Secondary Logo

Journal Logo

Randomized controlled trial of caregiver training for HIV-infected child neurodevelopment and caregiver well being

Bass, Judith K.a; Opoka, Robertb; Familiar, Itziarc; Nakasujja, Noelined; Sikorskii, Allac; Awadu, Joreme; Givon, Deborahf; Shohet, Cillyf; Murray, Sarah M.a; Augustinavicius, Juraa; Mendelson, Tamara; Boivin, Michaela,g

doi: 10.1097/QAD.0000000000001563
EPIDEMIOLOGY AND SOCIAL: CONCISE COMMUNICATIONS
Free
SDC

Objectives: HIV infection places children at neurodevelopmental risk; for young children in poverty, risk is compounded by compromised caregiving quality. The mediational intervention for sensitizing caregivers (MISC) program trained caregivers on fostering daily interactions with young children. We hypothesized that MISC could enhance neurodevelopment of rural Ugandan HIV-infected children and improve mental health outcomes of their caregivers, which might mediate improved caregiving quality.

Design: A randomized trial of HIV-infected young children (ages 2–5 years) and their female caregivers; cluster randomization was to MISC or a nutrition curriculum.

Setting: A total of 18 geographic clusters in rural Uganda.

Study participants: Children and caregivers were evaluated at baseline, 6 months, 1 year, and 1-year post-training.

Main outcome measures: Mullen Scales of Early Learning, the Color-Object Association Test for memory, the Early Childhood Vigilance Test of attention, and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function for the children. Caregivers completed measures of depression and anxiety symptoms and daily functioning.

Results: MISC had a significant impact on postintervention receptive language (adjusted mean difference = 3.13, 95% confidence interval 0.08, 6.18) that persisted at 1-year follow-up. MISC caregivers reported significantly less functional impairment postprogram (adjusted mean difference = −0.15, 95% confidence interval −0.28, −0.01). Other outcomes were NS.

Conclusion: Both intervention conditions resulted in improvements in the study children over time. MISC showed additional impacts on child language and caregiver well-being. Future directions that include assessing the extent enhanced language development resulting from improved caregiving may better prepare impoverished children for school.

aDepartment of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland, USA

bDepartment of Pediatrics and Child Health, Makerere University, Kampala, Uganda

cDepartment of Psychiatry, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA

dDepartment of Psychiatry, Bar Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel

eSchool of Education, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA

fSchool of Education, Bar Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel

gDepartment of Neurology and Ophthalmology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA.

Correspondence to Judith K. Bass, Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health, Applied Mental Health Research Group, 624 N. Broadway, Room 861, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA. Tel: +1 410 502 9849; e-mail: jbass1@jhu.edu

Received 3 January, 2017

Revised 15 May, 2017

Accepted 23 May, 2017

Supplemental digital content is available for this article. Direct URL citations appear in the printed text and are provided in the HTML and PDF versions of this article on the journal's Website (http://www.AIDSonline.com).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Introduction

Exposure to poverty-related cumulative risk in early childhood can negatively affects cognitive developmental trajectories through limited cognitive stimulation and nutrition [1,2]. HIV-infected children are at a particular disadvantage if their care depends on impoverished HIV-infected caregivers, themselves at risk for impaired functioning.

There is increasing evidence that parent-directed interventions can improve child cognitive development [3–5], including in Uganda [6]. Following earlier Ugandan feasibility and efficacy research [7,8], the present trial of caregiver training benefits for HIV-affected families was initiated in Tororo District, with 24% of the population living under the poverty line [9] and 5.8% HIV prevalence [10]. This cluster randomized controlled trial (RCT) evaluated whether a year-long biweekly caregiver training intervention could improve caregiver mental health, quality of caregiving, and child neurodevelopmental outcomes in HIV-infected children.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Methods

Procedures and participants

After Institutional Review Board approval by Michigan State University and Makerere University School of Medicine, 18 subcounties (unit of randomization) in Tororo and Busia districts were randomly assigned to treatment arms. Staff conducting child assessments (blind to cluster allocation) and the study coordinator (not blind to cluster allocations) enrolled study participants.

