It is believed that the completion of a preexercise (or presport) physical preparation routine is required to augment performance and reduce injury risk (1–3). One component of this routine that has received much scrutiny is the inclusion of static (particularly passive) muscle stretching (3–8). From an injury minimization perspective, studies have typically not confirmed a clear effect of preexercise static stretching on all-cause injury risk in sports (9,10), which has resulted in some researchers suggesting a limited role for the practice (6,7,10) or for the inclusion of dynamic forms of stretching (2). However, other authors conclude that static stretching might specifically provide a small-to-moderate protective effect for muscle–tendon injury risk, especially in running-based sports (e.g., the various football codes and court sports) (3,4,8,9), which attract by far the highest participation (11) and injury (12) rates. By contrast, no detailed studies have examined the effects of dynamic stretching on injury risk. Therefore, current scientific evidence favors static over dynamic stretching from an injury prevention perspective, although the overall benefit may be small to moderate and limited to a subset of sports.
Nonetheless, several recent reviews have also concluded that static stretching can significantly and negatively affect high-intensity physical performance (4,5,13). Several researchers and advocacy groups, including the European College of Sports Sciences (14) and the American College of Sports Medicine (15), do not recommend the inclusion of static stretching in preexercise routines or call for its replacement by dynamic forms of muscle stretching (2). Indeed, in some cases, the continued use of static stretching by sports participants has been explicitly admonished (16). Nonetheless, the majority of studies examining the effects of preexercise muscle stretching have not been designed to assess its effects on sports performance (e.g., see Supplement G in ref. 4). Common threats to external validity in previous studies include (a) total stretching durations being longer than those typically performed by athletes (17,18); (b) the stretching rarely being followed by other important components of a sport-specific warm-up, including high-intensity and movement pattern-specific exercises (1,19), although it may mitigate the negative effects of stretching (20); (c) participants being only minimally familiarized with the tests (athletes, on the other hand, are familiar with their sporting skills); (d) differences existing in the execution (movement pattern) of static versus dynamic stretches; and (e) the imposition of nonstretching rest periods in control conditions/groups, which would not be performed in sports (4). Also, studies have been susceptible to serious threats to internal validity, such as the expectancy effects of knowledgeable participants (21) and lack of experimenter blinding (22). Notwithstanding these threats to validity, the effects of static stretching on dynamic movement performance (e.g., jumping, running, sprint cycling) have been found to be small on average when stretches are performed for <60 s per muscle (weighted average = −1.1%), and the performance benefits of dynamic stretching performance is also surprisingly small (+1.3%) (4). The call for the removal of static stretching and possible replacement with dynamic stretching (16), despite the limited evidence of impact on sports performance, creates a dilemma for medical practitioners, physiotherapists, and physical trainers who may be asked to provide their opinions on proper sports participation practices.
Given the above, the decision to advocate against the static stretching, particularly on the grounds that it might reduce exercise performance, is questionable, especially given that sports participants show a preference to stretch their muscles despite this advocacy (23) and there being a potential small-to-moderate musculotendinous injury risk minimization benefit. In the present study, we have attempted to overcome some of the limitations of previous studies to specifically answer the question of whether the inclusion of short- or moderate-duration static or dynamic muscle stretching completed as part of a comprehensive preexercise routine (i.e., warm-up) influences performances in common, high-intensity sporting tasks. On the basis of the available evidence, we hypothesized that the imposition of short or moderate durations of static or dynamic stretching would not meaningfully affect high-intensity physical performance when performed as part of a comprehensive preexercise routine.
METHODS
Twenty healthy males (age = 21.1 ± 3.1 yr; body mass = 73.4 ± 6.8 kg; height = 1.79 ± 0.70 m) volunteered for the study. Participants were recruited if they were 18–25 yr of age, without recent injury or illness that would preclude exercise performance, and competing in running-based sports or performing at least three running-based exercise sessions per week. The study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of Edith Cowan University (STREAM11450/11541) and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants read and signed an informed consent document.
Study Design
This study used a randomized, crossover (repeated-measures) design with control condition and was designed to assess the effect of dynamic versus both shorter- (5 s) and longer-duration (30 s) static muscle stretching interventions on performances in tests that mimic common sporting tasks. There were three experimental (stretching) conditions and a nonstretching control condition (hereafter referred to as “pretesting routines”) performed at the same time of day over four testing sessions separated by a minimum of 72 h and each followed by a comprehensive test battery (see Fig. 1). The order of conditions and order of tests within each condition were randomized between the participants without replication by the participants choosing a numbered card randomly from a pack that related to a test and stretch condition order. The card was not replaced to ensure that some test and stretch condition orders could not be allocated more often than others.
