Secondary Logo

Journal Logo

A Simple Analogy for Nervous System Plasticity After Injury

Fouad, Karim1,2; Forero, Juan1; Hurd, Caitlin1

Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews: April 2015 - Volume 43 - Issue 2 - p 100–106
doi: 10.1249/JES.0000000000000040
Articles
Free
Journal Club

When considering plasticity, the central nervous system can be viewed as a building block house. After damage, building components might be lost or loosened and may be rearranged by renovation, analogous to neuroplasticity that occurs after central nervous system injury. In both scenarios, the location and severity of damage will determine the efficacy of renovation/rehabilitation and thus the quality of the adapted structure.

Viewing the central nervous system as a set of building components is useful to describe injury-induced neuroplasticity.

1Faculty of Rehabilitation Medicine, and 2Institute for Neurosciences and Mental Health, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada

Address for correspondence: Karim Fouad, Faculty of Rehabilitation Medicine, University of Alberta, 3-48 Corbett Hall, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada T6G 2G4 (E-mail: karim.fouad@ualberta.ca).

Accepted for publication: October 29, 2014.

Associate Editor: E. Paul Zehr, Ph.D.

Figure

Figure

Editor’s note: Go online to view the Journal Club questions in the Supplemental Digital Content: seehttp://links.lww.com/ESSR/A12.

Back to Top | Article Outline

INTRODUCTION

The dogma that the adult mammalian central nervous system (CNS) (brain, brainstem, and spinal cord) is a hardwired inflexible structure was upheld firmly for many years. This view was fueled by the findings that recovery after CNS injuries is limited to nonexistent; lost tissue cannot be replaced sufficiently and injured axons are unable to regenerate.

However, it is accepted generally now that the CNS can learn, rearrange, and adapt, a process often referred to as neuroplasticity. Plasticity can occur in the healthy CNS as experienced during learning of new skill or memorization of new information and after injuries where an even greater extent of plasticity can occur.

A number of research studies examining the effects of injuries to the central and peripheral nervous systems have reported plasticity at various anatomical and physiological levels, including the cortex, brainstem, and spinal cord. The mechanisms involved in this plasticity include changes in cortical maps, anatomical changes such as collateral sprouting, synaptic changes, and adaptations in neuronal properties (24,26). It has to be kept in mind that adaptive processes in the CNS do not occur only in motor systems and are not always beneficial. Quite the contrary, plasticity frequently has been associated with undesirable outcomes including autonomic dysreflexia, neuropathic pain, and spasticity (4). However, the focus here will be on motor function after injury.

Although various pharmacological approaches to promote plasticity have been reported, intensive training currently is viewed as the most successful treatment to promote functionally meaningful adaptive changes after CNS injuries. One could consider the effect of rehabilitative training as an extension of spontaneous recovery. In addition, in accordance with processes in the developing nervous system, where activity is essential to the establishment of functional circuitry, rehabilitative training (and, thereby, the activation of neuronal networks in a meaningful pattern) is needed to translate plasticity-promoting effects of drugs into functional recovery (7,34).

Considering the multifaceted underlying processes of plasticity and the fact that the use of the term plasticity is often ambiguous, it becomes quite clear that it is, and likely always will be, difficult to comprehend fully all adaptive changes after injuries of the CNS. We define plasticity as the entire spectrum of changes in the intact or injured CNS including structural changes, alterations in synaptic strength, and changes in neuronal properties. Thus, despite the analogy introduced in this article, plasticity should not be viewed as limited to structural changes but also should include functional reorganization of spared elements. Furthermore, predicting the effects of plasticity on particular aspects of recovery and maladaptive side effects is exceedingly difficult. Not surprisingly, investigating the mechanisms of plasticity currently is a pronounced focus of research to develop treatments targeted to promote the beneficial aspects of plasticity after injuries and diseases of the nervous system and, thereby, functional recovery. This is a daunting task, especially considering the relatively slow progression of our understanding of how the uninjured CNS functions. On the other hand, from a clinical point of view, with an interest in maximizing the positive effects of rehabilitative training, a detailed understanding of the mechanisms of plasticity might not be essential. In this case, a simplified view of the changes occurring after CNS injury might be sufficient and could prevent getting lost in details. We propose that the nervous system can be described as a house made of building components, including bricks and mortar. These building components represent neuronal structures or “hardware” of the CNS. The mortar that holds bricks together can be viewed as growth-inhibitory components of the CNS such as myelin-associated inhibitors or chondroitin sulfate proteoglycans (CSPG) of the perineuronal net (2,13). We are using the metaphor of a building structure in combination with the restriction that, comparable to the CNS, no additional building components are available after damage. It is important to note that even the uninjured CNS is changing constantly, and these changes can occur at a physiological level. This is comparable to superficial changes to a building. In contrast, more substantial remodeling has to occur after damage to a structure to keep the structure functional — similar to the structural plasticity after CNS injury. Viewing the nervous system as a set of building components simplifies the interpretation and illustration of adaptive changes after injury, stimulates creative thinking about neuroplasticity, and may assist with predictions regarding treatment effects such as rehabilitative training. Our proposed analogy is supported by various findings from studies in animal models of, and individuals with, CNS injuries.

Back to Top | Article Outline

ANALOGIES BETWEEN THE CNS AND A BUILDING BLOCK STRUCTURE

Damage to the CNS Enables Limited Remodeling and Adjustments

Similar to a natural disaster (e.g., earthquake) that loosens a building by shaking the structure and breaking up the mortar (Fig. 1), an injury to the nervous system temporarily enhances plasticity of the CNS. Comparable to the loosened mortar (that allows restructuring of a building), growth-inhibitory components such as CSPG in the perineuronal net are downregulated (13) after injury of the CNS. This results in heightened structural flexibility (i.e., enhanced neuroplasticity) and, thus, allows “remodeling,” a process used and amplified by intensive rehabilitative training (8,24). This remodeling conforms to certain rules that match the scenario in a building. For example, building components can be moved and reused, and the structure can be reassembled in an alternative or compensatory fashion (see later sections). Importantly, like a structure where building components have been damaged and cannot be reused, the nervous system cannot replace lost “neuronal hardware.” More specifically, there is no general supply of stem cells that can replace lost neurons or the surrounding glia cells. The remodeling process should not be viewed as “repair” because it will not restore the original structure but will instead maximize the use of spared building components. This is mirrored after CNS injuries by functional recovery that mainly consists of compensatory approaches rather than restoration of original functions (20,33). Spontaneous recovery can be viewed as a “home remodeling” without using any new building materials to rebuild, and plasticity-promoting treatments (including rehabilitative training) are comparable to hiring a contractor who has access to special tools (but not building components). Similar to home repair, when remodeling is performed without the assistance of a professional, the end product might not be satisfactory. In fact, if the homeowner attempts to repair the home, it may be more difficult to restore it back to normal. This could be compared with spontaneous recovery that is based frequently on functional substitution or the establishment of compensatory movements, whereas targeted rehabilitative training can enforce the training of lost function. Similarly, the immediate approach by a contractor yields a better chance to recreate a functional house (also see Fig. 2D). If repairs are delayed, further problems may arise or there may be further structural damage because weakened areas have not been reinforced. This could be compared with spontaneous recovery that is based frequently on functional substitution or the establishment of compensatory movements, whereas targeted rehabilitative training can enforce the training of lost function. Once a compensatory function has been established, retraining is more challenging. Furthermore, the phase of injury-induced enhanced neuroplasticity after CNS injury offers a time window of opportunity for rehabilitative training, during which the nervous system can rewire and is particularly responsive to activity-based therapies comparable to “critical periods” during development (23). The mechanisms that create this critical period or window of opportunity and enhanced neuroplasticity are not understood completely, but the injury-induced transient upregulation of so-called immediate early genes, neurotropic factors, and the downregulation of CSPG have been included in the speculations (29,31).

Figure 1

Figure 1

Figure 2

Figure 2

Back to Top | Article Outline

The Amount of Spared Hardware Determines the Degree of Spontaneous Recovery

Spontaneous functional recovery can be found for a few weeks after CNS injury. The duration of this process and the degree of recovery greatly depend on the severity of the injury. For example, it is generally acknowledged that more severe injuries allow a lesser degree of spontaneous recovery and likely reduced efficacy of rehabilitative training. Robust repair mechanisms to replace lost tissue are limited because of insufficient availability of stem cells and the inability of neurons to regenerate in the adult mammalian CNS (17). Consequently, beyond the obvious observation that more severe lesions result in more severe deficits, the number of spared neuronal building blocks also determines the degree of plasticity and, thus, spontaneous and training-induced functional recovery. When comparing this with a building, extensive damage to all bricks, analogous to a complete transection of the spinal cord or the ablation of an entire cortical area, results in no pieces remaining for meaningful rebuilding or plasticity (Fig. 2G). However, small injuries to the brain or spinal cord can be compensated for immediately without any obvious effects and often are hardly noticeable in animal models or the clinical setting (12,14). This offers a straightforward analogy to a building block structure: the more damage that occurs, the less restructuring without additional building materials is possible. Minor damage can be “patched up” without influencing the overall stability of the building but, when critical load bearing components are destroyed, reconstruction is impossible (Fig. 2A, B).

Back to Top | Article Outline

The Nonlinear Relation Between Structural Damage and Function

Although severe lesions to the CNS hardly allow spontaneous recovery controlled by descending input from the brain and small lesions can be compensated for immediately (see previous section), the relation between functional recovery and tissue loss generally is not linear (12,16,30). For example, the success rate in a reaching task in an animal model of spinal cord injury (SCI) remains high after small lesions; with increasing tissue damage, however, a drastic decline in function occurs before a plateau is reached. This plateau can be maintained for a surprisingly large increase in lesion size. Yet, when a critical point in lesion size is exceeded, the ability to reach is abolished even though not all tissue has been injured (Fig. 2). This could be interpreted as compensation by using spared, previously noncritical components to take on more critical roles when small losses of neuronal hardware occur. Adaptive strategies allow for the maintenance of a new plateau in motor function after a significant loss of tissue. No functionally meaningful neuronal network can be established once a critical amount of tissue loss has been exceeded. This is comparable to a house where a few building components can be damaged and, if they are essential for the house to be habitable, they can be compensated for by using components less critical for the structural integrity of the house (Fig. 2B, C). However, with a continuous increase in damage, a threshold will be reached where the house will have to be remodeled to remain habitable, for example, by downsizing (Fig. 2D, E). With further increase in the number of damaged building components, too many pieces will be missing and even the smaller house will become uninhabitable (Fig. 2F). Although pieces of the structure might still be available, it cannot be remodeled to be habitable again. These findings especially are important when attempting to judge and compare the efficacy of treatments including rehabilitative training. Treatment effects may range from significant to nonexistent depending on the severity of the lesion.

Back to Top | Article Outline

When Remodeling, Specific Use of Building Components Determines the Outcome

Currently, the most successful treatment after injuries to the CNS is rehabilitative training. It has been reported that training effects are often task specific and may even influence untrained motor functions negatively. For example, training to walk forward on a treadmill does not translate into recovery of backward walking or stepping in place in individuals with SCI (9). Cats with complete SCI that were trained to stand have more difficulty relearning to step compared with untrained cats and step-trained cats (10). Similarly, step-trained cats are not able to stand as well as stand-trained cats. Comparably, rats with SCI trained to grasp for food pellets showed deficits in an untrained task, that is, crossing a horizontal ladder (8). In addition, it has been reported that training of one language after stroke in bilingual individuals reduces the ability to reestablish speech in the second language (1). These findings suggest that spared neuronal hardware can be “remodeled” after an injury to the CNS according to the trained tasks. Similar to the developing nervous system (11), this process is driven by activity and illustrates an important aspect of postinjury rehabilitative training. After injury, this process will result in compensation, or functional substitution, rather than repair to reach the status quo.

In contrast, it is well established that disuse of an affected limb or of particular tasks after CNS injuries can contribute to further dysfunction, an idea clearly stated by the phrase “use it or lose it,” which is established firmly in the field of stroke (25). Constraint-induced movement therapy is an approach commonly used to counteract this effect on affected function by enforcing the use of the affected limb/function, thereby preventing compensatory approaches. The continuous training of the affected limb and its motor function by using this therapy is used successfully in the clinical setting to counteract the disuse of the affected limb (22,35). The success of constraint induced movement therapy inspired similar approaches in other areas, including retraining of speech where compensatory mechanisms of communication are restricted (21).

In summary, it can be generalized that the available neuronal hardware is dedicated in a competitive manner on a first-come, first-served basis, and tasks that require higher neuronal activity are favored. This scenario is comparable to a building where, once a structure is damaged, broken components become useless and spared pieces can be reused to recreate certain portions of the original structure. There will be competition for the limited building materials, which will decide the final structure and, therefore, functional capabilities (Fig. 3A). For example, the roof will be leaky if building pieces from the roof are used to stabilize the walls. Even though moving building pieces from the nervous system is not a realistic option, integrating neurons in different circuitries may result in an alternative utilization for them.

Figure 3

Figure 3

Back to Top | Article Outline

The Damaged Location Can Be Used to Predict Functional Outcome

Recovery after injury is not only determined by the amount of tissue damage but also depends on the location of the injury (6,16,30). This is based on the fact that the CNS is built by functional units. For example, different parts of the body are represented by specific cortical areas, and specific spinal tracts with different functions project in bundles in specific locations within the spinal cord. This can explain why a small lesion in the ventral spinal cord can have more severe effects on locomotor ability than a big lesion in the dorsal cord because tracts that are involved in initiating locomotion project in the ventrolateral portion of the spinal cord. Another example is that recovery in reaching function is better when two descending tracts involved in reaching are lesioned partly rather than the complete ablation of one track. Parallels to a building can be drawn, where it would be easier to create a downsized structure when pieces of all the building components are still available (Fig. 3B).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Effects of Spinal Lesions Staggered in Time Support the Reconstruction Idea

So far we have described a model in which the building blocks of the nervous system, similar to building components of a house, can be reassembled/rewired in an alternative manner after injury. Furthermore, we discussed that rehabilitative training is a key component to achieve this. Illustrative examples where an injury induces permanent changes to the nervous system are provided by experiments with two lesions staggered in time. This approach has been used with injuries to the spinal cord, where convincing differences in the response to a lesion were found depending on whether a second spinal lesion was induced at the same time as the initial one or later in time. For example, a spinal lesion ablating one side of the cord allows for nearly complete recovery of stepping in rats and cats alike (3,18). Following our hypothesis, this is caused by an activity-based rearrangement of building materials and reinforcing remodeling. When such a lesion is followed by a complete spinal transection caudal to the first lesion a few weeks later, the spinal networks below the lesion are able to recover the same stepping pattern much faster when compared with animals that only received a single transection (19). This demonstrates that the first injury induced compensatory rewiring within the spared spinal cord network that is maintained even when disconnected completely from the brain. Thus, if a house that was remodeled after a first earthquake had to overcome a second earthquake, the overall damage would not be necessarily comparable to the damage that same house would have had if only a single stronger earthquake had hit it.

Another example demonstrating the “remodeling” of the CNS after injuries comes from an experiment where we examined the recovery of reaching function for weeks following a cervical lesion of the corticospinal tract in rats. This recovery is paralleled by rewiring of injured corticospinal tract (CST) fibers to alternate targets, a process that is amplified by rehabilitative training (8). A second lesion of the corticospinal tract rostral to these new connections abolishes the observed recovery completely (15), indicating that this compensatory rewiring is meaningful functionally.

These results provide strong evidence for the involvement of plasticity in recovery and demonstrate that training is a key concept to enhance it. The spontaneous recovery found after CNS injuries is likely based on activity and training performed inadvertently during daily life. This has been demonstrated dramatically by the lack of spontaneous recovery in rats with thoracic SCI when the use of their hindlimbs was disabled (5).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Building Block Structure Analogy Applies Not Only to Common Treatment Strategies

In this article, we have compared the injured CNS with a structure where new building supplies are unavailable. Obviously, treatments designed to introduce new “building materials” or to minimize damage after CNS injury would be desirable. For example, a much-anticipated approach is the grafting of stem cells into injured areas of the CNS (17,28). Theoretically, these cells have the ability to develop into various cells types, including neurons and glia, thereby replacing damaged building materials. This approach would extend the opportunities to repair an injured structure greatly; however, to properly integrate new building blocks, rehabilitative training likely will be needed to connect the pieces in a functionally meaningful manner.

An alternative and already used approach for treating CNS injuries is based on the knowledge that lost neuronal hardware is difficult to replace. These treatments focus on neuroprotection. To be effective, these treatments have to be applied as soon as possible after injury. Following CNS injuries, there are various approaches in clinical use, in trials, or currently tested in animal models ranging from cooling the injured CNS to the application of antiinflammatory drugs (27,32). In our model, they could be compared with dampening the effects of the earthquake by reinforcing structures immediately after the damage to prevent further (secondary) damage.

Back to Top | Article Outline

CONCLUSIONS

In conclusion, a detailed understanding of the mechanisms of neuronal plasticity is important for optimizing treatments for CNS injury. However, from a more clinical point of view, these intricate details may be unnecessary. Viewing the CNS as a house made out of different components of neuronal hardware that can be reassembled in response to injury is not necessarily a simplified view of injury-induced plasticity, but a metaphor derived from various findings. A more tangible view of injury-induced plasticity in the brain and spinal cord may be helpful in communicating the process of plasticity.

The authors thank Dr. Jaynie Yang for comments on the manuscript.

This work was supported by the Canadian Institute of Health Research and Alberta Innovates Health Solutions.

K. Fouad is supported by AIHS, CIHR, and ISRT.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Back to Top | Article Outline

References

1. Abutalebi J, Rosa PAD, Tettamanti M, Green DW, Cappa SF. Bilingual aphasia and language control: a follow-up fMRI and intrinsic connectivity study. Brain Lang. 2009; 109 (2–3): 141–56.
2. Akbik F, Cafferty WBJ, Strittmatter SM. Myelin associated inhibitors: a link between injury-induced and experience-dependent plasticity. Exp. Neurol. 2012; 235 (1): 43–52.
3. Ballermann M, Fouad K. Spontaneous locomotor recovery in spinal cord injured rats is accompanied by anatomical plasticity of reticulospinal fibers. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006; 23 (8): 1988–96.
4. Brown A, Weaver LC. The dark side of neuroplasticity. Exp. Neurol. 2012; 235 (1): 133–41.
5. Caudle KL, Brown EH, Shum-Siu A, et al. Hindlimb immobilization in a wheelchair alters functional recovery following contusive spinal cord injury in the adult rat. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2011; 25 (8): 729–39.
6. Chen C-L, Tang F-T, Chen H-C, Chung C-Y, Wong M-K. Brain lesion size and location: effects on motor recovery and functional outcome in stroke patients. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2000; 81 (4): 447–52.
7. García-Alías G, Barkhuysen S, Buckle M, Fawcett JW. Chondroitinase ABC treatment opens a window of opportunity for task-specific rehabilitation. Nat. Neurosci. 2009; 12 (9): 1145–51.
8. Girgis J, Merrett D, Kirkland S, Metz GAS, Verge V, Fouad K. Reaching training in rats with spinal cord injury promotes plasticity and task specific recovery. Brain J. Neurol. 2007; 130 (Pt. 11): 2993–3003.
9. Grasso R, Ivanenko YP, Zago M, Molinari M, Scivoletto G, Lacquaniti F. Recovery of forward stepping in spinal cord injured patients does not transfer to untrained backward stepping. Exp. Brain Res. 2004; 157 (3): 377–82.
10. Hodgson JA, Roy RR, de Leon R, Dobkin B, Edgerton VR. Can the mammalian lumbar spinal cord learn a motor task? Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1994; 26 (12): 1491–7.
11. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Effects of monocular deprivation in kittens. Naunyn-Schmiedebergs Arch. Für Exp. Pathol. Pharmakol. 1964; 248: 492–7.
12. Hurd C, Weishaupt N, Fouad K. Anatomical correlates of recovery in single pellet reaching in spinal cord injured rats. Exp. Neurol. 2013; 247: 605–14.
13. Karetko-Sysa M, Skangiel-Kramska J, Nowicka D. Disturbance of perineuronal nets in the perilesional area after photothrombosis is not associated with neuronal death. Exp. Neurol. 2011; 231 (1): 113–26.
14. Kitago T, Liang J, Huang VS, et al. Improvement after constraint-induced movement therapy: recovery of normal motor control or task-specific compensation? Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2013; 27 (2): 99–109.
15. Krajacic A, Weishaupt N, Girgis J, Tetzlaff W, Fouad K. Training-induced plasticity in rats with cervical spinal cord injury: effects and side effects. Behav. Brain Res. 2010; 214 (2): 323–31.
16. Loy DN, Talbott JF, Onifer SM, et al. Both dorsal and ventral spinal cord pathways contribute to overground locomotion in the adult rat. Exp. Neurol. 2002; 177 (2): 575–80.
17. Lu P, Kadoya K, Tuszynski MH. Axonal growth and connectivity from neural stem cell grafts in models of spinal cord injury. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2014; 27C: 103–9.
18. Martinez M, Delivet-Mongrain H, Rossignol S. Treadmill training promotes spinal changes leading to locomotor recovery after partial spinal cord injury in cats. J. Neurophysiol. 2013; 109 (12): 2909–22.
19. Martinez M, Rossignol S. A dual spinal cord lesion paradigm to study spinal locomotor plasticity in the cat. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2013; 1279 (1): 127–34.
20. McKenna JE, Whishaw IQ. Complete compensation in skilled reaching success with associated impairments in limb synergies, after dorsal column lesion in the rat. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 1999; 19 (5): 1885–94.
21. Meinzer M, Rodriguez AD, Gonzalez Rothi LJ. First decade of research on constrained-induced treatment approaches for aphasia rehabilitation. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2012; 93 (1 Suppl.): S35–45.
22. Miltner WHR, Bauder H, Sommer M, Dettmers C, Taub E. Effects of constraint-induced movement therapy on patients with chronic motor deficits after stroke: a replication. Stroke. 1999; 30 (3): 586–92.
23. Norrie BA, Nevett-Duchcherer JM, Gorassini MA. Reduced functional recovery by delaying motor training after spinal cord injury. J. Neurophysiol. 2005; 94 (1): 255–64.
24. Nudo RJ. Recovery after brain injury: mechanisms and principles. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013; 7: 887.
25. Nudo RJ, Wise BM, SiFuentes F, Milliken GW. Neural substrates for the effects of rehabilitative training on motor recovery after ischemic infarct. Science. 1996; 272 (5269): 1791–4.
26. Onifer SM, Smith GM, Fouad K. Plasticity after spinal cord injury: relevance to recovery and approaches to facilitate it. Neurother. J. Am. Soc. Exp. Neurother. 2011; 8 (2): 283–93.
27. Onose G, Anghelescu A, Muresanu DF, et al. A review of published reports on neuroprotection in spinal cord injury. Spinal Cord. 2009; 47 (10): 716–26.
28. Reeves A, Keirstead HS. Stem cell–based strategies for spinal cord injury repair. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 2012; 760: 16–24.
29. Rickhag M, Wieloch T, Gidö G, et al. Comprehensive regional and temporal gene expression profiling of the rat brain during the first 24 h after experimental stroke identifies dynamic ischemia-induced gene expression patterns, and reveals a biphasic activation of genes in surviving tissue. J. Neurochem. 2006; 96 (1): 14–29.
30. Schucht P, Raineteau O, Schwab ME, Fouad K. Anatomical correlates of locomotor recovery following dorsal and ventral lesions of the rat spinal cord. Exp. Neurol. 2002; 176 (1): 143–53.
31. Sist B, Fouad K, Winship IR. Plasticity beyond peri-infarct cortex: spinal up regulation of structural plasticity, neurotrophins, and inflammatory cytokines during recovery from cortical stroke. Exp. Neurol. 2014; 252: 47–56.
32. Sutherland BA, Minnerup J, Balami JS, Arba F, Buchan AM, Kleinschnitz C. Neuroprotection for ischaemic stroke: translation from the bench to the bedside. Int. J. Stroke Off. J. Int. Stroke Soc. 2012; 7 (5): 407–18.
33. Webb AA, Muir GD. Compensatory locomotor adjustments of rats with cervical or thoracic spinal cord hemisections. J. Neurotrauma. 2002; 19 (2): 239–56.
34. Weishaupt N, Li S, Di Pardo A, Sipione S, Fouad K. Synergistic effects of BDNF and rehabilitative training on recovery after cervical spinal cord injury. Behav. Brain Res. 2013; 239: 31–42.
35. Wolf SL, Lecraw DE, Barton LA, Jann BB. Forced use of hemiplegic upper extremities to reverse the effect of learned nonuse among chronic stroke and head-injured patients. Exp. Neurol. 1989; 104 (2): 125–32.
Keywords:

central nervous system; spinal cord; motor function; plasticity; rehabilitation

Supplemental Digital Content

Back to Top | Article Outline
© 2015 American College of Sports Medicine