Women and child dyads (N = 120) were identified over a 12-month period from AIDS support (TASO) clinics. A female caregiver provided written consent for her and her child. Child eligibility was based on confirmed perinatally acquired HIV infection, being between 2 and 5 years of age, and no history of neurological insult, with a female caregiver able to participate.

Participants in both study arms received a biweekly nutritional supplement. The interventions were provided in 1-hour sessions with each caregiver alternating biweekly between home and the project office at Tororo District Hospital. All intervention providers were Ugandan Makerere University Psychology or Social-Work graduates who received a 2-week training in their respective intervention and participated in weekly supervision and a week-long refresher training.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Caregiver training interventions

Mediational Intervention for Sensitizing Caregivers (MISC). MISC is a model for training caregivers to enhance their children's development [11] based on Feuerstein's [12,13] theory of cognitive modifiability.

Uganda Community Based Association for Child Welfare Program (UCOBAC). The comparison condition was a manualized nutrition and hygiene information program designed for impoverished households by UCOBAC (http://ucobac.org/).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Measures

Study data were collected at baseline, at 6 months (midway through training), at 1 year (completion of training), and at a 12-month follow-up (24 months after baseline). Measures of caregiver mental health and all child outcomes were previously used in Uganda [7,8].

Demographics: Child demographics included age, sex, and current use of HAART (yes/no). Caregiver demographics included marital status (married/unmarried), education (any/none), and relationship to study child (mother/other).

Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL)[14] assesses visual reception, gross motor skills, fine motor skills, receptive, and expressive language. A composite score provides a measure of for example, the general measure of fluid intelligence thought to underlie general cognitive ability.

Color Object Association Test[15] (COAT) evaluates object placement memory with principal outcomes of immediate memory (assessed by number of recalled items) and overall total recall (assessed by number of correctly placed items).

Early Childhood Vigilance Test[16,17] assesses sustained attention, with the principal outcome of the proportion of time looking at an animation video as scored from a computer-mounted webcam video.

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool version (BRIEF) [18] evaluates behavior, attention and cognitive problems related to disruption of executive functions as reported in a series of questions to the principal caregiver; a combined Global Executive Composite (GEC) score is generated [18].

Caldwell Home Observation for the Measurement of the Environment (HOME) [19] assesses quality of child–caregiver interactions in the home using 45 yes/no items. More ‘yes’ answers indicate higher quality interactions.

Observing Mediational Interactions (OMIs). Intervention trainers collected 5-min videos every 6 months of caregivers bathing, feeding, and working with their child. Videos were scored by an independent observer using a standard rubric [11,20] to count specific occurrences of focusing, exciting, expanding, encouraging, and regulating interactions. Total number of interactions was used as a mediation indicator of caregiving quality.

Hopkins Symptoms Checklist-25[21,22] contains subscales for anxiety (10 symptoms) and depression (15 symptoms). Caregivers indicated frequency of each symptom in the last 2 weeks on a scale of 0 (not at all) to 3 (a lot). Subscale scores were calculated using mean item responses.

Caregiver functioning indicated how much difficulty caregivers had completing 12 tasks of daily living identified during a brief qualitative study, with responses from 0 (no difficulty at all) to 4 (cannot complete it). An impairment scale was calculated using mean item responses.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Analysis

Sample size was calculated based on the magnitude of effects seen in a prior study [7]; with 54 (MISC) and 58 (UCOBAC) children, an unadjusted effect size of 0.53 was detectable with 0.80 power in two-sided tests at P = 0.05. Baseline intervention arm comparisons were calculated using t, chi-square, or Fisher's exact tests. Linear mixed effects (LME) models were employed. Correlations arising from repeated measures were accounted for by specifying an autoregressive covariance structure. Inclusion of a random effect for clusters (unit of randomization) was explored, but the resulting intraclass correlation coefficients were virtually zero across outcomes.

Each outcome was analyzed separately using LME with common covariates. Time was entered as a categorical variable with levels corresponding to 6, 12, and 24 months. Time-by-intervention interactions were included to capture potential changes in differences by intervention arm over time. The least squares (adjusted) means for each time point and trial arm were output from the LME models, and differences between them by trial arm were tested to assess immediate and sustained intervention effects.

