Skip Navigation LinksHome > Summer 2013 - Volume 25 - Issue 2 > Intervention in the First Weeks of Life for Infants Born Lat...
Pediatric Physical Therapy:
doi: 10.1097/PEP.0b013e3182888b86
Case Report

Intervention in the First Weeks of Life for Infants Born Late Preterm: A Case Series

Dusing, Stacey C. PT, PhD; Lobo, Michele A. PT, PhD; Lee, Hui-Min PT, PhD; Galloway, James Cole PT, PhD

Free Access
Article Outline
Collapse Box

Author Information

Motor Development Laboratory (Dr Dusing), Department of Physical Therapy, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia; Department of Physical Therapy (Dr Lobo), Biomechanics and Movement Sciences Program (Drs Lee and Galloway), and Infant Behavior Laboratory (Dr Galloway), Department of Physical Therapy, University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware.

Correspondence: Stacey C. Dusing, PT, PhD, Motor Development Laboratory, Department of Physical Therapy, Virginia Commonwealth University, PO Box 980224, Richmond, VA 23298 (scdusing@vcu.edu).

Grant Support: This project was funded in part by the National Institutes of Health (1K12HD055931-01 and NIH NICHD R01 HD051748-01A1)

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Collapse Box

Abstract

Infants born late preterm (34–36 weeks of gestation) account for 350 000 US births per year, are at risk for developmental delays, and are rarely included in intervention studies.

Purpose: To describe a novel parent-delivered movement intervention program for very young infants and outcomes following intervention and to evaluate the feasibility of using a comprehensive set of outcome measures.

Summary of Key Points: Two infants born late preterm received intervention from 0.5 to 2.0 months of adjusted age. Development, postural control, reaching, and object exploration assessments were completed at 3 time points. The intervention was well tolerated by the family. Improvements in developmental outcomes, postural control, and object exploration are presented.

Statement of Conclusion: Very early movement experience provided daily by parents may improve development. In combination, norm-referenced and behavioral measures appear sensitive to changes in infant behaviors.

Back to Top | Article Outline

INTRODUCTION

Infants born late preterm (34-36 weeks of gestation) account for more than 70% of preterm births or 350 000 infants per year in the United States.1 Historically, these infants have received little attention as they were not typically admitted to the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) and have been considered to be at low risk for developmental delays.2 However, recent research suggests that infants born late preterm are at higher risk of developmental delays and poor school performance than infants born full term.35 For example, more than 30% of children in preschool or kindergarten who were born late preterm have delayed academic performance.4 Infants born late preterm are 3 times more likely to have cerebral palsy, 1.5 times more likely to have intellectual disabilities, and are diagnosed with developmental coordination disorder more often than infants born full term.5,6

Although infants born very preterm, at less than 30 weeks of gestation, frequently receive early intervention services under the Individuals with Disabilities Act, infants born late preterm often do not qualify for these services early in life, delaying potentially beneficial intervention.79 For example, infants born very preterm in Virginia automatically qualify for early intervention, which may include developmental monitoring, parent education, and/or therapy services.9 Infants who do not meet automatic eligibility criteria, including infants born late preterm, must demonstrate a 25% developmental delay. This level of delay is difficult to document without the use of special assessment tools designed for very young infants, which are not generally used for early intervention eligibility determination.10 By 12 months of age, however, infants born late preterm who required neonatal intensive care require early intervention at a rate similar to infants born very preterm.11 Delayed early intervention means that the infants born late preterm miss a year of intervention during a period of high brain plasticity and when parent education can shape positive parent-infant interaction. Given the prevalence of late preterm birth and the risk of developmental disabilities in this population, developmental monitoring, parent education, and/or direct intervention may be warranted.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Early Movement Experience

The way infants move and interact with objects during early development shapes their understanding of the world. Exploration generates cognition-perception-action cycles, which infants use to expand their movement repertoire, generate learning opportunities, and advance general development.12,13 For example, the ability to maintain a sitting position while reaching and grasping a toy facilitates an infant's learning about spatial relationships, properties of objects, and effect of interacting with the object.14,15 Infants with delayed motor development, reduced postural control, or atypical movement patterns may not generate the level of daily exploration needed to drive their cognitive, perceptual, and motor development.

