Empathy Decline and Its Reasons: A Systematic Review of Studies With Medical Students and Residents : Academic Medicine

Secondary Logo

Journal Logo

Empathy

Empathy Decline and Its Reasons: A Systematic Review of Studies With Medical Students and Residents

Neumann, Melanie PhD; Edelhäuser, Friedrich MD; Tauschel, Diethard MD; Fischer, Martin R. MD; Wirtz, Markus PhD; Woopen, Christiane MD, PhD; Haramati, Aviad MD; Scheffer, Christian MD, MME

Author Information
Academic Medicine 86(8):p 996-1009, August 2011. | DOI: 10.1097/ACM.0b013e318221e615
  • Free

Abstract

Communicating with patients is an essential medical activity.1 Patient–physician communication not only helps capture the anamnesis and transmit information but also has a therapeutic effect and supports the patient's healing process. Patient–physician communication has been shown to have a positive effect on psychosocial outcomes (e.g., quality of life, anxiety, depression) and on objectively measurable outcome parameters (e.g., symptom reduction, lowering of blood pressure and blood glucose levels).2–5

Physician empathy is a particularly effective therapeutic element of patient–physician communication. Mercer and Reynolds'6(pS10) widely accepted definition describes physician empathy as the ability of a physician to “(a) understand the patient's situation, perspective and feelings (and their attached meanings), (b) communicate that understanding and check its accuracy and (c) act on that understanding with the patient in a helpful (therapeutic) way.” Such empathic behavior may lead to

  • patients' reporting more about their symptoms and concerns,7–11
  • physicians' increased diagnostic accuracy,9,10,12,13
  • patients' receiving more illness-specific information,14–16
  • patients' increased participation and education,6,16,17
  • patients' increased compliance and satisfaction,16,18–20
  • patients' greater enablement,17,21–23 and
  • patients' reduced emotional distress and increased quality of life.15

Further, in patients with the common cold, physician empathy is a significant predictor of the duration and severity of the illness and is associated with immune system changes in immune cytokine interleukin-8.24

These specific therapeutic effects of physician empathy and their mutual associations can be detailed with the help of the “effect model of empathic communication in the clinical encounter,”25 which demonstrates how an empathically communicating physician can achieve improved patient outcomes.

This therapeutic relevance emphasizes the importance of developing and supporting physician empathy during medical school and residency. Moreover, according to the Institute of Medicine, empathy also plays an important role in achieving patient-centeredness, which is one of the six main goals of a 21st-century health system and comprises the “qualities of compassion, empathy, and responsiveness to the needs, values, and expressed preferences of the individual patient.”26(p48) In addition, studies have concluded that patients most value empathy, support, and information from their physicians.27–31 Correspondingly, empathy has long been a key element of the framework of medical professionalism32 as well as a defined educational objective in medical training in the United States, Canada, and Switzerland.33–35

By conducting a systematic review of the literature according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Guidelines,36 we aimed to describe the current state of research and address the following questions: (1) How does empathy change in trainees during medical school and residency? (2) Which factors influence trainees' ability to empathize?

Method

Data sources

During June 2009 to February 2010, three of us (M.N., C.S., D.T.) performed a systematic review of the literature to identify original articles reporting quantitative and qualitative investigations of the determinants of, development of, and changes in empathy during medical school and residency. We searched the PubMed, EMBASE, and PsycINFO electronic databases using the National Library of Medicine's Medical Subject Heading terms empathy, medical education, and change (see Table 1). We also conducted a manual search of reprint files, reference sections of review articles, and other publications.

T1-24
Table 1:
Key Words and Medical Subject Heading (MeSH) Terms Used in Searches of the PubMed, EMBASE, and PsycINFO Electronic Databases

Eligible studies were those published from January 1990 through January 2010 in English with a sample size of 30 or more. We excluded from our review intervention studies intended to enhance empathy because they were recently analyzed in a review by Stepien and Baernstein.37 We also excluded studies on the psychometric qualities of different empathy measures because such studies were recently reviewed by Hemmerdinger et al38 and Pedersen.39

Data extraction

Three of us (M.N., C.S., D.T.) identified relevant publications from the generated list by examining publication titles and abstracts and reading the entire article if in doubt. We excluded publications that did not meet the eligibility criteria, did not examine medical students or residents, or did not present empirical research, as well as those that presented personal experiences or case studies without explaining how the experiences or examples were selected or analyzed. We resolved any disagreements through discussion until we reached consensus.

Data synthesis and analysis

First, one author (M.N.) read all of the relevant studies on empathy during medical school and residency and created Appendix 1, which describes these studies and their findings. Second, she analyzed the reasons for changes in empathy that each study investigated and discussed. Third, she evaluated the methodological quality of each study, focusing on study design and method, response rates, and the validity of the self-assessment measures used. The other authors validated her analysis and conclusions. We discussed and resolved all disagreements.

Results

Study characteristics

We identified 669 unique studies. Among those, 18 studies satisfied all inclusion and no exclusion criteria (see Figure 1). Eleven investigated empathy during medical school, and 7 studied empathy during residency (Appendix 1). Nearly all of the studies were conducted in the United States, with the exception of 1 study conducted in Poland40 and 2 in the United Kingdom.41,42 All 18 studies were based on standardized questionnaires or surveys with students and residents and exclusively used tools for self-assessment of empathy.

F1-24
Figure 1:
Flowchart showing the study inclusion process. * Empathy, medical education, and change were used as Medical Subject Heading search terms to conduct the electronic database searches (see Table 1).