Variation in caregiver quality assessed, via the HOME and OMI, was explored post hoc as a potential mediator of intervention effect on child outcomes. Caregiver mental health and functioning at baseline were explored as potential moderators of intervention effects on child outcomes. For analysis of MSEL subscale scores, Benjamini and Hochberg [23] procedure for the control of false discovery rate was applied. SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina, USA) was used for all analyses.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Results

Of the 118 child–caregiver dyads who began the interventions, 112 children (95%) and 109 caregivers (92%) completed the mid-program assessment (6 months after baseline); 107 children and caregivers (91%) completed the postprogram assessment (12 months after baseline); and 106 children (90%) and 100 caregivers (85%) completed the follow-up assessment (24 months after baseline) (Supplement 1, http://links.lww.com/QAD/B116).

MISC and UCOBAC child–caregiver dyads are demographically similar and comparable in outcome scale scores at baseline (Table 1); only for the BRIEF scale of inhibitory self-control did MISC children score worse than UCOBAC children. At baseline, caregivers reported moderate mental health problems and low functional impairment.

Table 1

Table 1

Back to Top | Article Outline

Child outcomes

Children in both interventions experienced positive neurological and cognitive development changes (Table 2). MSEL scores are presented age standardized, so a decrease is interpreted as study children on average not making development gains on a similar trajectory as children from high-income countries, on which standardized scores are based. Of the five MSEL subscales, MISC had a significant impact on receptive language score postintervention [adjusted mean difference = 3.13, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.08, 6.18]; this effect did not remain significant after Benjamini–Hochberg adjustment and was attenuated at the maintenance follow-up (adjusted mean difference = 2.56, 95% CI −0.50, 5.63). At postprogram assessment, MISC children had significantly worse (higher) BRIEF metacognition and inhibitory self-control subscales and global executive function scores than UCOBAC. None of these differences were maintained at the maintenance follow-up. There were no other statistically significant effects of MISC on child outcomes.

Table 2

Table 2

Table 2

Table 2

Back to Top | Article Outline

Caregiver outcomes

Caregivers in both intervention arms experienced improvements in mental health and functionality over time (Table 2). MISC caregivers reported near significant fewer depression symptoms at the maintenance assessment (adjusted mean difference = −0.17, 95% CI −0.34, −0.02), as well as significantly less functional impairment postprogram (adjusted mean difference = −0.15, 95% CI −0.28, −0.01).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Mediation and moderation

For the exploratory mediation analysis, MISC participants showed greater increases over time in average HOME scores (mid-program: 22.45; postprogram: 23.60; 12-month follow-up: 24.00) than UCOBAC participants (mid-program: 20.36; postprogram: 20.38; 12-month follow-up: 19.89), with significant differences by arm at all time points (P < 0.001). HOME and MSEL receptive language scores were significantly associated across the full sample, irrespective of intervention arm. Controlling for HOME score in the LME with the MSEL receptive language outcome resulted in the trial arm variable losing significance, indicating probable mediation. Similar results were obtained with the OMI score as a potential mediator.

In the exploratory moderation analysis, no significant interactions were found between baseline caregiver mental health and trial arm for any of the MSEL outcomes. For caregiver functionality, better baseline caregiver functionality was associated with smaller gains in receptive language.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Discussion

Participation in both intervention conditions resulted in improved outcomes for children and caregivers. MISC led to additional small improvements in child receptive language acquisition and caregiver mental health. The lack of effects for other developmental domains may be a result of UCOBAC being an active control condition, which enhanced the caregiver's attention to their child's nutrition and health.

MISC caregivers showing greater improvement over time in mental health compared with controls is in line with prior findings [7]. With the MISC program encouraging caregivers to value their own ideas about childrearing, a possible mechanism by which MISC may impact caregiver mental health is through an increase in parenting-related self-efficacy and empowerment.