In this case series, we provided at-risk infants born late preterm with an intervention grounded in “action perception theory.”16 The purpose of this intervention was to provide each infant experiences to encourage postural control, self-directed movement, and a varied movement repertoire starting at 0.5 months of adjusted age, well before developmental interventions are typically provided to infants born late preterm who are not admitted to the NICU. Providing very young infants with experiences that are slightly advanced for their age can improve head control, reaching, object exploration, and stepping.12,1721 For example, infants born full term whose parents provided them with advanced movement experiences for 20 minutes per day for 3 weeks had an accelerated onset of reaching and grasping in the short term as well as advanced problem solving after the intervention ended.17 Separate studies involving similar postural and movement programs started at 1 to 2 months of age advanced a range of behaviors from head control to crawling and walking.19,22 Similar programs have been effective for infants born at less than 33 weeks of gestation to advance reaching and object exploration.21,23 Whereas evidence supports the potential for early experiences to advance motor development in both infants born full term and those born preterm, there is little evidence on the effectiveness of these programs in infants born late preterm. This case series starts to address this gap by describing a parent-delivered movement program used with 2 infants born late preterm in the first months of life. As in previous work, the use of a parent-delivered intervention maximizes the frequency of intervention, limits the cost to provide the activities, and may enhance translation into early intervention practice.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Measurement Challenges in Early Infancy

The assessment of emerging motor behaviors in the first months of life is equally important and challenging.10 Standardized, norm-referenced assessment tools such as the Test of Infant Motor Performance (TIMP)24 and Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley)25 provide general information on the development of the motor system and allow for comparison with a normative sample. However, standardized measures lack detailed information on specific functional behaviors in infancy such as the duration and/or frequency of keeping the head in midline, bringing the hands to the mouth, reaching, manipulating objects, and mouthing objects.10 Therefore, additional measurement tools are needed to describe the development of early functional motor behaviors. Measures of motor behavior, including the frequency or duration of head in midline, toy contact, bimanual grasp, hand to mouth, and toy to mouth, are sensitive to change and can be used to describe the emergence of behaviors, interactions with the environment, and the effects of intervention.12,17,26,27 Research is needed to evaluate the feasibility of using a combination of standardized, norm-reference developmental measures and measures of functional motor behaviors to evaluate responses to intervention in the first months of life. This case series uses norm-referenced and skill-specific behavioral measures to describe changes in general developmental and specific functional motor behaviors during the first months of life.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Purpose

The purpose of this case series is to (1) describe the use of a parent-delivered movement program for infants born late preterm starting in the first months of life, (2) describe the developmental and motor behavior outcomes following participation in the movement program for the 2 infants in this case series, and (3) determine the feasibility of using a comprehensive set of outcome measures for young infants in future studies.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Description of the Case Series Participants

A set of twins born late preterm participated in the movement program and are the focus of this case series. The infants were born at 35 weeks and 2 days of gestation, were white, and had 2 older siblings, and both parents had a college education. The female twin (infant A) was born at 5.02 pounds, was hospitalized for 4 days in the well baby nursery, and had hip dysplasia requiring the use of a Pavlik harness until 3 months of age. The male twin (infant B) was born at 6.02 pounds, was hospitalized for 7 days in the well baby nursery, and had mild reflux requiring medical management in the newborn period according to his parents. All ages throughout the article are adjusted for prematurity (age from the expected date of full-term delivery).

Back to Top | Article Outline
Assessment Protocol

A comprehensive battery of assessments was completed to quantify motor development, postural control, reaching, and object exploration, using a combination of norm-referenced standardized assessments and sensitive measures of behaviors in the first 6 months of life. Assessment visits were completed at 0.5 months (baseline), 2 months (immediately postintervention), and 4 and 6 months of age (2 and 4 months after termination of intervention) (Table 1). The mother was encouraged to feed, change diapers, or console as needed so that each infant was in a positive or neutral behavioral state during the assessments. One additional developmental follow-up assessment was completed at 12 months of age. All assessments were videotaped for behavioral coding and scoring.

Table 1
Table 1
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Developmental Assessments

Developmental assessments were included as a general measure of infant development over time and for a comparison with normative data. The TIMP was completed at baseline (0.5 months), immediately postintervention (2 months), and at 4 months of age. The TIMP is a reliable and valid norm-references assessment of movement and posture for infants from 34 weeks of gestation to 4 months of age adjusted for prematurity.24,2830 The TIMP Z scores [TIMP Z score = (TIMP raw score − mean for age group)/SD of the age group] were used to compare infants' development with the TIMP's normative data and to evaluate change over time in response to intervention. No developmental assessment was completed at 6 months of age. The Motor Scales of the Bayley were completed at 12 months of age.25 The Bayley is a reliable, valid, and norm-referenced standardized assessment of gross and fine motor, cognitive, receptive, and expressive language abilities for infants and toddlers.10,25 Scaled scores for the gross and fine motor subtests are presented as a comparison with the Bayley's normative sample. Both the TIMP and the Bayley were scored from videotaped assessments by reliable examiners who were blinded to the infant's gestational age at birth and to their intervention status.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Postural Control

The magnitude and complexity of center of pressure (COP) displacement at the base of support are measures of postural control that change during the emergence of motor behaviors, differ between infants with and without motor impairments, and change in response to intervention.3134 Postural sway was assessed while the infant was placed in a supine position without a visual stimulus for 5 minutes using a Conformat pressure sensitive mat (Tekscan, South Boston, Massachusetts) sampling at 5 Hz.34 Behavioral data were used to identify continuous COP time series of 500 data samples or 100 seconds in length in which the infant was in the supine position and alert, and no one was touching the infant. As described in detail in our previous work, the magnitude of the variability and complexity of postural control were quantified using the root-mean-square (RMS) displacement and approximate entropy (ApEN) in the caudal cephalic (cc) and medial lateral (mL) directions (RMScc, RMSml, ApENcc, and ApENml).3436 Magnitude of the variability in postural control (RMS) is a reflection of the standard deviation around a central mean value or the amount of variability without regard for the pattern of the variability. Our previous research suggests that the magnitude of the variability in postural control does not change systematically during development of early motor behaviors.33,36 Complexity in the variability of COP movement (ApEN) represents the repeatability of the postural control strategy or the pattern of variability within a time series. Repetitive COP movement or the use of a limited number of postural control patterns is signified by low complexity or ApEN values. Readers who are not familiar with these measures are encouraged to review the study by Dusing and Harbourne35 for an expanded explanation of these terms and clinical examples. In infants developing typically, postural control complexity is higher during the initial development of a skill when the infant is using multiple postural control strategies through trial and error. Postural control complexity decreases as an infant learns to consistently perform a skill using the most efficient postural control strategies for that infant.35,36 Infants with CP and those born preterm have reduced postural control complexity during development.34,37 We used these measures of postural control complexity to evaluate changes in postural control following intervention. As described later, we also assessed the infants' ability to maintain the head in midline in the supine position as an additional measure of postural control.19

Back to Top | Article Outline
Reaching and Object Exploration

Reaching was assessed in the supine and sitting positions at each visit. In the supine position, infants participated in 5 trials, 60 seconds in length, with a toy presented at 80% of arm's length. The toy was presented directly over the torso at the nipple line. All toys used in the reaching tasks were commercially available small infant rattles. For all sitting tasks, the infant was positioned in an infant high chair with the side removed and a soft strap to stabilize the trunk while allowing for free arm movements. During seated reaching, the infant participated in 6 trials of 30 seconds with a toy presented at 80% of arm's length. In the sitting object exploration condition, infants participated in 6 trials holding onto an object that was placed in the infant's left or right hand in random order for a maximum of 30 seconds. A total of 3 objects were presented once to each hand. The objects were all commercially available infant toys such as a ring, infant keys, and a soft rattle. If the infant dropped the object, it was replaced up to 3 times within the 30-second trial.

Video recordings of the supine COP, supine and seated reaching, and seated object exploration assessments were used to code behaviors including looking, head in midline, toy contact, and toy to mouth. Behavioral coding was completed using the MacShapa v1.1.2a (Department of Mechanical and Industrial Engineering, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, Urbana-Champaign, Illinois) coding program and trained coders maintained greater than 85% agreement for each variable coded with the formula: Agree/(Agree + Disagree) × 100.38 Three pairs of coders were responsible for coding the data included in this case series. Coding pairs were trained to code specific variables and 20% of the visits were coded twice to ensure a high level of reliability on each variable and between the 2 coders in the pair. The percentage of the assessment duration that the infant's head was in midline was calculated during the supine assessments of COP (no toy) and during the supine reaching assessment (toy). Head in midline was coded anytime the trained coder observed that the infant's head was within 30° of midline. Reaching was quantified by the percentage of time during the reaching assessment that each hand was in contact with a toy for each position separately. The sum of the percentage of time each hand was in contact with the toy was calculated. Mouthing object exploration in sitting was calculated as the percentage of the time while holding the object that the object was touching the infant's lips or mouth. Looking was calculated as the percentage of the time the object was presented during the seated reaching trials in which the infant was looking at the object.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Description of Intervention

The movement program used in this case series was designed to encourage parents to provide additional experiences to enhance postural and motor control at an age when most infants born late preterm receive no intervention. On the basis of our previous research with infants born full term and preterm as well as action-perception theory, we hypothesized that additional postural control and movement experience would advance motor development, postural control, reaching, and object exploration.17,19,21

The movement program encouraged parents to interact with their infants for a minimum of 20 minutes per day from 0.5 months to 2.0 months of age to “strengthen baby's neck, shoulders, stomach, legs, and arms using a variety of different activities.” Following the baseline assessment, the mother of the twin infants born late preterm was provided with an activity booklet, which provided a verbal and pictorial description of activities, including suggested progressions of each activity. The 7 activities included prone play, sitting with upper trunk support, head control practice, midline hand-to-hand and hand-to-foot play, assisted kicking, and visualizing/tracking toys (Figure 1). The activities were reviewed with the mother and demonstrated. She was encouraged to support the infant's attempts with these activities and to provide the least assistance possible to complete the activities. The activity booklet provided a suggestion for the amount of time to spend on each activity ranging from 2 to 4 minutes for a total of 20 minutes per day, which could be completed in a single 20-minute session or multiple shorter sessions. The mother completed a daily log documenting how much time she spent on each activity with each infant per day. The twin infants born late preterm completed at least 20 minutes of the activities on 90% and 80% of days during the 1.5 months of the intervention. The intervention each infant received differed slightly. Infant A did not receive experience in kicking and hand-to-feet activities because of the Pavlik harness. Infant B completed all 7 items of the movement program for a total of 20 minutes. To make up for these activities, Infant A spent more time on the other 5 items to complete 20 minutes of activity. Although the intervention each infant received differed because of the use of the Pavlik harness worn by infant A, both infants received additional experiences controlling posture in a variety of positions, practicing self-directed movements, and observing toys. These differences in intervention may have influenced each infant's outcomes. Therefore, each infant's outcomes are described separately and in detail in this case series.

Fig 1
Fig 1
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Description of Outcomes
Developmental Assessments

The outcomes of the norm-referenced standardized tests are presented using Z scores or standard scores as a comparison with the normative sample for each assessment. Both of the infants in this case series improved their raw score on the TIMP at each study visit (Figure 2A). Both infants demonstrated an improvement in TIMP Z scores, meaning that they were less delayed or closer to the average range at the postintervention visit (Figure 2B). Infant A's score on the TIMP was more than 1 standard deviation below the mean at the baseline visit, indicating that she had delayed movement and postural control compared with the normative sample on the TIMP.29 Her TIMP Z score improved slightly at the immediate postintervention visit and she made a very large improvement, into the average range, between the immediately postintervention visit at 2 months and the follow-up at 4 months of age. Infant B's TIMP Z score was in the average range at all assessments but improved over time.

Fig 2
Fig 2
Image Tools

Developmental follow-up assessments with the Bayley25 at 12 months of adjusted age demonstrated that infant A had gross and fine motor skills in the average range. Infant B had average fine motor skills, but his gross motor skills were between 1 and 2 standard deviation below the mean, suggesting a mild gross motor delay (Figure 3).

Fig 3
Fig 3
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Postural Control

The development of postural control in the infants born late preterm in this case series differed from each other with infant A being similar to infants born full term, based on the data derived from previous research on infants born full term using the same COP protocol (Figures 4A–C).36 Infant A demonstrated baseline complexity values and a trajectory similar to the data of infants born full term. Infant B had a similar developmental trajectory, with a more repetitive or less-complex postural control at all time points. During the intervention, his postural control complexity remained stable; however, the decrease in complexity following the end of the intervention was quite large compared with that of infant A and the full-term comparison data.36 Midline head control in the supine position improved for both infants born late preterm over time with each maintaining his or her head in midline most of the time by 4 months of age. By 2 months of age, each infant maintained his or her head in midline while in a supine position, for a longer duration when a toy was presented than without a visual stimulus (Figure 5).

Fig 4
Fig 4
Image Tools
Fig 5
Fig 5
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Reaching and Object Exploration

Infants born late preterm both demonstrated improving reaching and object exploration, which may have been affected by the movement program. The infants did not demonstrate reaching in either the sitting or supine position or during object exploration in sitting on a regular basis before 4 months of age. Both infants made contact with the toy by 6 months of age from both the supine and sitting positions (Figures 6A and 6B). Both infants born late preterm were in contact with the toy for a greater percentage of the assessment period when in the sitting position, compared with the supine position. Infant B contacted the toy more in the supine position than infant A at 4 and 6 months of age (Figure 6A). By 6 months of age, both infants born late preterm had a hand in contact with the toy during most of the sitting assessment (Figure 6B). Both infants born late preterm started to explore objects while sitting with trunk support in a chair by bringing the toy to his or her mouth at 4 months with an increase in this behavior by 6 months of age (Figure 6C). From 4 to 6 months of age both infants increased the percentage of the time they looked at a toy suspended in front of them while sitting in a chair (Figure 6D).

Fig 6
Fig 6
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Feasibility

The movement program and the comprehensive assessment paradigm used in this case series were feasible and well tolerated by the infants and family in the first months of life. The infants born late preterm included in this case series completed the intervention at least 80% of the recommended frequency, although both were receiving the intervention and other children were in the home. Initiating the intervention at 0.5 months of adjusted age was feasible and helped this mother establish a routine for developmental play early in the infants' lives. Infants completed each assessment visit in 1 to 3 sessions. In combination, the 10 minutes of supine (COP assessment and supine reaching) and 10 minutes of sitting (reaching and object exploration) plus the TIMP were tolerated well. Equipment setup time in the family home, transition time between activities, and caregiving breaks resulted in most sessions lasting about 1 hour per infant. In general, the visits at 2 months of age or less required multiple sessions as parents had more difficulty estimating when their infants would be awake and playful and infants fatigued faster than at older ages. Scoring the assessments by video enabled the research team to save time during the assessment visit and allowed for scoring by individuals blind to infants' medical and intervention history.

Back to Top | Article Outline

DISCUSSION

Infants born late preterm, between 34 and 36 weeks of gestation, can participate in a parent-delivered movement program as early as 0.5 months of age (adjusted for prematurity) when parents are establishing caregiving routines. The movement program presented in this 2 infant case series is novel as it engaged the parent in activities to support posture and movement experiences starting weeks or months before these infants typically would receive early intervention services under the Individuals with Disabilities Act. The mother who completed the movement program activities with her twins was eager to support the twin's development and performed the activities almost daily during the 1.5 months of intervention. She reported that the activities became a part of her routine play with the twins and that it was feasible to include them in caregiving. Previous research suggests that parents can modify their interaction with their infants following education in the first months after hospital discharge.39 We speculate that parents who participate in this or similar movement programs will change their interactions with their infants to include components of the movement program in their daily activities during and after the intervention periods, increasing the ability for these programs to enhance infant development.40

Back to Top | Article Outline
A Movement Program May Improve Development

Both infants in this case series demonstrated improved motor development and postural control during the study period. Both demonstrated increasing TIMP Z scores during and after the movement program, suggesting that the infants' motor development improved at a faster rate than the normative sample of the TIMP. Similar to infants born full term, the infants' postural control complexity remained fairly constant during the movement program. Infant A's experience for more than 3 months in a Pavlik harness may have altered her development of postural control as well as her postural control assessment, as she wore the Pavlik Harness for these assessments. However, both infants demonstrated a slight decrease in postural control complexity over time, which signifies the infants' abilities to select efficient postural control strategies.33,35,36 The very rapid decline in postural control complexity observed in infant B after the completion of the movement program suggests that his postural control may have become excessively repetitive. This type of repetitive postural control may be an early indicator of developmental delay or motor impairments, which were identified in this infant at 12 months of age.35 In combination, the postural control findings from this case series are generally consistent with the research documenting differences in postural control complexity between infants at risk of developmental disabilities and the ability to change postural control complexity through intervention.31,32,34

Improvement in postural control supports the development of reaching and object exploration.41 Although the movement program did not directly provide the infants with additional reaching or object exploration experiences, the 2 infants in this case series improved their reaching, especially in sitting. Postural control and reaching in the supine position are linked to coordination and motor impairments at 6 years of age.41,42 Although our measures of postural control and reaching differ from this research, the evidence suggests that reduced variability of postural control at 4 months (“still” posture42) and limited reaching at 6 months are associated with future motor dysfunction. Both infants in this case series demonstrated increased successful reaching for objects in the supine and sitting positions by 6 months of age. We speculate that the improvements observed in reaching in the case series infants may be, in part, related to improved postural control.

Both of the infants who participated in the movement program in this case series demonstrated increasing exploration of toys by looking at suspended toys and bringing toys to the mouth. While infants developing typically bring hands and toys to the mouth in early infancy, additional experience with objects has been shown to advance mouthing behaviors.18 While our intervention did not specifically focus on object experience, we speculate that increased reaching and postural control facilitated more opportunities to interact with objects.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Early Intervention May Be Warranted for Infants Born Late Preterm

Intervention programs have been shown to improve development for infants born full term and preterm, yet, aside from the intervention received as a result of this study, these infants born late preterm did not receive any early intervention services and their mothers were not taught by anyone in the medical community about play activities in the first 12 months of life. Although neither infant was cared for in the NICU, the infants were still at risk for developmental delays based on their late preterm birth status. Although further research is needed, this case series, in combination with other literature, provides preliminary evidence that parents may be taught to provide experiences that help their infants “catch up” from mild motor delays.17,21,23,38 This case series cannot confirm that the movement program improved development in these infants born late preterm, however, it does support the need and feasibility for infants born late preterm to participate in developmental monitoring and parent education in the first year of life. If the benefit of very early intervention is documented through larger studies, it could have significant policy implications for timing and eligibility for early intervention services and supporting parents in providing early movement experiences for infants born late preterm.

Back to Top | Article Outline
A Comprehensive Assessment Plan Is Needed to Document Changes

The comprehensive assessment included in this study was feasible to complete for infants as young as 0.5 months of adjusted age. It required a significant commitment from the families and researchers to complete all portions of the assessment, using multiple sessions if necessary. Although the TIMP and the Bayley provide a view of an infant's motor development, they are not sensitive to changes in postural control, spontaneous movement, reaching, or object exploration. Likewise, the measures of postural control, reaching, and object exploration do not provide information on the infant's broader motor development. However, the combination of the measures in this case series provided a sensitive measure of change. The TIMP, head control, and postural sway measures changed from 0.5 to 2 months of age when the infants were participating in an intervention to advance postural control and were provided movement experience. The TIMP, reaching, and object manipulation measures all demonstrated change in the months following intervention, suggesting that any one of these measures alone would be insufficient to show the broad scope of changes during the first 6 months of life. To evaluate the efficacy of interventions on development, researchers must quantify an infant's ability to generate movement, reach, and interact with objects all of which support motor, cognitive, and language development.17,43 This case series supports the feasibility of this protocol to evaluate developmental change in young infants. Additional measures of parent-infant play may be warranted to document how the intervention influences parent/infant play routines during and beyond the intervention period.

Back to Top | Article Outline

CONCLUSION

The findings from this 2 infant case series, while preliminary in nature, support the feasibility of providing a parent-delivered movement program to infants born late preterm starting at 0.5 months of adjusted age. The positive changes in motor development, postural control, reaching, and object exploration seen in the twins born late preterm suggest the need for a larger study to evaluate the efficacy of a movement program to enhance development. Future studies should include a combination of measures to ensure sensitivity to change and to document the relationships between the intervention and parent-child interaction, postural control, skill acquisition, object exploration, and motor and cognitive development.

Back to Top | Article Outline
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank the families who generously allowed us to share in the development of their infants. We also thank the research staff in the Infant Motor Behavior Lab at the University of Delaware and the Motor Development Lab at Virginia Commonwealth University for their support in this study.

Back to Top | Article Outline

REFERENCES

1. Martin J, Hamilton B, Ventura S, et al. Births: final data for 2009. Natl Vital Stat Rep. 2011;60(1).

2. Ishiguro A, Namai Y, Ito YM. Managing “healthy” late preterm infants. Pediatr Int. 2009;51:720–725.

3. Morse SB, Zheng H, Tang Y, Roth J. Early school-age outcomes of late preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2009;123:e622–e629.

4. McGowan JE, Alderdice FA, Holmes VA, Johnston L. Early childhood development of late-preterm infants: a systematic review. Pediatrics. 2011;127:1111–1124.

5. Petrini JR, Dias T, McCormick MC, Massolo ML, Green NS, Escobar GJ. Increased risk of adverse neurological development for late preterm infants. J Pediatr. 2009;154:169–176.

6. Moster D, Lie RT, Markestad T. Long-term medical and social consequences of preterm birth. N Engl J Med. 2008;359:262–273.

7. McCormick MC, Litt JS, Smith VC, Zupancic JAF. Prematurity: an overview and public health implications. Ann Rev Public Health. 2011;32:367–379.

8. Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004. Public Law 108-446. STAT 2647. 108th Congress. 2004. http://www.copyright.gov/legislation/pl108-446.pdf.

9. Infant & Toddler Connection of Virginia. Practice manual. Chapter 5 Infants and Toddler Connection Web site: http://www.infantva.org/documents/PracManCh5.pdf. Updated 8/7/11. Accessed February 23, 2012.