FIGURE 1: Study design. After completing a low-intensity warm-up including 3-min jog and running drills, a randomly assigned stretching (no NS control) condition was completed. This was followed by a high-intensity warm-up comprising further jogging and running drills and then three circuits at increasing intensity (to maximum) composed of the performance tests. After a 7-min rest, during which time the participants rated their confidence that the warm-up would improve their performance (see text for details), a sit-and-reach flexibility test was completed before the high-intensity performance tests were completed in a random order (order repeated at each session). 5-rep, 5-repetition.
A pretesting routine was completed before the test battery was administered. The pretesting routine, including any muscle stretching, was monitored by a research coordinator who ensured that procedures (described below) were followed correctly but who could not communicate with researchers overseeing the test battery (hereafter referred to as “testers”). After completion of the pretesting routine, the coordinators relinquished participant responsibility to the testers, who were given no information as to the pretesting stretch condition administered and were naïve to the time required to complete the pretesting routine; this prevented the possibility of guessing the pretesting routine type because each required a different time to complete. Thus, the testers were blinded to the pretesting routine condition.
Familiarization of Muscle Stretching and Performance Tests
At least one familiarization session was completed by each participant before data collection to become accustomed to the stretching protocols, to learn the correct testing procedures, and to acquaint themselves with the equipment, laboratory facility, and verbal instructions issued by the coordinators and testers for the stretching exercises and tests. A video demonstration of each stretch was provided to the participants to ensure similarity in instruction of the stretches, then each participant received individual feedback to correct errors. The participants were then shown how to complete each test and given multiple untimed trials to become familiar. The movement patterns of the tests (described below) were similar to the movement patterns used by the participants in their sports. An additional familiarization session was provided to four participants who declared a lack of confidence in the performance of one or more testing protocols.
Prestudy Participant Outcome Expectations
At the end of the familiarization session, each participant completed an outcome expectation survey to determine which preexercise routine they believed would prove most beneficial to performance. The participants were asked to “List in descending order the stretch condition you believe will stimulate the best improvement in your performance (dynamic, 5 s static, 30 s static, and no stretch)” when compared with the other conditions. They therefore nominated in order from 1 (best) to 4 (worst) which routine they believed would improve (or reduce) performance the most. Post hoc, these expectations were compared with the outcomes of the testing to determine whether expectation was aligned with outcome.
Testing Session Design
Participants were required to wear the same sports shoes and athletic clothing at each session, to refrain from intensive exercise in the 24-h period before testing, and to abstain from caffeine or any form of stimulant/depressant 24 h before testing. As the participants were team sport athletes, other physical training completed by the participants outside of the study was monitored (for type, volume, and intensity) by the participants providing a log book record of their activities in the 48 h before testing as well as a rating of their muscle soreness from 1 to 10 to ensure that significant (>2 units) changes in their performance of, or recovery from, their programs did not occur. If the standard training programs of the participants were not adhered to, the testing session was to be cancelled and completed at least 72 h later; however, no instances of this occurred.
Each session commenced with a short prestretching warm-up consisting of a 3-min jog at 50% of perceived maximum exertion, then 5-s high knees (to ~90° hip angle) and 5-s heel-to-butt (i.e., knee flexion) drills at 50% of maximum perceived exertion. Heart rate was obtained immediately after the warm-up phase by manual palpation of the carotid artery for post hoc examination of the repeatability of efforts; that is, repeatability of the physical intensities used (heart rate itself could not be used as a target for intensity because of its slow temporal response after exercise commencement).
Participants then completed one of three experimental (stretching) conditions or progressed immediately to the test-specific (i.e., “sport-specific”) warm-up (described below); note that a rest condition of equal duration to the experimental conditions was not included in the no-stretch (control) session as this is not typical sports practice. The four conditions were 5-s static stretch (5S), 30-s static stretch (30S; 3 × 10-s stretches), 5-repetition (per muscle group) dynamic stretch (DYN), and no-stretch (NS) condition (see Text, Supplemental Digital Content 1, Detailing the instructions [with photo] for each stretch, https://links.lww.com/MSS/B187). The 5S, 30S, and DYN stretching protocols each consisted of nine stretches that were close replicates (in body position) of each other to minimize the effect of stretching movement pattern on test outcomes. The static stretches were held at the point of “discomfort,” and maximal ROM was achieved in the dynamic stretches by ensuring a secondary pulling motion with each repetition. The order of preexercise routines was randomized without replication between participants to minimize order effects.
After the stretches (or after progressing immediately from the low-intensity warm-up in NS), a test-specific (i.e., “sport-specific”), higher-intensity warm-up was completed. This started with a 2-min moderate-intensity jog at 60% of perceived effort, and 5-s high knees and 5-s heel-to-butt kick drills at 60% of perceived maximum effort. The participants then performed three circuits of the six performance tests, which were organized into three activity groups: 1) running vertical jump; 2) squat jump (SJ), countermovement jump (CMJ), and drop jump (DJ); 3) T agility test; and 4) 20-m sprint run, and the participants completed them in an order identical to that of the following testing session (see below). The intensity of each circuit increased from 60% to 80% and then 100% of perceived maximal exertion with a 30-s walk recovery between each activity set. This second part of the pretesting routine took approximately 15 min to complete.