Baseline MSEL scores were indicative of an at-risk sample. Compared with similarly aged populations in the USA, our sample had significantly lower MSEL scores [24,25], but similar to other HIV-infected samples in Uganda [7]. Although children in our study showed gains in cognitive development over time, their measured rate of improvement is less than that seen in the USA, which is why the standardized scores appear to decline over time.

In exploratory analyses, quality and quantity of caregiver–child interactions were identified as potential mediators of MISC's impact on receptive language; having confirmation of the hypothesized mediation model strengthens the study findings. We also found that MISC appears to be more effective in improving language outcomes of children whose caregivers reported better functionality in tasks of daily living at the beginning of program participation.

Several limitations should be noted. The MSEL and COAT have been previously used in Uganda [7,26]; however, standardized local norms were not available. We used scores standardized based on non-lower and middle income countries samples rather than raw scores to account for developmental growth. We were not able to formally test for spillover effects of the MISC intervention into the control condition, but through fidelity monitoring, we think this was minimal. There is a chance through multiple testing for identifying significant differences by chance.

With more than 200 million children not reaching their developmental potential due to poverty, illness, and lack of social and educational resources, improved early caregiving is important [27]. Both interventions resulted in improvements for HIV-infected children, providing additional support for the importance of early childhood programming.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Acknowledgements

The research was supported by NIH grant RO1 HD070723 (PIs: M.B. and J.K.B.). S.M.M. was supported by NIMH Global Mental Health training grant (MH103210). The study sponsor had no role in the study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the article for publication. J.K.B. M.B., I.F., S.M.M., and A.L. have full access to all of the data and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

The University of Michigan Medical School Global Reach program and the Michigan State University College of Human Medicine and College of Osteopathic Medicine provided summer stipends to support summer research internships for a number of students during the study period, and the support of these programs and participation of these medical students are greatly appreciated. Dr Elizabeth Schut helped provide field-based and clinic-based medical support to our study children and households on a volunteer basis during medical school clinical training, and her efforts are greatly appreciated.

This effort is dedicated to Professor Pnina Klein (1945–2014), who dedicated her professional life to the development and promotion of the Mediational Intervention for Sensitizing Caregivers (MISC), and without whose efforts, this study would never have been possible.

J.K.B., as study PI, shared oversight over all aspects and phases of study design and implementation and wrote the first draft of the article. R.O.O., as Ugandan co-I, was responsible for management and oversight of Tororo-based research staff and intervention providers, contributed to editing the article. I.F., as the on-site scientific director of study and contributed to writing the article. N.N., as Ugandan co-PI, was responsible for psychiatric care and referral, Ugandan IRB submissions, and contributing to the editing of the article. A.S. was responsible for all statistical analyses and data presentation and drafted the data analysis plan. J.A. led the on-site assessment team and outcomes scoring and contributed to the editing of the article. D.G. was responsible for adaptation of MISC to study context and training and certification of MISC research assistants, support of MISC training team, and contributed to the editing of the intervention description in the article. C.S. was responsible for adaptation of MISC to the study context, training and certification of MISC research assistants, support of MISC training team, and contributed to the editing of the intervention description in the article. S.M.M. was responsible for finalization of all study protocols, caregiver assessment validations, and participated in drafting and editing of the article. J.A. assisted in on-site study protocol implementation, caregiver assessments, and contributed to the editing of the article. T.M. assisted in the evaluation of caregivers and contributed to the development of the article. M.J.B., as study PI, shared oversight over all phases of study design and implementation and participated in drafting and editing the article. All authors approved the article as submitted.