Changes in empathy during medical school and residency

None of the 18 studies documented increases in trainees' self-assessed empathy. Two of the studies showed increases in empathy during early student years but significant declines on entering the clinical practice phase when students had contact with patients.41,43

Of the 11 studies on medical students, 3 had longitudinal designs and 8 had cross-sectional designs. All 3 longitudinal studies44–46 and 6 cross-sectional studies40,41,43,47–49 described significant declines in empathy as training progressed. One of the other cross-sectional studies noted a trend toward empathy decline, but the trend was not significant.50 The last cross-sectional study, by Todres et al,42 suggested stable emotional intelligence scores during medical school. However, the study's “managing emotions” subscale of the emotional intelligence score showed significantly improved scores in final-year students compared with those of students in their first two years of study.

Of the seven studies on residents, five had longitudinal designs,51–55 one incorporated both longitudinal and cross-sectional designs,56 and one had a cross-sectional design.57 Six of these studies found significant downward trends in self-assessed empathy51–55,57; in the other study, a significant downward trend was present in the longitudinal results whereas only a slight trend was present in the cross-sectional findings.56

Reasons for empathy decline during medical school and residency

Each study investigated only a small number of influential factors compared with those theoretically possible.32 Investigators' analysis of obvious variables, such as gender41,44,48–50 and age,42,45,48 did not yield consistent results in studies of medical students.42,45,48

Some studies of medical students investigated specialty choice as a determinant of self-perceived empathy and indicated that medical students who selected patient-oriented specialties had higher empathy scores than did those who entered patient-remote areas (e.g., surgery, radiology).43,44,46,50

Ten of the 11 studies that focused on medical students40,41,43–50 agreed that self-assessed empathy decreased significantly between the student's third year of study and his or her first experience with a patient. Similarly, the seven studies of residents found that empathy decreased during clinical practice.51–57

Studies that investigated distress (e.g., burnout, low sense of well-being, reduced quality of life, depression) identified it as a factor with significant influence on self-assessed empathy in medical students and residents.47–49,51–53,55,57 Because distress seems to be a main cause of empathy decline, almost all studies considered the issue of why trainees experience it. The following points represent the common denominators in the studies' discussions of trainee distress and describe aspects of the “hidden curriculum”32:

  • Mistreatment by superiors or mentors: Medical students may experience situations of harassment, belittlement, degradation, humiliation, gender-specific discrimination, or sexual harassment.45–47,49
  • Vulnerability of medical students and residents: Values of idealism, enthusiasm, and humanity are present in students at the beginning of medical school,49 but these may diminish as trainees are confronted with clinical reality (characterized by illness, human suffering, and death) and their focus shifts to technology and objectivity rather than the humanistic aspects of medicine.43,45
  • Social support problems: Students and residents suffer from reduced contact with their families and a lack of social support from their peer groups.45,49,51
  • High workload: Students and residents face long working hours, with an associated lack of sleep and inadequate relaxation time.43,49,51

Some studies also discussed aspects of the “formal/informal curriculum”32 as potential causes for empathy decline. These include

  • short length of stay of the patient, which can result in a fragmented patient–physician relationship and allows no time for related work or corresponding learning from and with the patient43,45;
  • unsuitable learning environment, which may include unstructured studying,49 few “bedside interactions,”43 and medical students' being treated like immature human beings46; and
  • inadequate role models combined with the media's idealized view of the medical profession, which can lead students to hold unrealistic expectations regarding physicians' behavior.44–46,49

In addition, one study44 considered elitist thinking by medical students and residents as another potential reason for empathy decline and found that the possibility of belonging to an elite and privileged group may induce a rational distancing from the patient. Further, various authors mentioned certain personality traits as possible determinants of empathy decline.41,53 However, these traits were not discussed extensively in all of the studies.

Discussion

Decline of empathy during medical school and residency

Our review findings show that self-perceived empathy declines significantly during the course of medical school and residency; in students and residents who choose non-patient-oriented specialties; and, particularly, as a result of increased contact with patients in the clinical phase of training. The fact that a decline in empathy was also found in the more evidence-based longitudinal studies44–46,51–55 underlines the significance of the results of our review.

Reports on students of dental medicine paint a similar picture.58,59 In Sherman and Cramer's59 cross-sectional comparison of 130 U.S. dental students, self-assessed empathy declined significantly in the clinical phase of training.

Schillinger's60 cross-sectional survey findings also merit consideration because “moral judgment competence”61—the capacity to make decisions and judgments which are moral (i.e., based on internal principles) and to act in accordance with such judgments—was measured as a construct which is similar to empathy and is also an important educational value. Schillinger surveyed 1,149 students studying various subjects in Germany, Switzerland, and Brazil. Among the 531 respondents from five German or Swiss universities, 304 were studying medicine and the rest were studying psychology or economics. Whereas the moral judgment competency of the economics and psychology students improved considerably during their studies, that of medical students worsened.60 Students' responses to questions regarding study environment provided relevant explanations for these findings. For example, moral judgment competence was less developed among students dealing with theory; those who were already pragmatically engaged in their field while still in university trained their moral judgment competence (i.e., “learning by practice”62; see Patenaude et al63 for a Canadian study on moral development in medical school).

Some of the studies included in our review reported significant increases in cynicism among medical students.44–46,49 Crandall et al64 also found students' commitment to caring for medically underserved patients to be greater when they entered medical school than at graduation. This result was independent of gender and curriculum type (problem based versus traditional).

Clinical practice phase and distress as key factors of empathy decline

Nearly all of the studies in our review showed that empathy declines significantly on entering the clinical practice phase of training and with increased contact with patients. One possible explanation for this phenomenon may be that encountering morbidity and mortality heightens trainees' feelings of vulnerability. As a result, students and residents may overidentify with patients, causing them to suffer more from distress themselves; they thus become unable to provide rational health care or protect themselves by dehumanizing patients. Consequently, humane treatment, including empathizing with patients, may suffer.65 This explanation corresponds with the results of a recent study of physicians' brains which demonstrated that medical expertise down-regulates the sensory processing elicited by the perception of pain in others.66 This down-regulation occurs at an early stage, which is thought to reflect the automatic emotional sharing component of empathy.67

Another critical experience during initial clinical practice is trainees' increased responsibility for the patient, which is often guided by their unrealistic expectations that medicine can always cure and that there is always “a right thing” to do.68 Students' and residents' expectations may lead them to react to the stress of overwhelming responsibility in undesirable ways—such as detached concern and decreased empathy—as they concentrate only on molecules, organs, reports, and data rather than on the patient.65 Werner and Korsch65 hypothesized that these negative coping reactions are enhanced by the fact that, in most cases, trainees are left alone to deal with the stresses mentioned above as well as their feelings of uncertainty and their fears of academic failure and of inability to master the material. They recommended that faculty recognize these problems and allow students to discuss them in a supportive environment.

Another key factor of empathy decline among medical students and residents is distress (e.g., burnout, reduced quality of life, depression), which is probably caused by the previously mentioned elements of the “hidden curriculum.”32 Some authors interpret distress as a means of survival and self-protection and as a coping mechanism45,46,69—that is, distress may be a strategy and behavior pattern that trainees use to confront and cope with stress factors.70 These findings seem to correspond with those of a recently published 12-year longitudinal study which found that self-assessed empathy in adults did not decline with age but was associated with positive well-being (e.g., life satisfaction) and a positive social interaction profile (e.g., a positive relationship with others).71

The distress hypothesis is also in line with recent neurophysiologic studies on mirror neurons. Mirror neurons display physiological correlations of empathy, which can be activated both during an action or sensation in the body and when the same action is merely observed in another person (e.g., trainees' observation of patients' suffering25). Various investigations have linked mirror neuron function with empathic ability.72–76 According to Bauer's74 hypothesis, existing empathic ability can suffer serious damage through extreme experiences of callousness or inconsiderateness. Furthermore, anxiety, tension, and stress can significantly reduce the signal rate of mirror neurons: “Once pressure, fear and stress are present, everything that depends on the system of mirror neurons stops functioning: the ability to empathize, to understand others and to perceive subtleties.”74(p71) Correspondingly, the negative experiences of a “hidden curriculum”32 may contribute to the decline of empathy in students and residents.

A recent review25 suggested that a broad range of biographical experiences (e.g., upbringing or experiences during adulthood) may also influence the development and promotion of empathy. Together with personality traits,77 these seem to be likely moderator variables78 in medical students' stress experiences and empathy development.32

Further, as reported in the Results, additional factors that contribute to empathy decline are elements of the “formal/informal curricula,”32 namely, shorter hospital stays,43,45 an inappropriate learning environment,43,46,49 and inadequate role models.44–46,49

A recent brainstorming survey79 supports these findings. The medical students and interns surveyed were asked which factors they viewed as affecting empathy during education. They considered “mentoring and clinical experiences that promote professional growth” to be the most important; “negative feelings and attitudes toward patients” and “negative school and work experiences” were less important in their view.

Our model of reasons contributing to a decline in empathy during medical school and residency (Figure 2) provides a graphical summary of the potential determinants of empathy decline that we have discussed above.

F2-24
Figure 2:
Model of reasons contributing to a decline in empathy during medical school and residency. MS indicates medical student; R, resident; QoL, quality of life.

Methodological limitations and reflection

The following methodological limitations of the reviewed studies should be considered when looking at Appendix 1:

  • Only one multicenter study47 was identified. However, that study drew no quasi-experimental curricular comparisons between the different faculties involved.
  • Only three longitudinal studies on medical student empathy were available at the time of the review.44–46
  • Control groups from other health-related areas (e.g., nursing) were missing, as were those from nonmedical, yet relationship-intensive, career areas (e.g., teaching).
  • No explanatory variables apart from stress, gender, and specialty were included in the reviewed studies.32
  • Only two of the studies43,46 conducted nonresponse analyses, so little is known about the possible effects of selection bias.
  • Established methods for increasing response rates were rarely used.80–82
  • Some of the studies on residents had small samples.54,55,57
  • The most reliable and valid measures of self-assessed empathy are the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) and the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy (JSPE), which were the most frequently used measures in the reviewed studies. All other measures used in the reviewed studies lack adequate psychometric evidence.38,39
  • Empathy was only self-assessed in the studies.

In particular, the measurement of empathy via self-assessment requires intense methodological reflection. Both self-assessments and external assessments (by patients, for example) may be used as sources of information. However, both are characterized by measurement errors and/or social desirability bias.83 The fact that empathy-related constructs such as emotional intelligence41,42,49 and moral judgment competence60 have also been shown to decline during the course of university study63 lends support to the validity of the self-assessment tools employed in the reviewed studies. Moreover, self-report measures are the most direct method for assessing subjective and internal cognitive or emotional events involving the respondents' thoughts and feelings, which cannot otherwise be observed or mechanically recorded.84

Two studies have been conducted to test the validity of the JSPE. These compared scores from the JSPE self-assessment measure85,86 with external observations made by senior staff87 or patients.88 Both studies yielded positive correlations between self-assessments and external assessments. Conversely, a recent study by Chen et al89 found that self-assessed empathy measured by the JSPE decreased between the second and third years of medical school, whereas observed empathy, measured as demonstrated empathic behavior during objective structured clinical examinations (OSCEs), increased. However, Chen and colleagues'89 results should be interpreted with caution because empathic behavior represents a desired behavior pattern during an OSCE. Thus, there is a potential for social desirability bias. Given the conflicting results of these studies, methodically structured validation studies with external (e.g., faculty, patient) and self-assessment measures seem both necessary and timely.32

Using self-assessment tools with overlapping constructs such as emotional intelligence,41 interpersonal competence,90 perspective taking,83 and the ability to reflect91 constitutes another possibility for increasing the validity of results. An alternative to self-assessment measures may be the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET),92,93 which tests a specific facet of mind reading—that is, the ability to infer an individual's internal state from observation of his or her subtle affective facial expressions. The RMET seems to be a promising alternative because it is based on the “theory of mind” and is therefore closely related to empathy,92 and it is associated with diverse self-measures of empathy.93

Implications for future research

The fact that our review includes only three European studies investigating empathy in medical students40–42 highlights the need for more research on the topic by European medical schools.

When planning future studies, researchers should place emphasis on theory-based investigations of the reasons for empathy decline.94 To gain initial insight, such studies could use as a basis our model of reasons contributing to a decline in empathy during medical school and residency (Figure 2).

The methodological limitations of the studies we reviewed should also be considered when designing future studies to improve the evidence base on empathy development and its determinants. A combination of theory-based and well-designed studies that incorporate a quasi-experimental comparison of different medical curricula26 and/or an experimental investigation of well-established interventions seems highly appropriate for deriving suitable medical education recommendations. Examples of possible quasi-experimental approaches include drawing comparisons between problem-based41 and traditional curricula or between U.S. and European curricula.

It also seems necessary to investigate whether the observed empathy decline is a “normal” process that health care professionals must go through as they adjust to their surroundings and the demands of their field—a process shaped by their experience with ill, seriously ill, and dying patients.43 Study designs seeking to answer this question would ideally integrate comparison groups from the medical profession (e.g., nurses) as well as nonmedical, yet relationship-intensive, professions (e.g., teachers). Interesting in this context is the example of an older study by Becker and Sands95 which found no decline in empathy among nursing students (see also Fields et al96 and Hojat et al97). Also pertinent to the question of whether empathy decline is a normal development process are the results of a recent meta-analysis conducted by Konrath et al98 among 13,737 American college students. These researchers identified a significant decline in self-perceived empathy from 1979 to 2009, with more recent college students demonstrating approximately 40% less empathy than students 20 to 30 years ago. The decrease in empathy seems to be most prominent in post-2000 samples, suggesting that empathy decline has become a social phenomenon69 in young Americans. A possible explanation for this observation may be that the “information flooding” which started to occur in education after 2000, and the increasing use of communication technologies, have led to a kind of “emotional anesthesia”; that is, our perceptions of our thoughts and emotions may be increasingly suppressed. Future medical education research should therefore investigate whether students are forced to learn too much and whether their use of information technologies is associated with empathy.

On the basis of our findings that the clinical practice phase of training and trainee distress seem to be key determinants of empathy decline, we propose addressing these problems by testing different, sound interventions. Mindfulness-based stress reduction, for example, is a particularly well-researched and highly promising intervention method for reducing stress and enhancing empathy.99–109 Other interventions include self-awareness training,110–112 Balint groups,113,114 and “meaningful experiences and reflective practice discussions,”111 which support students and residents in the clinical practice phase by allowing them to discuss and reflect on issues of vulnerability and responsibility within the context of health care provision.65

A recent, illuminating review by Shapiro69 offers strong support for studying such interventions. She argues that in the absence of appropriate discourse on how to emotionally manage distressing aspects of the human condition, it is likely that trainees will resort to coping mechanisms that result in distance and detachment from patients. Shapiro69 suggests incorporating reflection and self-awareness as consistent elements in the medical school curriculum.62,115

Finally, we believe it is highly important for future research to investigate the relationship between scores on different self-assessment measures of empathy and actual patient health outcomes. As we noted above, current knowledge primarily concerns the positive effects of physician empathy on patient health outcomes and is based on studies using patient-reported measures of physician empathy. However, evidence of the criterion validity and outcome relevance of self-assessment measures is also needed in order to show, for example, that different scores on the JSPE or the different subscales of the IRI positively or negatively affect patient well-being. For example, Krasner116 and colleagues plan to investigate the relationship between general practitioners' scores on the JSPE and their patients' health outcomes.

Limitations of this review

There are limitations to this review. First, the quality of a systematic review is limited by both the study design quality of the available studies and the psychometric quality of the measures used in these studies. Many of the studies included in this review suffer from the methodological difficulties discussed above. Second, not all relevant publications are indexed in the databases we searched. Therefore, we may have overlooked some studies during our electronic and manual searches. Third, the evidence we identified is not strong enough to make causal inferences.

A meta-analysis approach is the most evidence-based and comprehensive method of summarizing empirical findings. Therefore, it would be very useful for future research to calculate effect size estimates to determine the clinical and practical significance of changes in empathy.

Conclusions

Our analysis of the eligible studies, especially those with longitudinal data, suggests that empathy decreases during the course of training, particularly among trainees in the clinical practice phase and in those who have selected patient-remote specialties. Additionally, our review provides evidence that trainee distress is a key determinant of empathy decline, which can be considered a coping mechanism for dealing with various stress factors.

Given the current state of research, it is not possible to fully determine the exact reasons causing the observed empathy decline. However, identifying these would be important for making specific, evidence-based statements as well as developing targeted interventions for education and further training. Prospective and experimental multicenter studies, ideally with control groups, are necessary to give adequate consideration to the variance and interdependence of influential factors. Such a goal can only be reached through structured, interdisciplinary, and evidence-based medical education research,94 which ultimately places the responsibility on high-quality teaching and health care.

Acknowledgments:

The authors wish to thank Gudrun Lamprecht for her tireless support in providing literature. The authors are grateful to Sarah Frances and Fawn Zarkov for their qualified support concerning our use of English. We are also grateful to the anonymous reviewers for their very constructive feedback and valuable advice.

Funding/Support:

The authors would like to thank the Software AG Foundation, the Mahle Foundation, and the Cultura Foundation for their financial support of the “Research Network on Empathy in Medical Education and Patient Care” (Dr. Neumann, Dr. Scheffer, Dr. Tauschel, Dr. Edelhäuser). Dr. Haramati received funding from the Institute for Integrative Health.

Other disclosures:

None.

Ethical approval:

Not applicable.

Previous presentations:

Portions of this study were presented at the International Conference on Communication in Healthcare (ICCH) in Verona, Italy, and at the Association for Medical Education in Europe in Glasgow, United Kingdom, both held in September 2010.

References

1 Simpson M, Buckman R, Stewart M, et al. Doctor–patient communication: The Toronto Consensus Statement. BMJ. 1991;303:1385–1387.
2 DiBlasi Z, Harkness E, Ernst E, Georgiou A, Kleijnen J. Influence of context effects on health outcomes: A systematic review. Lancet. 2001;357:757–762.
3 Stewart MA. Effective physician–patient communication and health outcomes: A review. CMAJ. 1995;152:1423–1433.
4 Stewart M, Brown JB, Donner A, et al. The impact of patient-centered care on outcomes. J Fam Pract. 2000;49:796–804.
5 Griffin SJ, Kinmouth A, Veltman M, Grant J, Stewart M. Effect on health-related outcomes of interventions to alter the interaction between patients and practitioners: A systematic review of trials. Ann Fam Med. 2004;2:595–608.
6 Mercer SW, Reynolds WJ. Empathy and quality of care. Br J Gen Pract. 2002;52(suppl):S9–S13.
7 Squier RW. A model of empathic understanding and adherence to treatment regimens in practitioner–patient relationships. Soc Sci Med. 1990;30:325–329.
8 Maguire P, Faulkner A, Booth K, Elliot C, Hiller V. Helping cancer patients disclose their concerns. Eur J Cancer Care. 1996;32A:78–81.
9 Coulehan J, Platt F, Egner B, et al. “Let me see if I have this right ...”: Words that build empathy. Ann Intern Med. 2001;135:221–227.
10 Beckman HB, Frankel RM. Training practitioners to communicate effectively in cancer care: It is the relationship that counts. Patient Educ Couns. 2003;50:85–89.
11 Neumann M, Wirtz M, Bollschweiler E, Warm M, Wolf J, Pfaff H. Psychometric evaluation of the German version of the “Consultation and Relational Empathy” (CARE) measure at the example of cancer patients [in German]. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol. 2008;58:5–15.
12 Larson EB, Yao Y. Clinical empathy as emotional labor in the patient–physician relationship. JAMA. 2005;293:1100–1106.
13 Halpern J. From Detached Concern to Empathy: Humanizing Medical Practice. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2001.
14 Irving P, Dickson D. Empathy: Towards a conceptual framework for health professionals. Int J Health Care Qual Assur Inc Leadersh Health Serv. 2004;17:212–220.
15 Neumann M, Wirtz M, Bollschweiler E, et al. Determinants and patient-reported long-term outcomes of physician empathy in oncology: A structural equation modelling approach. Patient Educ Couns. 2007;69:63–75.
16 Kim SS, Kaplowitz S, Johnston MV. The effects of physician empathy on patient satisfaction and compliance. Eval Health Prof. 2004;27:237–251.
17 Price S, Mercer SW, MacPherson H. Practitioner empathy, patient enablement and health outcomes: A prospective study of acupuncture patients. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;63:239–245.
18 Roter DL, Stewart M, Putnam S, Lipkin MJ, Stiles W, Inui T. Communication patterns of primary care physicians. JAMA. 1997;277:350–356.
19 Nightingale SD, Yarnold PR, Greenberg MS. Sympathy, empathy, and physician responses in primary care and surgical settings. J Gen Intern Med. 1991;6:420–423.
20 Levinson W, Gorawa-Bhat R, Lamb J. A study of patient cues and physician responses in primary care and surgical settings. JAMA. 2000;284:1021–1027.
21 Howie JG, Heaney DJ, Maxwell MW, Freeman GK, Rai H. Quality at general practice consultations: Cross sectional survey. BMJ. 1999;319:738–743.
22 MacPherson H, Mercer SW, Scullion T, Thomas KJ. Empathy, enablement, and outcome. J Altern Complement Med. 2003;9:869–876.
23 Bikker AP, Mercer SW, Reilly D. A pilot prospective study on the consultation and relational empathy, patient enablement, and health changes over 12 months in patients going to the Glasgow Homoeopathic Hospital. J Altern Complement Med. 2005;11:591–600.
24 Rakel DP, Hoeft TJ, Barrett BP, Chewning BA, Craig BM, Niu M. Practitioner empathy and the duration of the common cold. Fam Med. 2009;41:494–501.
25 Neumann M, Bensing J, Mercer SW, Ernstmann N, Pfaff H. Analyzing the “nature” and “specific effectiveness” of clinician empathy: A theoretical overview and contribution towards a theory-based research agenda. Patient Educ Couns. 2009;74:339–346.
26 Institute of Medicine. Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for the 21st Century. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2001.
27 Little P, Everitt H, Williamson I, et al. Observational study of effect of patient-centredness and positive approach on outcomes of general practice consultations. BMJ. 2001;323:908–911.
28 Mechanic D, Meyer S. Concepts of trust among patients with serious illness. Soc Sci Med. 2000;51:657–668.
29 Coulter A. Patients' views of the good doctor. BMJ. 2002;325:668–669.
30 Klingenberg A, Bahrs O, Szecsenyi J. What do patients want from their GP? First results from a European study [in German]. Z Allg Med. 1996;72:180–186.
31 Wensing M, Jung HP, Olesen F, Grol R. A systematic review of the literature on patient priorities for general practice care. Part 1: Description of the research domain. Soc Sci Med. 1998;47:1573–1588.
32 West CP, Shanafelt TD. The influence of personal and environmental factors on professionalism in medical education. BMC Med Educ. 2007;7:1–9.
33 Association of American Medical Colleges. Report I: Learning objectives for medical student education—Guidelines for medical school. Acad Med. 1999;74:13–18. http://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/Abstract/1999/01001/Learning_objectives_for_medical_student.10.aspx. Accessed April 5, 2011.
34 Frank JR. The CanMEDS 2005 Physician Competency Framework. Better Standards. Better Physicians. Better Care. Ottawa, Ontario, Canada: Royal College of Physicians and Surgeons of Canada; 2005.
35 Working Group Under a Mandate of the Joint Commission of the Swiss Medical Schools. Swiss Catalogue of Learning Objectives for Undergraduate Medical Training. http://sclo.smifk.ch. Accessed April 5, 2011.
36 Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, and The PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PloS Med. July 21, 2009;6:e1000097.
37 Stepien KA, Baernstein A. Educating for empathy. A review. J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21:524–530.
38 Hemmerdinger JM, Stoddort S, Lilford RA. A systematic review of tests of empathy in medicine. BMC Med Educ. 2007;7:1–8.
39 Pedersen R. Empirical research on empathy in medicine—A critical review. Patient Educ Couns. 2009;76:307–322.
40 Kliszcz J, Hebanowski M, Rembowski J. Emotional and cognitive empathy in medical schools. Acad Med. 1998;73:541. http://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/Abstract/1998/05000/Emotional_and_cognitive_empathy_in_medical_schools.25.aspx. Accessed April 5, 2011.
41 Austin EJ, Evans P, Magnus B, O'Hanlon K. A preliminary study of empathy, emotional intelligence and examination performance in MBChB students. Med Educ. 2007;41:684–689.
42 Todres M, Tsimtsiou Z, Stephenson A, Jones R. The emotional intelligence of medical students: An exploratory cross-sectional study. Med Teach. 2010;32:e42–e48.
43 Chen D, Lew R, Hershman W, Orlander J. A cross-sectional measurement of medical student empathy. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22:1434–1438.
44 Hojat M, Vergare MJ, Maxwell K, et al. The devil is in the third year: A longitudinal study of erosion of empathy in medical school. Acad Med. 2009;84:1182–1191. http://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/Fulltext/2009/09000/The_Devil_is_in_the_Third_Year__A_Longitudinal.12.aspx. Accessed April 5, 2011.
45 Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, et al. An empirical study of decline in empathy in medical school. Med Educ. 2004;38:934–941.
46 Newton B, Barber L, Clardy J, Cleveland E, O'Sullivan P. Is there hardening of the heart during medical school? Acad Med. 2008;83:244–249. http://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/Fulltext/2008/03000/Is_There_Hardening_of_the_Heart_During_Medical.6.aspx. Accessed April 5, 2011.
47 Thomas MR, Dyrbye LN, Huntington JL, et al. How do distress and well-being relate to medical student empathy? A multicenter study. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22:177–183.
48 DiLalla LF, Hull SK. Effect of gender, age, and relevant course work on attitudes toward empathy, patient spirituality, and physician wellness. Teach Learn Med. 2004;16:165–170.
49 Stratton TD, Saunders JA, Elam CL. Changes in medical students' emotional intelligence: An exploratory study. Teach Learn Med. 2008;20:279–284.
50 Newton BW, Savidge MA, Barber L, et al. Differences in medical students' empathy. Acad Med. 2000;75:1215. http://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/Fulltext/2000/12000/Differences_in_Medical_Students__Empathy.20.aspx. Accessed April 5, 2011.
51 Bellini LM, Baime M, Shea JA. Variation of mood and empathy during internship. JAMA. 2002;287:3143–3146.
52 Bellini LM, Shea JA. Mood change and empathy decline persist during three years of internal medicine training. Acad Med. 2005;80:164–167. http://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/Fulltext/2005/02000/Mood_Change_and_Empathy_Decline_Persist_during.13.aspx. Accessed April 5, 2011.
53 West C, Huschka M, Novotny P, et al. Association of perceived medical errors with resident distress and empathy. JAMA. 2006;296:1071–1078.
54 West C, Huntington J, Huschka M, et al. A prospective study of the relationship between medical knowledge and professionalism among internal medicine residents. Acad Med. 2007;82:587–592. http://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/Fulltext/2007/06000/A_Prospective_Study_of_the_Relationship_between.11.aspx. Accessed April 5, 2011.
55 Rosen IM, Gimotty PA, Shea JA, Bellini LM. Evolution of sleep quantity, sleep deprivation, mood disturbances, empathy, and burnout among interns. Acad Med. 2006;81:82–85. http://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/Fulltext/2006/01000/Evolution_of_Sleep_Quantity_Sleep_Deprivation.20.aspx. Accessed April 5, 2011.
56 Mangione S, Kane G, Caruso J, Gonnella J, Nasca T, Hojat M. Assessment of empathy in different years of internal medicine training. Med Teach. 2002;24:370–373.
57 Shanafelt TD, West C, Zhao X, et al. Relationship between increased personal well-being and enhanced empathy among internal medicine residents. J Gen Intern Med. 2005;20:559–564.
58 Yarascavitch C, Regehr G, Hodges B, Haas DA. Changes in dental student empathy during training. J Dent Educ. 2009;73:509–517.
59 Sherman JL, Cramer A. Measurement of changes in empathy during dental school. J Dent Educ. 2005;69:338–345.
60 Schillinger M. Learning Environment and Moral Development: How University Education Fosters Moral Judgment Competence in Brazil and Two German-Speaking Countries. Aachen, Germany: Shaker Verlag; 2006.
61 Lind G. Review and Appraisal of the Moral Judgment Test (MJT). http://www.uni-konstanz.de/ag-moral/pdf/Lind-2000_MJT-Review-and-Appraisal.pdf. Accessed April 5, 2011.
62 Branch WT. Supporting the moral development of medical students. J Gen Intern Med. 2000;15:503–509.
63 Patenaude J, Niyonsenga T, Fafard D. Changes in students' moral development during medical school: A cohort study. CMAJ. 2003;7:840–844.
64 Crandall S, Reboussin BA, Michielutte R, Anthony J, Naughton M. Medical students' attitudes toward underserved patients: A longitudinal comparison of problem-based and traditional medical curricula. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2007;12:71–86.
65 Werner ER, Korsch BM. The vulnerability of the medical student: Posthumous presentation of L. L. Stephens' ideas. Pediatrics. 1976;57:321–328.
66 Decety J, Yang C, Cheng Y. Physicians down-regulate their pain empathy response: An event-related brain potential study. Neuroimage. 2010;50:1676–1682.
67 Cheng Y, Lin C, Liu H, et al. Expertise modulates the perception of pain in others. Curr Biol. 2007;17:1708–1713.
68 Schrauth M, Kowalski A, Weyrich P, Begenau J, Zipfel S, Nikendei C. Self-image, the real physician and the ideal physician: A comparison of medical students from 1981 and 2006 [in German]. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol. 2009;59:446–453.
69 Shapiro J. Walking a mile in their patients' shoes: Empathy and othering in medical students' education. Philos Ethics Humanit Med. 2008;3:10.
70 Aronson E, Wilson TD, Akert RM. Social Psychology [in German]. Munich, Germany: Pearson Studium; 2004.
71 Gruehn D, Rebucal K, Diehl M, Lumley M, Labouvie-Vief G. Empathy across the adult lifespan: Longitudinal and experience-sampling findings. Emotion. 2008;8:753–765.
72 Gallese V. The roots of empathy: The shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology. 2003;36:171–180.
73 Hojat M. Empathy in Patient Care. Antecedents, Development, Measurement and Outcomes. Berlin, Germany: Springer; 2007.
74 Bauer J. Why I Feel What You Feel. Communication and the Mystery of Mirror Neurons [in German]. Hamburg, Germany: Hoffmann und Campe; 2005.
75 Decety J, Jackson PL. The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2004;3:71–100.
76 Decety J, Jackson P. A social-neuroscience perspective on empathy. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2006;15:54–58.
77 McManus IC, Keeling A, Paice E. Stress, burnout and doctors' attitudes to work are determined by personality and learning style: A twelve-year longitudinal study of UK medical graduates. BMC Med. 2004;2:29.
78 Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51:1173–1182.
79 Winseman J, Malik A, Morison J, Balkoski V. Students' views on factors affecting empathy in medical education. Acad Psychiatry. 2009;33:484–491.
80 Dillman DA, Smyth JD, Christian LM. Internet, Mail, and Mixed-Mode Surveys: The Tailored Design Method. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 2008.
81 Edwards PJ, Roberts IG, Clarke MJ, et al. Methods to increase response rates to postal questionnaires. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2009;1:DOI: 10.1002/14651858.MR000008.pub3.
82 McColl E, Jacoby A, Thomas L, et al. Design and use of questionnaires: A review of best practice applicable to surveys of health service staff and patients. Health Technol Assess. 2001;5:31.
83 Steins G. A diagnostic analysis of empathy and perspective taking: A validation of the relationship between both constructs and implications for measurement [in German]. Diagnostica. 1998;44:117–129.
84 Nezu AM, Nezu CM. Evidence-Based Outcome Research: A Practical Guide to Conducting Randomized Controlled Trials for Psychosocial Interventions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2008.
85 Hojat M, Gonnella J, Nasca T, Mangione S, Vergare M, Magee M. Physician empathy: Definition, components, measurement, and relationship to gender and specialty. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1563–1569.
86 Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, Magee M. Physician empathy in medical education and practice: Experience with the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy. Semin Integr Med. 2003;1:25–41.
87 Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca T, Gonnella J. Empathy scores in medical school and ratings of empathic behavior in residency training 3 years later. J Soc Psychol. 2005;145:663–672.
88 Glaser KM, Markham FW, Adler HM, McManus RP, Hojat M. Relationships between scores on the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy, patient perceptions of physician empathy and humanistic approaches to patient care: A validity study. Med Sci Monit. 2007;13:CR291–CR294.
89 Chen DC, Pahilan ME, Orlander J. Comparing a self-administered measure of empathy with observed behavior among medical students. J Gen Intern Med. 2010;25:200–202.
90 Kanning UP. Development and validation of a German-language version of the Interpersonal Competence Questionnaire (ICQ). Eur J Psychol Assess. 2006;22:43–51.
91 Aukes LC, Geertsma J, Cohen-Schotanus J, Zwierstra RP, Slaets J. The development of a scale to measure personal reflection in medical practice and education. Med Teach. 2007;29:177–182.
92 Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42:241–251.
93 Lawrence EJ, Shaw P, Baker D, Baron-Cohen S, David A. Measuring empathy: Reliability and validity of the Empathy Quotient. Psychol Med. 2004;34:911–924.
94 Eva K, Lingard L. What's next? A guiding question for educators engaged in educational research. Med Educ. 2008;42:752–754.
95 Becker H, Sands D. The relationship of empathy to clinical experience among male and female nursing students. J Nurs Educ. 1988;27:198–203.
96 Fields SK, Hojat M, Gonnella J, Mangione S, Kane G, Magee M. Comparisons of nurses and physicians on an operational measure of empathy. Eval Health Prof. 2004;27:80–94.
97 Hojat M, Fields SK, Gonnella JS. Empathy: An NP/MD comparison. Nurse Pract. 2003;28:45–47.
98 Konrath S, O'Brien E, Hsing C. Changes in dispositional empathy in American college students over time: A meta-analysis. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2011;15:180–198.
99 Shapiro SL, Schwartz G, Bonner G. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on medical and premedical students. J Behav Med. 1998;21:581–599.
100 Shapiro SL, Astin JA, Bishop SR, Cordova M. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for health care professionals: Results from a randomized trial. Int J Stress Manag. 2005;13:164–176.
101 Epstein RM. Mindful practice. JAMA. 1999;282:833–839.
102 Connelly JE. Narrative possibilities: Using mindfulness in clinical practice. Perspect Biol Med. 2005;48:84–94.
103 Rosenzweig S, Reibel DK, Greeson JM, Brainard GC. Mindfulness-based stress reduction lowers psychological distress in medical students. Teach Learn Med. 2003;15:88–92.
104 Lovas JG, Lovas DA, Lovas PM. Mindfulness and professionalism in medicine. J Dent Educ. 2008;72:998–1009.
105 Beddoe A, Murphy S. Does mindfulness decrease stress and foster empathy among nursing students? J Nurs Educ. 2004;43:305–312.
106 Krasner M, Epstein R, Beckman H, et al. Association of an educational program in mindful communication with burnout, empathy, and attitudes among primary care physicians. JAMA. 2009;302:1284–1293.
107 Hassed C, de Lisle S, Sullivan G, Pier C. Enhancing the health of medical students' outcomes of an integrated mindfulness and lifestyle program. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2009;14:387–398.
108 Grepmair L, Mitterlehner F, Loew T, Bachler E, Rother W, Nickel M. Promoting mindfulness in psychotherapists in training influences the treatment results of their patients: A randomized, double-blind, controlled study. Psychother Psychosom. 2007;76:332–338.
109 Irving JA, Dobkin PL, Park J. Cultivating mindfulness in health care professionals: A review of empirical studies of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR). Complement Ther Clin Pract. 2009;15:61–66.
110 Young B, Salmon P. Core assumptions and research opportunities in clinical communication. Patient Educ Couns. 2005;58:225–234.
111 Novack DH, Suchman AL, Clark W, Epstein R. Calibrating the physician. Personal awareness and effective patient care. JAMA. 1997;278:502–509.
112 Novack DH, Epstein RM, Paulsen RH. Toward creating physician–healers: Fostering medical students' self-awareness, personal growth, and well-being. Acad Med. 1999;74:516–520. http://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/Abstract/1999/05000/Toward_creating_physician_healers__fostering.17.aspx. Accessed April 5, 2011.
113 Balint M. The Doctor, His Patient and the Disease [in German]. Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta; 2001.
114 Brock C, Salinsky JV. Empathy: An essential skill for understanding the physician–patient relationship in clinical practice. Fam Med. 1993;25:245–248.
115 Spencer J. Decline in empathy in medical education: How can we stop the rot? Med Educ. 2004;38:916–920.
116 Personal communication with M. Krasner, September 2008.
TU2A-24
Appendix 1:
Descriptions of 18 Studies Investigating Trainees' Self-Assessed Empathy During Medical School (11 Studies) and Residency (7 Studies), Published From January 1990 through January 2010, Organized by Study Group
TU2B-24
Appendix 1:
, continued
TU2C-24
Appendix 1:
, continued
TU2D-24
Appendix 1:
, continued
TU2E-24
Appendix 1:
, continued
© 2011 Association of American Medical Colleges