To address the study design limitation relating to the time between completion of the final stretch and the commencement of testing (4), a 7-min passive rest period was imposed between the completion of the pretesting routine and the start of testing. This was done to more closely simulate game- or match-day situations where a short precompetition briefing or an individual-specific sport preparation period is completed before match or competition commencement and allowed a better determination of the likely effect of the different preexercise routines on game- or match-day performance.
Participants were permitted to consume plain water ad libitum throughout the testing sessions, and all sessions were conducted in the biomechanics laboratory at Edith Cowan University under similar environmental conditions. The test battery was completed in a circuit at specified testing stations: 1) sit-and-reach flexibility test; 2) running vertical jump test; 3) SJ, CMJ, and DJ (from a 40-cm height) tests; 4) T agility test; and 5) 20-m sprint running test. The order of tests was randomized between participants without replication and then repeated at each session; however, the sit-and-reach test was always completed first to determine the effect of the pretesting routine on flexibility (maximum range of motion) without the potential influence of other tests. The performance of the sit-and-reach test was not expected to influence performances in subsequent tests because of the short duration of the stretch procedure. For the testing, 4 min was allocated to each test station so that constant test timing was achieved regardless of the order of tests. An audio signal prompted the commencement of each test.
Post–Warm-up Participant Outcome Expectations
To address issues around expectancy bias (21), during the 7-min rest period before testing in each session, the participants also provided a rating score ranging from 1 to 10 for “how effective you believe the warm-up will be on your performance,” where 1 = no effect/possibly harmful to performance, 2 = very small improvement to performance, 5 = noticeable improvement in performance, and 10 = performance will improve dramatically. Obtaining this information immediately after completion of each pretesting routine was expected to yield different results to the outcome expectation survey completed in the study familiarization session and, thus, to allow a better analysis of whether participant expectancy might influence study results. Equal ratings between conditions were allowed.
Testing Procedures
Sit-and-reach flexibility
The sit-and-reach test was conducted using the Flex-Tester apparatus (Novel Products Inc., Rockton, IL). A double-leg protocol was used as prescribed by the Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology (24). Each participant was instructed to sit bare-footed with knees in maximal extension and with both feet together and flat against the device. The participant then exhaled and stretched forward with palms overlapping and fingertips aligned, holding the furthest end point for 2 s. The score was recorded to the nearest 0.1 cm and repeated after a 30-s rest, with the greatest touch distance used for analysis.
Three-meter running vertical jump
A jump-and-reach system (Vertec; Swift Performance Equipment, Wacol, Australia) was used for the running vertical jump to directly measure jump height based on the difference between the reach height and the jump height obtained. Reach height was obtained before each test with the participant standing in a static position underneath the Vertec device and reaching as high as possible with the arm touching their ear but with shoulders remaining parallel to the floor. The fingers displaced vanes (each 1 cm apart) within touching distance, and the maximum reach height was obtained. For jump testing, each participant’s takeoff foot was predetermined during the familiarization session, and a self-selected starting position was assumed 3 m from the device, which was kept consistent across all testing sessions. At their own volition, the participant executed a running, single-leg jump to displace the vanes with the opposite hand. The maximum jump-and-reach height was recorded as the number below the score reflected on the Vertec device, and the true jump height was then calculated as the difference between the maximum jump-and-reach height and the standing reach height. Each participant was given a maximum of five attempts; however, the test was stopped when the participant failed to further improve jump scores on two successive attempts. A 30-s passive rest was imposed between each jump, and the best (i.e., final) true jump height score was used for analysis.
SJ, CMJ, and DJ
A piezoelectric force platform (987B, Kistler Instrumente, Winterthur, Switzerland) was used to measure vertical jump height using the flight time method (height = ½ g (t/2)2, where g = 9.81 m·s−2 and t = time in air). The analog signal from the force platform was converted to a digital signal using Bioware software (Kistler Instrumente) sampling at 1000 Hz. Flight time was identified as the period between takeoff and contact after flight, and this was obtained in each jump via analysis of the force–time curve. A 15-s passive recovery was imposed between each jump, which allowed the tester to record vertical jump height and to reset the systems for recording of the next trial. Two attempts were allowed for each jump type; however, a third trial was completed if jump heights varied >5%. The best score was used for analysis.
SJ trials were performed from a squatted position with heels in contact with the platform and with a self-selected knee angle (~75°). Each participant’s hands were kept on their hips throughout the jump, and a countermovement was not allowed. The participant was instructed to hold the squat position for at least 2 s before jumping. Visual observation of both jumping technique and the force–time trace was made to ensure that there was no countermovement in the jump. Trials were repeated if a countermovement could be visually observed by the tester. CMJ trials were performed from a vertical standing position with hands on hips and knees about shoulder width apart. The participants then executed a two-footed vertical jump immediately after an eccentric countermovement to a self-selected depth (although the thighs could not be lower than parallel to the floor [19]). In the DJ, the participant stepped horizontally off a 40-cm box onto the force platform and then immediately jumped vertically. The instruction was given to “jump with minimal ground contact time upon landing” and then to jump as high as possible. The starting position on the top of the box was identical with the CMJ start position.
T agility test
For the T agility (change of direction) test, participants started at their own volition from a standing start 0.4 m behind a start line, sprinted forward to touch the base of a cone located 10 m in front of them, shuffled 5 m to the left to touch a cone, shuffled 10 m to the right to touch a cone, shuffled 5 m left to touch the center cone once again, and then ran backward past the start line. A dual-beam photocell timing gate (Swift Performance) positioned at the start line was triggered when the participant broke the light beam after the start and was stopped when the participant completed the course. Each athlete faced forward at all times and could not cross their feet while shuffling. The participants were instructed to use a standing sprint start and were not allowed to build momentum by rocking back and forth at the start line. They performed the test twice with a 30-s passive rest between, and the fastest time was used for analysis.
Twenty-meter sprint run
The 20-m sprint test was performed on an indoor synthetic 60-m sprint track. The participants used the same starting position as for the T agility test and ran with maximum speed to a cone placed 1.5 m past a 20-m mark. This cone was included to prevent the participants from decelerating before crossing the 20-m mark. The tester counted down and then instructed the participants to sprint at their own volition, and timing gates were placed at 0 and 20 m measured running time. Two attempts were given with a 30-s walk-back recovery between attempts, and the fastest time was used for analysis.
Statistical Analysis
Using IBM SPSS statistical software (version 22; IBM, New York, NY), repeated-measures MANOVA was performed to compare test performances between conditions (5S, 30S, DYN, and NS), whereas a repeated-measures ANOVA was used to compare the performances between conditions specifically for sit-and-reach scores. The alpha level was set at 0.05, and significant main or interaction effects were examined in further detail using ANOVA and univariate tests, as appropriate. In addition, magnitude-based inference tests were performed, and the precision of estimation was calculated. Qualitative descriptors of standardized effects used the following criteria: trivial <0.2, small 0.2–0.6, moderate 0.6–1.2, and large >1.2. Effects where the 95% confidence limits substantially overlapped the thresholds for small positive and negative effects (i.e., exceeding 0.2 of the SD on both sides of zero) were defined as unclear. Clear small or larger effect sizes (i.e., those with >75% likelihood of being >0.20), as calculated using the spread sheet developed by Hopkins (25), were defined as definitive. Precision of estimates was indicated with 95% confidence limits, which defined the range representing the uncertainty in the true value of the (unknown) population mean (26). To better assess the similarity (or lack) of performances between trials, both Pearson’s correlation (r) and intraclass correlation (ICC) were calculated; no corrections were required for outliers or nonuniformity of scatter. ICC values <0.5, 0.5–0.75, 0.75–0.9, and >0.90 were considered indicative of poor, moderate, good, and excellent reliability, respectively. The 90% confidence interval (CI) values were also computed for ICC values, but this is not possible for r values calculated from multiple repeated measurements. Finally, the Bland–Altman method for calculating correlation coefficients for repeated measurements (within subjects) was used to determine whether higher participant expectation scores were correlated with better performances (27).
RESULTS
Participant bias
When assessed during the familiarization session (i.e., before the commencement of the data collection period), 18 of the 20 participants nominated DYN as the most likely beneficial pretesting routine (i.e., they ranked it first out of the four conditions), whereas two participants nominated 30S as the most likely beneficial. In addition, 15 of the 20 participants nominated NS to be least likely beneficial (i.e., ranked it fourth out of the four conditions), whereas five participants nominated 30S. The commonest ranking order among the participants was DYN > 5S > 30S > NS. Thus, there was a clear a priori bias within the participant group.
When asked upon completion of each pretesting routine to rate (on a scale of 1–10) how effective they believed the routine would be for their performance, NS was rated consistently worst (4.0 ± 2.2), and 5S (5.7 ± 1.9) and DYN (6.4 ± 1.6) were rated statistically higher (P < 0.05) than NS; a tendency toward a greater rating for 30S (5.3 ± 2.3) did not reach statistical significance. No statistical differences were observed between the three stretching conditions, and using magnitude-based inference, it was found that all three stretch conditions were rated definitively (>75%) higher by participants than the NS condition, with 97%, 87%, and 100% likelihoods of 5S, 30S, and DYN, respectively, being perceived of greater benefit than NS. Nonetheless, correlation coefficients computed for repeated measurements (within subjects) were small, ranging from −0.16 to 0.21 and with explained variance (R2) ranging from 0.1% to 4.5%, indicating a lack of relationship between ratings of perceived benefit and performance outcomes.
Jumping, running, change of direction, and flexibility
No statistical differences were detected between conditions for the 3-m running vertical jump, SJ, CMJ, or DJ tests (P = 0.471 for condition–time interaction; see Fig. 2), indicating a lack of effect of pretesting routine on performance, and no statistical difference was detected between sessions 1 and 4, indicating a lack of order effect (i.e., effect of session number irrespective of condition). All three stretch conditions were definitively (>75% likelihood) found to elicit trivial effects on running vertical jump (95%, 92%, and 86% likelihood of trivial effect for 5S, 30S, and DYN, respectively) and CMJ (97%, 89%, and 95% likelihood of trivial effect) performances when compared with NS. The effects on SJ (44%, 65%, and 74% likelihood of trivial effect) and DJ scores (72%, 38%, and 50% likelihood of trivial effect) were less clear in SJ (56%, 32%, and 22% likelihood of higher jump in 5S, 30S, and DYN, respectively) and DJ (7%, 62%, and 50% likelihood of lower jump).
FIGURE 2: SJ (A), CMJ (B), DJ (C), and 3-step running jump (3-step Jump; D) heights recorded in 5S, 30S, DYN, and NS (control) conditions. There were no differences in jump test performances between the conditions. Shown are the mean ± SE (black column with error bar) and 95% CI of the mean (separate gray bar) jump performances.
No statistical differences were detected between conditions for the 20-m sprint run (P = 0.354 for condition–time interaction) or T agility test (P = 0.996; see Fig. 3), indicating a lack of effect of pretesting routine on performances. Furthermore, no differences were detected between sessions 1 and 4, indicating a lack of order effect. All three stretch conditions were found to definitively (>75%) elicit trivial effects on 20-m sprint run time (88%, 86%, and 91% likelihoods of trivial effect for 5S, 30S, and DYN, respectively) and T agility time (84%, 93%, and 75% likelihood of trivial effect) when compared with NS.
FIGURE 3: The 20-m sprint run (bottom panel) and T agility (top panel) times recorded in 5S, 30S, DYN, and NS (control) conditions. There were no differences in test performances between the conditions. Shown are the mean ± SE (black column with error bar) and 95% CI of the mean (separate gray bar) jump performances.
No statistical differences were detected for sit-and-reach scores (P = 0.076 for condition–time interaction) between 5S (27.1 ± 8.9 cm), 30S (27.8 ± 8.8 cm), DYN (28.4 ± 8.36 cm), and NS (28.9 ± 9.2 cm). A definitively trivial effect of condition was observed for DYN (98% likelihood of trivial effect) when compared with NS, but 45% and 31% likelihoods of trivial effects for 5S and 30S, with 55% and 68% likelihoods of lower sit-and-reach scores, were observed in these conditions when compared with NS.
Reliability analysis
Both Pearson’s correlation (r) and ICC (±90% CI) analyses completed on the test data revealed a high between-session repeatability of performances for SJ (r = 0.87; ICC = 0.84 [0.73–0.92]), CMJ (r = 0.90; ICC = 0.92 [0.83–0.95]), DJ (r = 0.88; ICC = 0.87 [0.78–0.93]), 3-step jump (r = 0.92; ICC = 0.92 [0.85–0.96]), and 20-m sprint running (r = 0.93; ICC = 0.92 [0.87–0.96]) tests despite the different stretching interventions being imposed. Reliability estimates were slightly lower, but still moderate, for the T agility test (r = 0.70; ICC = 0.71[0.54–0.84]).
Pretesting routine intensities
Heart rates measured immediately upon completion of the low-intensity jogging bouts during the pretesting routine were not different between conditions. The heart rates after the 3-min jog at 50% of perceived maximum exertion (before the stretching) and after the 2-min jog at 60% of perceived exertion (after the stretching) were 125 ± 4 and 139 ± 19 bpm, respectively.
DISCUSSION
The main finding of the present study was that the inclusion of a period of either static (passive) or dynamic stretching within a comprehensive preexercise physical preparation routine (i.e., a “warm-up”) did not detectibly influence flexibility or maximal vertical jump, sprint running acceleration, or change of direction (T agility) test performances compared with NS control condition. In fact, intersession test reliability coefficients were good to excellent for 3-m running, SJ, CMJ, and DJ (ICC = 0.87–0.92) and 20-m sprint running (ICC = 0.93) tests, and moderate (ICC = 0.71) for the T agility test, despite the stretching component of the warm-up differing between sessions. On the basis of these results, athletic individuals who are well familiarized with the physical performance tasks and who complete a properly structured warm-up period (e.g., [1]) may not experience alterations in performance when short- or moderate-duration muscle stretching interventions are included within the warm-up period. The participants showed a clear bias in their beliefs with regard to the effects of stretching in the warm-up routine, with 90% (18/20) of participants expecting performances to be better after inclusion of a dynamic stretching period when asked to “list in descending order the stretch condition you believe will stimulate the best improvement in your performance.” This might result from participants having knowledge of sports science research, either as a university-level student or as an interested reader. It may also have influenced perceptions of preparedness for high-intensity physical activity after the warm-up period, with participants scoring 6.4 ± 1.6 on a 1–10 scale after a warm-up incorporating dynamic stretching when asked to rate “how effective you believe the warm-up will be on your performance” (1 = no effect/possibly harmful, 5 = noticeable improvement in performance, 10 = performance will improve dramatically). Nonetheless, no statistical difference was observed between ratings after any stretching condition, and warm-up routines incorporating 5-s static, 30-s static, or dynamic stretching were 97%, 87%, and 100% likely to be perceived of greater benefit than when no stretching was allowed. Furthermore, correlation coefficients (computed for repeated measurements within subjects [27]) were small (R2 = 0.1%–4.5%), indicating a lack of relationship. These data differ slightly from those presented recently by Janes et al. (21), where improvements in knee extensor, although not knee flexor, strength were observed after static stretching in participants who were told that the stretching should improve performance (i.e., there was an expectancy effect). We conclude that the participants felt as though the warm-up period prepared them better for high-intensity exercise performance when stretching was performed, irrespective of the type of stretching, than when no stretching was allowed. Although such beliefs did not meaningfully influence test performances in the present study, participants might theoretically perform better in a competitive sport environment when their perceptions of preparedness are higher, and this might be examined in future studies.
The current results, that static (passive) muscle stretching did not compromise, and dynamic stretching did not enhance, high-intensity exercise performance (Figs. 2 and 3), appear to contradict the consensus findings of previous research. However, several previous studies have shown a lack of effect of muscle stretching on high-intensity exercise performance when comprehensive warm-ups were performed. Taylor et al. (20) found no differences in vertical jump or 20-m sprint performances after a progressive, skill-based warm-up in high-level netball athletes despite performance decrements being observed immediately after a preceding static stretch period (VJ = −4.2% and 20-m sprint = −1.4%). In professional (English Premier League) soccer players, Little and Williams (28) observed no differences in 20-m sprint time or CMJ height after static or dynamic stretching, although a statistically faster zigzag agility (change of direction) performance after dynamic stretching, when the stretching was performed as part of a full warm-up session (notably, 20-m sprint performance was improved in both static and dynamic stretch conditions). Also, Samson et al. (19) found no differences in rapid kicking, CMJ, or 20-m sprint test performances between static and dynamic stretch conditions when performed alongside general and specific warm-up activities in recreational and competitive athletes. Such outcomes are not always observed when a warm-up opportunity is provided, however. Static stretching has resulted in decrements in high-intensity exercise performances when the sport-specific warm-ups were brief (e.g., 2 × 50-m sprints [29]) or of moderate duration and/or intensity (e.g., 10-m high knees, sidestepping, carioca, skipping, and 20-m zigzag run [30,31]). When considered together, the available evidence indicates that muscle stretching does not influence high-intensity exercise test performances when they are followed by a warm-up period of sufficient duration and incorporating exercises performed at high (or maximal) intensities. Such warm-up periods have been endorsed for the improvement of sports performance and reduction in musculoskeletal injury risk, even when static stretching is incorporated (3,32).
It is of practical importance that static or dynamic stretching early in the warm-up did not improve flexibility more than warm-up alone, as measured by a maximal sit-and-reach test. Time constraints did not allow for the specific testing of ranges of motion at different joints; however, a single multijoint test was expected to reveal changes given that nine different stretches were performed. The lack of change in sit-and-reach distance indicated that any effect of a stretch condition within the warm-up on maximal range of motion was negligible, which is in agreement with previous evidence (33). Thus, the dynamic warm-up activities may have elicited improvements in maximal range of motion that were not improved upon by the performance of further stretching, as has been observed previously (34,35). Alternatively, changes may have occurred in muscles other than those in the lower back and hamstrings and did not meaningfully affect sit-and-reach performance. Although it cannot be excluded that the addition of muscle stretching to a warm-up routine might improve maximal range of motion at specific joints, especially if longer or more intense stretch periods are practiced (36), the present results indicate that stretching provided negligible flexibility benefit in addition to the low- and high-intensity dynamic activities (i.e., high knees, butt kicks, and test practice) of the warm-up. It would be of interest to determine whether the stretching protocols evoked changes in muscle–tendon stiffness (extensibility) as opposed to maximum length (range of motion), as these have been shown to be differentially influenced by warm-up and stretching (36). Nonetheless, any possible effects in the current study were clearly insufficient to affect physical performance.
Steps were taken in the current study to improve both the external and the internal validity of the results. With respect to external validity, we accepted only participants who competed in running-based sports or performed at least three running-based exercise sessions per week and then allowed time for extensive familiarization of the tests. We also used stretching durations that are common in athlete populations (17,18), ensured that the static and dynamic stretch movement patterns were identical, did not allow a passive rest condition in the nonstretch condition, and imposed a 7-min no-activity period after the completion of the full warm-up period. These steps were taken to replicate as closely as possible what might occur in the sporting environment. With respect to internal validity, we ensured that the researchers who conducted the tests were blinded to the warm-up conditions completed by the participants (although these were closely supervised by another researcher), and all instructions were scripted so that they were identical on each test occasion; the stretch maneuvers were also shown by video with written instructions so that variations in instruction were minimized. It was not possible to recruit participants who lacked prior knowledge of the potential effects of stretching. However, by assessing participant beliefs before the study as well as after the completion of each warm-up condition, we were able to examine relationships between participant expectation and study outcomes. Together, these steps will have reduced both experimenter and participant bias, allowing us to more confidently accept the study outcomes. It should be acknowledged, however, that the study was not designed to examine the effects of prolonged periods of static (passive) stretching performed immediately before a physical task, as might be reflective of practice in some rehabilitation and resistance training settings.
One potential limitation of the current study design is that the tests were conducted in a circuit, with 4 min being allowed for the completion of each test block (i.e., 3-m running jump; SJ, CMJ, and DJ; 20-m sprint run; and T agility test). Therefore, the final test on any test day may have commenced up to 12 min after the commencement of the test battery, and it will have been performed after several other maximal-intensity tests. It can then be questioned whether tests performed closer to the end of the warm-up period might have been more strongly influenced by the interventions. However, our analysis did not reveal any evidence of an order effect of the tests, so performances achieved when a test was first in the circuit (immediately after the 7-min imposed rest) were not different to those when the same test was completed at another time point. On the basis of this evidence, it appears that the (lack of) effect of the stretching is consistent when a full warm-up is completed and a short post–warm-up rest is imposed regardless of the time elapsed or the number of other tests performed in the intervening period.
CONCLUSIONS
The results of the present randomized, controlled, crossover trial indicate that neither short- or moderate-duration static (passive) nor dynamic muscle stretching influence flexibility or high-intensity running, jumping, or change of direction (agility) performances in young, athletic individuals who perform a complete, progressive preexercise warm-up routine. However, the incorporation of static (passive) or dynamic stretching into a warm-up routine allowed for individuals to feel more confident of high performance in the ensuing sports-related tests; that is, there was a psychological effect. On the basis of the present results and previous findings of small-to-moderate reductions in muscle injury risk in running-based sports, we conclude that short- or moderate-duration static stretching should be allowed, or even promoted, as part of the warm-up routine before sports participation. According to our results, dynamic stretching practices may also be incorporated into the warm-up routine, although it should be reminded that no data currently exist documenting the influence of dynamic stretching on injury risk.
The authors are grateful to the athletes who took part in the study. The authors declare no conflicts of interest. No external funding was received for this research.
The results of the present study do not constitute endorsement by the American College of Sports Medicine. The authors declare that the results of the study are presented clearly, honestly, and without fabrication, falsification, or inappropriate data manipulation.
REFERENCES
1. Bishop D. Warm Up II: performance changes following active warm up and how to structure the warm up.
Sports Med. 2003;33(7):483–98.
2. Donaldson A, Cook J, Gabbe B, Lloyd DG, Young W, Finch CF. Bridging the gap between content and context: establishing expert consensus on the content of an exercise training program to prevent lower-limb injuries.
Clin J Sport Med. 2015;25(3):221–9.
3. Woods K, Bishop P, Jones E. Warm-up and stretching in the prevention of muscular injury.
Sports Med. 2007;37(12):1089–99.
4. Behm DG, Blazevich AJ, Kay AD, McHugh M. Acute effects of muscle stretching on physical performance, range of motion, and injury incidence in healthy active individuals: a systematic review.
Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2016;41(1):1–11.
5. Behm DG, Chaouachi A. A review of the acute effects of static and dynamic stretching on performance.
Eur J Appl Physiol. 2011;111(11):2633–51.
6. Knudson D. Stretching during warm-up: do we have enough evidence?
J Phys Educ Recr Dance. 1999;70(7):24–7.
7. Shrier I. Meta-analysis on preexercise stretching.
Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2004;36(10):1832.
8. Small K, Mc Naughton L, Matthews M. A systematic review into the efficacy of static stretching as part of a warm-up for the prevention of exercise-related injury.
Res Sports Med. 2008;16(3):213–31.
9. McHugh MP, Cosgrave CH. To stretch or not to stretch: the role of stretching in injury prevention and performance.
Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2010;20(2):169–81.
10. Shrier I. Does stretching help prevent injuries? In:
Evidence-Based Sports Medicine. Blackwell Publishing; 2007. pp. 36–58.
11. Australian Sports Commission.
AusPlay: Participation Data for the Sport Sector—Summary of Key National Findings October 2015 to September 2016 data. Australian Government, Australian Sports Commission Report; 2016: ASC34648.
12. Hootman JM, Dick R, Agel J. Epidemiology of collegiate injuries for 15 sports: summary and recommendations for injury prevention initiatives.
J Athl Train. 2007;42(2):311–9.
13. Kay AD, Blazevich AJ. Effect of acute static stretch on maximal muscle performance: a systematic review.
Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2012;44(1):154–64.
14. Magnusson P, Renström P. The European College of Sports Sciences Position Statement: the role of stretching exercises in sports.
Eur J Sports Sci. 2006;6(2):87–91.
15. Garber CE, Blissmer B, Deschenes MR, et al. American College of Sports Medicine Position Stand: quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory, musculoskeletal, and neuromotor fitness in apparently healthy adults: guidance for prescribing exercise.
Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011;43(7):1334–59.
16. Judge LW, Bellar DM, Gilreath EL, et al. An examination of preactivity and postactivity stretching practices of NCAA division I, NCAA division II, and NCAA division III track and field throws programs.
J Strength Cond Res. 2013;27(10):2691–9.
17. Ebben WP, Hintz MJ, Simenz CJ. Strength and conditioning practices of Major League Baseball strength and conditioning coaches.
J Strength Cond Res. 2005;19(3):538–46.
18. Simenz CJ, Dugan CA, Ebben WP. Strength and conditioning practices of National Basketball Association strength and conditioning coaches.
J Strength Cond Res. 2005;19(3):495–504.
19. Samson M, Button DC, Chaouachi A, Behm DG. Effects of dynamic and static stretching within general and activity specific warm-up protocols.
J Sports Sci Med. 2012;11(2):279–85.
20. Taylor K-L, Sheppard JM, Lee H, Plummer N. Negative effect of static stretching restored when combined with a sport specific warm-up component.
J Sci Med Sport. 2009;12(6):657–61.
21. Janes WC, Snow BB, Watkins CE, Noseworthy EA, Reid JC, Behm DG. Effect of participants’ static stretching knowledge or deception on the responses to prolonged stretching.
Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2016;41(10):1052–6.
22. Rosenthal R.
Experimenter Effects in Behavioral Research. Vol. XIII. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1966. pp. 464.
23. Popp JK, Bellar DM, Hoover DL, et al. Pre- and post-activity stretching practices of collegiate athletic trainers in the United States.
J Strength Cond Res. 2017;31(9):2347–54.
24. Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology.
Canadian Physical Activity, Fitness & Lifestyle Approach: CSEP—Health & Fitness Program’s Health-Related Appraisal & Counselling Strategy. Ottawa (Ontario): Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology; 2004.
25. Hopkins WG. A spreadsheet for deriving a confidence interval, mechanistic inference and clinical inference from a
P value.
Sportscience. 2007;11:16–21.
26. Batterham AM, Hopkins WG. Making meaningful inferences about magnitudes.
Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2006;1(1):50–7.
27. Bland JM, Altman DG. Statistics notes: calculating correlation coefficients with repeated observations: part 1—correlation within subjects.
BMJ. 1995;310(6977):446.
28. Little T, Williams AG. Effects of differential stretching protocols during warm-ups on high-speed motor capacities in professional soccer players.
J Strength Cond Res. 2006;20(1):203–7.
29. Fletcher IM, Anness R. The acute effects of combined static and dynamic stretch protocols on fifty-meter sprint performance in track-and-field athletes.
J Strength Cond Res. 2007;21(3):784–7.
30. Pearce AJ, Kidgell DJ, Zois J, Carlson JS. Effects of secondary warm up following stretching.
Eur J Appl Physiol. 2009;105(2):175–83.
31. Pearce AJ, Latella C, Kidgell DJ. Secondary warm-up following stretching on vertical jumping, change of direction, and straight line speed.
Eur J Sports Sci. 2012;12(2):103–12.
32. Herman K, Barton C, Malliaras P, Morrissey D. The effectiveness of neuromuscular warm-up strategies, that require no additional equipment, for preventing lower limb injuries during sports participation: a systematic review.
BMC Med. 2012;10(1):75.
33. Beedle BB, Mann CL. A comparison of two warm-ups on joint range of motion.
J Strength Cond Res. 2007;21(3):776.
34. Kay AD, Husbands-Beasley J, Blazevich AJ. Effects of contract-relax, static stretching, and isometric contractions on muscle–tendon mechanics.
Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2015;47(10):2181–90.
35. O’Sullivan K, Murray E, Sainsbury D. The effect of warm-up, static stretching and dynamic stretching on hamstring flexibility in previously injured subjects.
BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2009;10:37.
36. McNair PJ, Stanley SN. Effect of passive stretching and jogging on the series elastic muscle stiffness and range of motion of the ankle joint.
Br J Sports Med. 1996;30(4):313–7.