Trial registration: clinicaltrials.gov identifier: NCT01640561

Back to Top | Article Outline

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

Back to Top | Article Outline

References

1. Britto PR, Engle PL. Handbook of early childhood development research and its impact on global policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2013.
2. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Grantham-McGregor S, Black MM, Nelson CA, Huffman SL, et al. Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development. Lancet 2011; 378:1325–1338.
3. Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR. Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Dev Psychol 2006; 42:627–642.
4. Olds DL, Robinson J, Pettitt L, Luckey DW, Holmberg J, Ng RK, et al. Effects of home visits by paraprofessionals and by nurses: age 4 follow-up results of a randomized trial. Pediatrics 2004; 114:1560–1568.
5. Engle PL, Black MM, Behrman JR, Cabral de Mello M, Gertler PJ, Kapiriri L, et al. Strategies to avoid the loss of developmental potential in more than 200 million children in the developing world. Lancet 2007; 369:229–242.
6. Singla DR, Kumbakumba E, Aboud FE. Effects of a parenting intervention to address maternal psychological wellbeing and child development and growth in rural Uganda: a community-based, cluster randomised trial. Lancet Glob Health 2015; 3:e458–e469.
7. Boivin MJ, Bangirana P, Nakasujja N, Page CF, Shohet C, Givon D, et al. A year-long caregiver training program improves cognition in preschool Ugandan children with human immunodeficiency virus. J Pediatr 2013; 163:1409–1416.e1–e5.
8. Boivin MJ, Bangirana P, Nakasujja N, Page CF, Shohet C, Givon D, et al. A year-long caregiver training program to improve neurocognition in preschool Ugandan HIV-exposed children. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2013; 34:269–278.
9. Uganda Bureau of Statistics and ICF International Inc. Uganda Demographic and Health Survey 2011. Kampala, Uganda and Calverton, Maryland: UBOS and ICF International Inc; 2012.
10. Uganda AIDS Commission. HIV and AIDS Uganda country progress report. 2014; Kampala, Uganda: Uganda AIDS Commission, http://http://www.unaids.org/sites/default/files/country/documents/UGA_narrative_report_2014.pdfhttp://www.unaids.org/sites/default/files/country/documents/UGA_narrative_report_2014.pdf. [Accessed 3 August 2015].
11. Klein PS, Hundeide K. Early intervention: cross-cultural experiences with a Mediational approach. New York, NY: Garland Publishing Inc.; 1996.
12. Feuerstein R. The dynamic assessment of retarded performers. New York: University Park Press; 1979.
13. Feuerstein R. Instrumental enrichment: redevelopment of cognitive functions of retarded performers. New York, NY: University Park Press; 1980.
14. Mullen EM. Mullen Scales of Early Learning (AGS ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1995.
15. Jordan CM, Johnson AL, Hughes SJ, Shapiro EG. The Color Object Association Test (COAT): the development of a new measure of declarative memory for 18- to 36-month-old toddlers. Child Neuropsychol 2008; 14:21–41.
16. Zelinsky D, Hughes S, Rumsey R, Jordan C, Shaprio E. The Early Childhood Vigilance Task: a new technique for the measurement of sustained attention in very young children. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1996; 2:23.
17. Goldman DZ, Shapiro EG, Nelson CA. Measurement of vigilance in 2-year-old children. Dev Neuropsychol 2004; 25:227–250.
18. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychol 2000; 6:235–238.
19. Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas Press; 1979.
20. Klein P, Rye H. Interaction-oriented early intervention in Ethiopia – the MISC approach. Infants Young Child 2004; 17:340–354.
21. Hesbacher PT, Rickels K, Morris RJ, Newman H, Rosenfeld H. Psychiatric illness in family practice. J Clin Psychiatry 1980; 41:6–10.
22. Winokur A, Winokur DF, Rickels K, Cox DS. Symptoms of emotional distress in a family planning service: stability over a four-week period. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci 1984; 144:395–399.
23. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B Methodol 1995; 57:289–300.
24. Akshoomoff N. Use of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning for the assessment of young children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Child Neuropsychol 2006; 12:269–277.
25. Swineford LB, Guthrie W, Thurm A. Convergent and divergent validity of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning in young children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Psychol Assess 2015; 27:1364–1378.
26. Busman RA, Page CF, Oka E, Giordani B, Boivin MJ. Boivin MJ, Giordani B. Factors contributing to the psychosocial adjustment of Ugandan preschool children with HIV/AIDS. Springer, Neuropsychology of children in Africa: perspectives on risk and resilience. New York: 2013.
27. Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Richter L, Strupp B. Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet 2007; 369:60–70.
Keywords:

caregiver intervention; child development; HIV-infected children; lower and middle income countries; psychosocial

Supplemental Digital Content

Back to Top | Article Outline
Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc.