK7]), developed initially by SAGE Laboratories (now Horizon Discovery), has been shown to develop symptoms and neuropathology of PD by 8 months of age,14–16 including motor deficits and loss of dopaminergic neurons, with male rats developing symptoms more significantly than females. The DJ-1 model does not form alpha-synuclein aggregates. In this study, we hypothesized that repeated exposure to inhaled anesthetics in a presymptomatic rat model of PD would accelerate the progression of disease. We tested this hypothesis by exposing presymptomatic 6-month-old DJ-1 rats to isoflurane 3 times and then measuring the progression of neuropathology and symptoms acutely and up to 12 months of age.

METHODS

Detailed methods can be found in the Supplemental Digital Content 1, Materials and Methods, https://links.lww.com/AA/D609.

Animals

The study was approved by the University of Pennsylvania Animal Care and Use Committee. All animals were treated in strict accordance with National Institutes of Health (NIH) and institutional guidelines, the United States Public Health Service Policy on Humane Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.17 The Animal Research: Reporting of In Vivo Experiments guidelines18 were consulted when designing the experiments. Forty-eight male homozygous DJ-1 (−/−) (PARK7) KO rats, a previously characterized model of PD14 developed from Long Evans hooded rats, were obtained at 8 weeks of age from SAGE Laboratories (Boyertown, now Horizon Discovery). Males show greater PD progression than females in this model.14

Anesthetic Exposures and Monitoring

All rats were 6 months old at the time of the first anesthetic exposures. The exposed animals (maximum 9 rats at 1 time) were placed in a large (27” × 14” × 12”) gas-tight acrylic chamber (Cleatech). The chamber was equipped with gloves so rats could be manipulated and monitored without opening the chamber. Isoflurane (2% in 30% oxygen balanced with nitrogen for induction, followed by 1.2% isoflurane for maintenance) flowed into the vented chamber. A gas analyzer monitored the isoflurane, oxygen, and carbon dioxide concentrations. Heart rate, respiratory rate, oxygen saturation (Spo2; PhysioSuite and rat paw sensor, Kent Scientific), and rectal temperature were recorded every 10 to 15 minutes on select rats. The rats were anesthetized for 2 hours, and anesthesia was repeated at 2 and 4 weeks later. If any rat showed signs of pain or distress, including significant loss of body weight (body conditioning scores [BC] of BC1 or BC2 with other signs including piloerection, rough/stained haircoat, abnormal arched back, porphyrin staining), it would be immediately euthanized. Body weight for all groups from 6 months of age until euthanasia was recorded (Supplemental Digital Content 2, Figure S1, https://links.lww.com/AA/D610).

Behavioral Testing

F1
Figure 1.:
Experimental timeline. A, The acute group of DJ-1 rats underwent 2 h isoflurane exposures every 2 wk from 6 to 7 mo of age. Motor coordination testing (ladder rung and accelerating rotarod) was conducted 1–2 d after each exposure and 1–2 d before the third exposure. The acute group was euthanized after the last behavioral testing, at approximately 7 mo of age. B, The long-term group underwent the same 2 h isoflurane exposures from 6 to 7 mo of age. The ladder rung and rotarod testing were conducted 4 wk after the third exposure and then every 3–4 wk until 12 mo of age. At 12 mo of age, the NOR memory test was also conducted in a subset of animals. All rats in the long-term group were euthanized following the behavioral testing at around 12 mo of age. DJ-1 indicates the protein, DJ-1, encoded by the Park7 gene; NOR, novel object recognition.

Motor function and memory were assessed using the ladder rung test, rotarod, and object recognition, as shown in the timeline (Figure 1). Our primary outcome for this study was coordinated motor behavior. The testing regimen was different for the acute group (AG) and the long-term group (LTG), on the basis of our hypotheses. For the AG, our hypothesis was that the animals’ motor behavior would acutely change within days after the exposure, and that cumulative exposures would worsen the progression, as has been reported in wild-type and transgenic Alzheimer mouse models. We tested the AG after each of the 3 exposures, 2 weeks apart. In addition, we tested right before and right after the third exposure to examine the before-and-after scenario. The hypothesis for the LTG was that after the same 3 exposures, the progression of PD would be accelerated with age. The AG underwent ladder rung (1 day after each isoflurane exposure) and rotarod (2 days after each exposure) testing, ending at 7 months of age. In addition, the AG underwent the same motor tests 1 and 2 days before the third exposure. The LTG began the same testing regimen 4 weeks after the third exposure and then again at 3- to 4-week intervals until they were 12 months of age. Cognitive dysfunction was not a primary outcome for this study as cognitive deficits are not early or pronounced in patients with PD, especially in midlife, and cognitive dysfunction has not been reported in this model. We decided to evaluate a subset of animals in the LTG with the novel object recognition (NOR) test as a secondary end point to determine whether 12-month-old PD rats exhibited cognitive decline.

Horizontal Ladder Rung Task

F2
Figure 2.:
AG motor skills testing. A, The total number of foot faults on the ladder rung test 1 d after each of the 3 anesthetic exposures for the AG of CON (black) and ISO (red) rats showed no overall significant differences between groups. Within groups, the rats’ total foot faults decreased significantly from the first to the third exposure and between the second and third exposures for the controls and exposed rats. n = 6 for controls and exposed groups, *P < .05, **P < .01, ***P < .001. B, The total number of foot faults on the ladder rung test a day before and a day after the third exposure showed no significant differences between groups and no difference before or after the exposures for the control and exposed groups, as well as no difference between controls and isoflurane group before and after the third exposure. n = 6 for controls and exposed groups. C, The total number of foot faults after each of the 3 anesthetic exposures for each ladder rung pattern. There were no significant differences for the 3-way interactions (exposure, time point, and rung pattern). On ladder rung pattern 1, the exposed rats performed significantly better after the first exposure to after the third exposure and after the second exposure to after the third exposure. n = 6 for controls and exposed groups, *P < .05, **P < .01. D, The total number of foot faults was examined as a function of ladder rung pattern before and after the third exposure. While a significant effect for the 3-way interaction was found, no significant simple 2-way interactions were detected. Further analysis of the simple–simple comparisons showed that control rats performed significantly better than exposed rats after the third exposure on ladder rung pattern 2. n = 6 for controls and exposed groups, *P < .05. E, The 4 ladder rung patterns used for each rat at each time point are depicted. F, The time the AG animals spent on the rotarod before and after the third exposure was not significantly different between groups, and no differences were found in the exposed and control groups before and after the third exposure. n = 6 controls and n = 4 for exposed rats for the rotarod test. All data are presented as the mean with 95% CI. AG indicates acute group; CI, confidence interval; CON, control; ISO, isoflurane exposed.

The Horizontal Ladder Rung Test19 was used to assess motor function and coordination in the rats. After all animals completed the training, the test was repeated for 4 test trial runs, each with a different predetermined rung pattern (Figure 2E). The slow-motion video recordings were analyzed after the rats were euthanized, and the total number of foot faults were manually recorded for each of the 4 rung patterns. The data were analyzed for both the total number of foot faults for each rung pattern and the total number of foot faults per animal. This procedure was performed at 4 time points for the AG and 6 time points for the LTG.

Accelerating Rotarod

The Accelerating Rotarod Test20 was used to assess motor coordination and balance. The time spent on the rod was recorded for each animal. Animals were tested at 4 time points for the AG and 6 time points for the LTG. However, unexpectedly, some of the rats, irrespective of experimental group, refused to perform all 3 testing trials at some of the time points. The mean time spent on the rotarod at each time period was recorded for the analyses.

Novel Object Recognition

The NOR test21 was performed at 12 months of age in a subset of rats to assess cognitive function, specifically short- and long-term memory, and rearing. The rats were video-recorded and tracked using the AnyMaze software to determine the time spent exploring each object, as well as the time spent rearing.

Euthanasia

All rats were euthanized after their respective behavioral testing, at 7 months of age for the AG and 12 months of age for the LTG. Terminal surgery was performed under 4% to 5% isoflurane anesthesia balanced with 30% oxygen and nitrogen, by left ventricular intracardiac perfusion with ice cold phosphate-buffered saline and concomitant atrial exsanguination.

Immunohistochemistry

After euthanasia, the dissected brains were cut, fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde (Fisher Scientific), and processed for paraffin embedding. Rat brains from the AG and LTG were randomly chosen to be sectioned for immunohistochemistry. Due to technical issues with sectioning, the animal numbers for the AG were n = 5 controls, n = 6 exposed, and for the LTG n = 11 controls and n = 9 exposed. Slides were quantified by the same 2 blinded investigators, and the means of their results were used for each animal.

Statistical Analyses

The primary outcome was the ladder rung test, and secondary outcomes were the other behavioral and immunohistochemical assays. For the AG ladder rung and rotarod tests, sample size was determined using an expected large effect size f of 0.7 (due to acute nature of the AG), alpha error probability of .05, power of 0.80, 2 groups, 3 time points, correlation among repeated measures of 0.5, and nonsphericity of 1; the sample size was 6. For the LTG, using an expected medium effect size f of 0.3 (due to the long-term nature), alpha error probability of .05, power of 0.80, 2 groups, 6 time points, correlation among repeated measures of 0.5, and nonsphericity of 1, the sample size was 14. Because the DJ-1 model was newly developed with no data on mortality at 12 months of age, we allowed for possible illness or mortality by 12 months and increased sample size to n = 18. For the ladder rung test and rotarod data analyses for the AG and LTG, we used repeated measures 2-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and/or 3-way ANOVA. For the NOR test and the immunohistochemical assays, the sample size was determined based on the literature22,23 and from our laboratory,24–26 and an unpaired t test with Welch’s correction was used for the analyses, with the data tested for normality (Shapiro-Wilks test). Sample sizes are presented in the Results and in figure legends for each analysis. The statistical analyses were performed with unblinded data using GraphPad Prism v8.4 - 9.0 for Windows (GraphPad Software, www.graphpad.com) or R, v 4.0.2, (https://www.r-project.org/), and are described in detail in the Results for each analysis. For all data, α = .05 and significance was determined to be P < .05 (2-tailed); data are presented as the mean with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Data are available in the Harvard Dataverse online data repository (https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:10.7910/DVN/MKDXMP).

RESULTS

Behavioral Assays

The isoflurane-exposed and control DJ-1 rats underwent behavioral testing to assess motor and cognitive function (Figure 1).

Acute Group

To evaluate motor function, the AG underwent training on the ladder rung and rotarod immediately after each of the 3 isoflurane exposures to evaluate behavior with multiple exposures and before the third isoflurane exposure to examine behavior immediately before and after an exposure.

AG Ladder Rung

The rats had no difficulty navigating any of the rung patterns on the ladder rung test.

AG Total Number of Foot Faults

No significant differences were detected in the total number of foot faults with the repeated measures 2-way ANOVA with Geisser-Greenhouse correction, with variables of time and exposure, P = .10, F(2, 20) = 2.55, and with Sidak’s multiple comparisons tests between the control and the isoflurane-exposed groups after each of the 3 exposures (adjusted P values: P = .64, P = .97, P = .80, respectively; Figure 2A). However, both controls and exposed groups performed significantly better (decreased number of foot faults) from the first exposure to the third exposure (adjusted P values, P = .05 and P < .001, respectively) and from the second to the third exposure (adjusted P values, P = .01, P = .002, respectively; n = 6 rats per group; Figure 2A). In addition, no significant differences were detected in the AG tested immediately before and after the third exposure with the repeated measures 2-way ANOVA with Geisser-Greenhouse correction (time × exposure, P = .45, F(1, 10) = 0.63) and with Sidak’s multiple comparisons tests between the controls and exposure groups before and after the third exposure (adjusted P values: P = .12, P = .56, respectively; n = 6 rats per group; Figure 2B). There was also no difference in the control group before and after the exposures (adjusted P = .11) or in the isoflurane group (0.56).

AG Rung Patterns

No significant 3-way interactions between exposure, time point, and ladder rung pattern were detected for the AG, P = .71, F(6, 60) = 0.63, with the repeated measures 3-way ANOVA with Geisser-Greenhouse correction using a type III sum-of-squares (n = 6 rats per group; Figure 2C). On the other hand, 2-way interactions between exposure and ladder rung pattern, P = .02, F(3, 30) = 3.89, exposure and time point, P = .01, F(2, 20) = 5.60, and ladder rung pattern and time point, P = .02, F(6, 60) = 2.79, were all significant. However, when the effect of exposure was tested at each level of time point and ladder rung pattern, no significant P values were reported using Holm’s multiple comparisons test. By contrast, when the effect of time point was tested at each level of exposure and ladder rung pattern, the effect of time point was found to be significant for exposed animals on ladder rung pattern 1 (Figure 2C, first panel) using Holm’s multiple comparisons test (adjusted P = .04). Specifically, using Holm’s multiple comparisons test to perform post hoc simple–simple comparisons, we found that the exposed group performed significantly better (decreased number of foot faults) on ladder rung pattern 1 from the first exposure to the third exposure (adjusted P = .03) and from the second exposure to the third exposure (adjusted P = .01; Figure 2C, first panel).

For the “before and after the third exposure” analysis (Figure 2D), we used 3-way repeated measures ANOVA (exposure, time point, and ladder rung pattern) with Geisser-Greenhouse correction using a type III sum-of-squares (n = 6 rats per group) and observed a significant effect for the 3-way interaction, P = .05, F(3, 30) = 2.98. While further analysis revealed that none of the simple 2-way interactions were significant, the effect of ladder rung pattern specifically in control animals was found to be significant using Holm’s method to adjust for multiple comparisons, adjusted P = .01, F(3, 15) = 8.53. More specifically, examination of the simple–simple comparisons with Holm’s multiple comparisons correction showed that this could be attributable to control rats performing significantly better than exposed rats after the third exposure, specifically on ladder rung pattern 2 (adjusted P = .04; Figure 2D, second panel). The 4 ladder rung patterns are depicted in Figure 2E.

AG Rotarod

Due to inconsistency in animal performance on the rotarod task, as described in the Methods, no data were available for analysis after the first and second exposures for the AG. Rotarod performance before and after the third exposure was analyzed with repeated measures 2-way ANOVA, which detected no significant differences between the groups (time × exposure), P = .35, F(1,8) = 1.01. With Sidak’s multiple comparison tests, no differences were found between control and exposure groups (adjusted P values: P = .87 and P = .19, respectively) or within groups (adjusted P values: P = .79 and P = .20, respectively), before and after the third exposure (n = 6 controls, n = 4 exposed; Figure 2C).

Long-Term Group

The rats in the LTG were initially trained on the ladder rung and rotarod at 8 months of age (4 weeks after the third exposure) and then tested regularly (every 3–4 weeks) until 12 months of age.

LTG Total Number of Foot Faults

F3
Figure 3.:
LTG motor skills and cognitive testing. A, For the LTG, the total number of foot faults on the ladder rung test for the CON (black) and ISO (red) rats was determined at 6 time points, every 3–4 wk after the third exposure, until 12 mo of age. No significant differences were detected over time, and there were no significant differences between the controls and exposed rats at any time point. n = 18 control, and n = 16 isoflurane-exposed rats. B, The total number of foot faults for each rung pattern at each time point and for each exposure group was examined. There was no significant effect for the 3-way interaction, nor any significant effects for any of the 2-way interactions involving exposure. The effect of time point, however, was found to be significant for control animals on ladder rung pattern 1. n = 8 controls, and n = 11 exposed rats. C, The time spent on the rotarod for the LTG is shown at 7 wk after the third exposure (second time point) and then every 3–4 wk until 12 mo of age. No overall significant differences were detected with time, and there were no significant differences between the controls and exposed groups at any time point. In addition, there were no significant differences between the controls or exposed rats at any time points. n = 13 (controls) and n = 14 (isoflurane). D–F, The NOR test was used to assess cognitive changes in a subset of rats at 12 mo of age. There were no differences in short-term memory (D), long-term memory (E), or in the number of rearings (F) between controls and isoflurane-exposed rats. n = 8 (controls) and n = 9 (isoflurane). All data are presented as the mean with 95% CI. CI indicates confidence interval; CON, control; ISO, isoflurane exposed; LTG, long-term group; NOR, novel object recognition.

As with the AG, the rats in the LTG had no difficulty performing the ladder rung test. No significant differences were detected in the total number of foot faults (P = .15, n = 18 controls and n = 16 exposed rats) with repeated measures 2-way ANOVA with Geisser-Greenhouse correction with variables of time and exposure, P = .15, F(5, 160) = 1.66, nor between controls and isoflurane-exposed rats at each time point with Sidak’s multiple comparisons test (adjusted P values for the 6 time points, respectively, P > .99, P = .51, P = .41, P = .73, P > .99, P > .99; Figure 3A).

LTG Rung Patterns

For the ladder rung test, no significant 3-way interactions between exposure, time point, and ladder rung pattern were detected, P = .60, F(6.83, 116.08) = 0.78, with the repeated measures 3-way ANOVA with Geisser-Greenhouse correction using a type III sum-of-squares (n = 8 control animals, n = 11 exposed animals; Figure 3B). Video technicalities resulted in 10 controls and 5 exposed rats having to be omitted due to lack of complete data at each time point for the ladder rung pattern analyses. Moreover, none of the effects involving exposure were found to be significant, including the 2-way interaction between exposure and time point, P = .09, F(5, 85) = 1.99, the 2-way interaction between exposure and ladder rung pattern, P = .09, F(2.04, 34.64) = 2.59, and the main effect of exposure, P = .68, F(1, 17) = .172. However, the 2-way interaction between time point and ladder rung pattern was found to be significant, P = .03, F(6.83, 116.08) = 2.36; when further investigated, we found that when using Holm’s method to adjust for multiple comparisons, the effect of time point was significant for control animals on ladder rung pattern 1, adjusted P = .02, F(5, 35) = 4.57.

Rotarod Test

As with the AG, the LTG rats also frequently refused to perform the accelerated rotarod test. For the analyses, the best 2 of 3 trials were used for the repeated measures 2-way ANOVA with Geisser-Greenhouse correction. The first time point for the rotarod testing had to be omitted due to incomplete data, and for the remaining time points, 5 controls and 2 exposed rats had to be omitted due to lack of complete data at each time point (n = 13 controls, n = 14 exposed). No significant difference was detected in the repeated measure 2-way ANOVA (time × exposure), P = .08, F(4, 100) = 2.18, and with Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, there were no significant differences between the controls and exposed groups at any time point (adjusted P values, P = .69, P = .99, P = .99, P = .99, P = .98, time points 2–6, respectively; Figure 3C).

Novel Object Recognition

As a secondary end point, cognitive function in a subset of animals was examined at 12 months of age to test the hypothesis that anesthetics accelerated the onset of cognitive decline in PD-vulnerable rats. The NOR test was used to assess cognitive function (n = 8 controls, n = 9 exposed; technical error with tracking software resulted in no data for 1 control rat). There were no differences in short-term memory (P = .57; Figure 3D), long-term memory (P = .47; Figure 3E), or in the number of rearings (P = .10; Figure 3F) between controls and isoflurane-exposed rats using the unpaired t test with Welch’s correction.

Immunohistochemistry

Following their respective behavioral assays, the AG and LTG were euthanized for immunohistochemical assays (tyrosine hydroxylase [TH] and ionized calcium-binding adaptor protein-1 [Iba-1]) at 7 and 12 months of age, respectively.

Tyrosine Hydroxylase

F4
Figure 4.:
Tyrosine hydroxylase. A–D, Representative images of TH immunohistochemistry in the substantia nigra pars compacta from the acute group (A) controls and (B) isoflurane-exposed and the long-term group (C) controls and (D) exposed. Bar = 100 μm. E, The density of TH-positive neurons was significantly reduced in the isoflurane-exposed rats in the acute group at 7 mo of age. *P < .05. Controls, n = 5, and exposed, n = 6. F, There were no significant differences in the density of TH neurons between controls and exposed rats in the long-term group at 12 mo of age. Controls, n = 11, and exposed, n = 9. Data are presented as the mean with 95% CI. CI indicates confidence interval; TH, tyrosine hydroxylase.

Brain sections through the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNpc) were stained for TH from the AG and LTG groups (Figure 4A–D). The AG was found to have a significantly lower density of TH-containing cells in the SNpc (P = .04; n = 5 controls, n = 6 exposed) compared to controls using an unpaired t test with Welch’s correction (Figure 4E) while, for the LTG, no significant difference was detected in the density of these neurons (P = .24; n = 11 controls, n = 9 exposed; Figure 4F).

Ionized Calcium-Binding Adaptor Protein-1

F5
Figure 5.:
Microglial activation in the hippocampus. A–D, Representative images of Iba-1 immunohistochemistry in the hippocampal CA1 region. Bar = 100 μm. No significant differences were detected in the density of Iba-1–positive cells in the controls compared to exposed rats in either (E) the acute group or (F) the LTG. The AG: controls, n = 5, and exposed, n = 6; the LTG: controls, n = 14, and exposed, n = 12. Data are presented as the mean with 95% CI. AG indicates acute group; CI, confidence interval; Iba, ionized calcium-binding adaptor protein-1; LTG, long-term group.
F6
Figure 6.:
Microglial activation in the substantia nigra pars compacta. A–D, Representative images of Iba-1 immunohistochemistry in the substantia nigra pars compacta. Bar = 200 μm. No significant differences were detected in the density of Iba-1–positive microglia in the controls compared to exposed rats in either (E) the acute group, controls, n = 3, and exposed, n = 4 or (F) the long-term group, controls, n = 4, and exposed, n = 3. Data are presented as the mean with 95% CI. CI indicates confidence interval; Iba-1, ionized calcium-binding adaptor protein-1.

The hippocampal CA1 region (Figure 5A–D) and substantia nigra (Figure 6A–D) were examined for microglial activation, measured by the density of Iba-1–positive microglial cells, in the AG and LTGs. No significant differences between isoflurane-exposed and controls were detected in the density of microglial cells in the CA1 region in the AG or the LTG (P = .11 [n = 5 controls, n = 6 exposed], P = .54 [n = 14 controls, n = 12 exposed], respectively; Figure 5E, F). Likewise, the density of Iba-1–positive microglia in the SNpc showed no significant differences in the AGs (P = .82, n = 3 controls, n = 4 exposed) or LTGs (P = .38, n = 4 controls, n = 3 exposed; Figure 6E, F).

DISCUSSION

Anesthetic acceleration of amyloidopathy and tauopathy in preclinical animal models has been well documented, although less well documented in clinical studies. PD is another common proteinopathy, for which only limited clinical evidence of anesthetic modulation exists. Nevertheless, transient movement disorders resembling PD are fairly common during recovery from general anesthesia, and no studies have searched for an association between incident PD and prior anesthesia/surgery. Thus, we undertook this study with the hypothesis that isoflurane anesthesia alone would accelerate the neuropathology and symptomatology associated with an existing PD vulnerability. The DJ-1 rat is a PARK7 KO model and has been reported to develop dopaminergic neuron loss, as well as progressive movement disorders resembling PD compared to wild-type rats, with 50% of rats being affected by 8 months of age.14–16 This model therefore should allow the detection of either acceleration or deceleration of the disease process.

The ladder rung test was sufficiently sensitive to detect a learning effect in both groups over 6 weeks of testing; thus, our inability to detect an effect of the isoflurane exposure is unlikely to have been due to the use of an insufficiently sensitive assay. Similarly, there were no differences in weight gain (Supplemental Digital Content 2, Figure S1, https://links.lww.com/AA/D610), grooming, or other general features of daily living, all dependent to a degree on mobility and coordination. Despite this, we detected a significant decrease in TH-containing cells in the SNpc in the acute-exposed group. That the effect was not dramatic enough to elicit motor or behavioral changes is consistent with previous research suggesting that the much larger inflammatory insult of a surgical procedure is necessary to produce functional changes. Other possible explanations are that the animal either was robust enough to compensate for small decreases in dopaminergic signaling or had sufficient dopaminergic reserve to not produce a phenotype. Interestingly, and also consistent with our previous study,26 the effects caused by the anesthetic alone resolved as the animal aged to 12 months.

The mechanism(s) responsible for the transient change in TH expression is not clear. It may indicate an activation of neuroinflammation, although significant microglial activation was not found in either the hippocampus or substantia nigra. We and others have reported no increase in neuroinflammation after anesthesia alone,26,27 so this is not unexpected. Alternatively, the decrease in TH expression might be a direct effect of isoflurane alteration in dopaminergic signaling pathways,28 or a compensatory change in expression due to an anesthetic-induced increase in TH activity.29 Leikas et al,28 however, reported no changes in TH protein shortly after several brief exposures to isoflurane in a mouse model of PD, with improved sensorimotor function. Regardless, the transient acute decrease in TH expression is likely of little significance as it does not appear to be persistent or progressive, and therefore is unlikely to reflect an anesthetic acceleration of PD neuropathology.

There are several limitations and future directions from this study. Characterizing subtle motor defects in a quadruped can be challenging, and there are assays available other than those we used. However, the ladder rung task was sensitive enough to detect learning in the 2 groups, and therefore we believe should have detected a functionally relevant change due to the anesthetic, if present. Also, rats cannot always be successfully encouraged to complete a task, as we found in the rotarod task and noted elsewhere.30 Thus, our rotarod data are missing a considerable number of animals and should therefore be considered to have significant selection bias, although we do not know whether refusal to perform the rotarod indicates high or low performance. The transient decrease in the density of dopaminergic neurons may not be real and could be due to type I error, especially since the density was calculated as the number per area and was not performed using stereological methods. Another limitation is that only male DJ-1 rats were used in this study, based on previous research indicating that male rats develop symptoms more significantly than females.14 Finally, anesthesia is rarely given in the absence of surgery or some other painful and inflammation-provoking procedure. We found previously that the addition of a simple surgical procedure converted a transient memory defect in mice to a much more durable one26; thus, it is possible that the addition of surgery here could have done the same.24,26 This has also been proposed for studying postoperative neurocognitive disorders in orthopedic patients at risk for PD.31

In summary, repeated exposures to isoflurane in the prodromal phase of PD transiently accelerates the loss of TH expression in a rat model of PD, in the absence of an acute motor effect and with no long-term detrimental effects on motor skills or neuropathology. We conclude that in this animal model, isoflurane alone does not accelerate the onset of PD.

DISCLOSURES

Name: Daniel A. Xu, BS.

Contribution: This author helped perform experiments and collect data, conduct statistical analyses, prepare figures, and write and edit the manuscript.

Name: Timothy P. DeYoung, MSN, RN.

Contribution: This author helped conceive of and design the experiments, perform experiments and collect data, analyze data, and write and edit the manuscript.

Name: Nicholas P. Kondoleon, MD.

Contribution: This author helped perform experiments and collect data, prepare figures, and write and edit the manuscript.

Name: Roderic G. Eckenhoff, MD.

Contribution: This author helped conceive of and design the experiments and edit the manuscript.

Name: Maryellen F. Eckenhoff, PhD.

Contribution: This author helped conceive of and design the experiments, perform experiments and collect data, conduct statistical analyses, prepare figures, and write and edit the manuscript.

This manuscript was handled by: Oluwaseun Johnson-Akeju, MD, MMSc.

REFERENCES

1. Eckenhoff RG, Johansson JS, Wei H, et al. Inhaled anesthetic enhancement of amyloid-beta oligomerization and cytotoxicity. Anesthesiology. 2004;101:703–709.
2. Dong Y, Wu X, Xu Z, Zhang Y, Xie Z. Anesthetic isoflurane increases phosphorylated tau levels mediated by caspase activation and Aβ generation. PLoS One. 2012;7:e39386.
3. Zhang Y, Xu Z, Wang H, et al. Anesthetics isoflurane and desflurane differently affect mitochondrial function, learning, and memory. Ann Neurol. 2012;71:687–698.
4. Lin D, Cao L, Wang Z, Li J, Washington JM, Zuo Z. Lidocaine attenuates cognitive impairment after isoflurane anesthesia in old rats. Behav Brain Res. 2012;228:319–327.
5. Wei H. The role of calcium dysregulation in anesthetic-mediated neurotoxicity. Anesth Analg. 2011;113:972–974.
6. Planel E, Bretteville A, Liu L, et al. Acceleration and persistence of neurofibrillary pathology in a mouse model of tauopathy following anesthesia. FASEB J. 2009;23:2595–2604.
7. Xie Z, Culley DJ, Dong Y, et al. The common inhalation anesthetic isoflurane induces caspase activation and increases amyloid beta-protein level in vivo. Ann Neurol. 2008;64:618–627.
8. Xie Z, Dong Y, Maeda U, et al. The inhalation anesthetic isoflurane induces a vicious cycle of apoptosis and amyloid beta-protein accumulation. J Neurosci. 2007;27:1247–1254.
9. Tang JX, Mardini F, Caltagarone BM, et al. Anesthesia in presymptomatic Alzheimer’s disease: a study using the triple-transgenic mouse model. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:521–e1.531
10. Wang Q, Liang G, Yang H, Wang S, Eckenhoff MF, Wei H. The common inhaled anesthetic isoflurane increases aggregation of Huntingtin and alters calcium homeostasis in a cell model of Huntington’s disease. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2011;250:291–298.
11. Muravchick S, Smith DS. Parkinsonian symptoms during emergence from general anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 1995;82:305–307.
12. Peretz C, Alexander BH, Nagahama SI, Domino KB, Checkoway H. Parkinson’s disease mortality among male anesthesiologists and internists. Mov Disord. 2005;20:1614–1617.
13. Mastrangelo G, Comiati V, dell’Aquila M, Zamprogno E. Exposure to anesthetic gases and Parkinson’s disease: a case report. BMC Neurol. 2013;13:194.
14. Dave KD, De Silva S, Sheth NP, et al. Phenotypic characterization of recessive gene knockout rat models of Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol Dis. 2014;70:190–203.
15. Yang KM, Blue KV, Mulholland HM, Kurup MP, Kelm-Nelson CA, Ciucci MR. Characterization of oromotor and limb motor dysfunction in the DJ1 -/- model of Parkinson disease. Behav Brain Res. 2018;339:47–56.
16. Kyser TL, Dourson AJ, McGuire JL, Hemmerle AM, Williams MT, Seroogy KB. Characterization of motor and non-motor behavioral alterations in the Dj-1 (PARK7) knockout rat. J Mol Neurosci. 2019;69:298–311.
17. National Research Council US. Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. 2011.8th ed. National Academies Press;
18. Kilkenny C, Browne WJ, Cuthill IC, Emerson M, Altman DG. Improving bioscience research reporting: the ARRIVE guidelines for reporting animal research. PLoS Biol. 2010;8:e1000412.
19. Metz GA, Whishaw IQ. The ladder rung walking task: a scoring system and its practical application. J Vis Exp. 2009;28:1204.
20. Shiotsuki H, Yoshimi K, Shimo Y, et al. A rotarod test for evaluation of motor skill learning. J Neurosci Methods. 2010;189:180–185.
21. Antunes M, Biala G. The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, and its modifications. Cogn Process. 2012;13:93–110.
22. Schaefer ML, Perez PJ, Wang M, et al. Neonatal isoflurane anesthesia or disruption of postsynaptic density-95 protein interactions change dendritic spine densities and cognitive function in juvenile mice. Anesthesiology. 2020;133:812–823.
23. Clark RE, Zola SM, Squire LR. Impaired recognition memory in rats after damage to the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2000;20:8853–8860.
24. Mardini F, Tang JX, Li JC, Arroliga MJ, Eckenhoff RG, Eckenhoff MF. Effects of propofol and surgery on neuropathology and cognition in the 3xTgAD Alzheimer transgenic mouse model. Br J Anaesth. 2017;119:472–480.
25. DeYoung TP, Li JC, Tang X, et al. Absence of neuropathology with prolonged isoflurane sedation in healthy adult rats. J Neurosurg Anesthesiol. 2017;29:439–447.
26. Tang JX, Mardini F, Janik LS, et al. Modulation of murine Alzheimer pathogenesis and behavior by surgery. Ann Surg. 2013;257:439–448.
27. Terrando N, Monaco C, Ma D, Foxwell BM, Feldmann M, Maze M. Tumor necrosis factor-alpha triggers a cytokine cascade yielding postoperative cognitive decline. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:20518–20522.
28. Leikas JV, Kohtala S, Theilmann W, Jalkanen AJ, Forsberg MM, Rantamäki T. Brief isoflurane anesthesia regulates striatal AKT-GSK3β signaling and ameliorates motor deficits in a rat model of early-stage Parkinson’s disease. J Neurochem. 2017;142:456–463.
29. Snyder GL, Galdi S, Hendrick JP, Hemmings HC Jr. General anesthetics selectively modulate glutamatergic and dopaminergic signaling via site-specific phosphorylation in vivo. Neuropharmacology. 2007;53:619–630.
30. Brooks SP, Dunnett SB. Tests to assess motor phenotype in mice: a user’s guide. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:519–529.
31. Price CC, Levy SA, Tanner J, et al. Orthopedic surgery and post-operative cognitive decline in idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: considerations from a pilot study. J Parkinsons Dis. 2015;5:893–905.

Supplemental Digital Content

Copyright © 2021 International Anesthesia Research Society