10. Spittle AJ, Doyle LW, Boyd RN. A systematic review of the clinimetric properties of neuromotor assessments for preterm infants during the first year of life. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2008;50:254–266.

11. Kalia JL, Visintainer P, Brumberg HL, Pici M, Kase J. Comparison of enrollment in interventional therapies between late-preterm and very preterm infants at 12 months' corrected age. Pediatrics. 2009;123:804–809.

12. Libertus K, Needham A. Teach to reach: the effects of active vs. passive reaching experiences on action and perception. Vision Res. 2010;50:2750–2757.

13. Kanakogi Y, Itakura S. Developmental correspondence between action prediction and motor ability in early infancy. Nat Commun. 2011;2:341.

14. Soska KC, Adolph KE, Johnson SP. Systems in development: motor skill acquisition facilitates three-dimensional object completion. Dev Psychol. 2010;46:129–138.

15. Adolph KE, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Ishak S, Karasik LB, Lobo SA. Locomotor experience and use of social information are posture specific. Dev Psychol. 2008;44:1705–1714.

16. Bertenthal BI, Longo MR. Motor knowledge and action understanding: A developmental perspective. In: Klatzky RL, MacWhinney B, Behrman M, eds. Embodiment, Ego-space, and Action. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2008:323–368.

17. Lobo MA, Galloway JC. Postural and object-oriented experiences advance early reaching, object exploration, and means-end behavior. Child Dev. 2008;79:1869–1890.

18. Needham A, Barrett T, Peterman K. A pick-me-up for infants' exploratory skills: Early simulated experiences reaching for objects using “sticky mittens” enhances young infants' object exploration skills. Infant Behav Dev. 2002;25:279–295

19. Lee HM, Galloway JC. Early intensive postural and movement training advances head control in very young. Infants Phys Ther. 2012;83:1290–1302.

20. Zelazo N, Zelazo P, Cohen K, Zelazo P. Specificity of practice effects on elementary neuromotor patterns. Dev Psychol. 1993;29:686–691.

21. Heathcock JC, Lobo M, Galloway JC. Movement training advances the emergence of reaching in infants born at less than 33 weeks of gestational age: a randomized clinical trial. Phys Ther. 2008;88:310–322.

22. Lobo MA, Galloway JC. Enhanced handling and positioning in early infancy advances development throughout the first year. Child Dev. 2012;83:1290–1302.

23. Heathcock JC, Galloway JC. Exploring objects with feet advances movement in infants born preterm: a randomized controlled trial. Phys Ther. 2009;89:1027–1038.

24. Campbell S. The Test of Infant Motor Performance: Test User's Manual Version 2.0. Chicago, IL: Infant Motor Performance Sclares, LLC; 2005.

25. Bayley N. Balyey Scales of Infant and Toddler Development. 3rd ed. San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp; 2006.

26. Corbetta D, Snapp-Childs W. Seeing and touching: the role of sensory-motor experience on the development of infant reaching. Infant Behav Dev. 2009;32:44–58.

27. Rochat P. Self-perception and action in infancy. Exp Brain Res. 1998;123:102–109.

28. Campbell SK. Test-retest reliability of the test of infant motor performance. Pediatr Phys Ther. 1999;11:60–66.

29. Campbell SK, Kolobe TH, Wright BD, Linacre JM. Validity of the Test of Infant Motor Performance for prediction of 6-, 9- and 12-month scores on the Alberta Infant Motor Scale. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2002;44:263–272.

30. Campbell SK, Levy P, Zawacki L, Liao PJ. Population-based age standards for interpreting results on the test of motor infant performance. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2006;18:119–125.

31. Harbourne R, Willett S, Kyvelidou A. A comparison of interventions for children with cerebral palsy to improve sitting postural control. Phys Ther. 2010;90:1881–1898.

32. Deffeyes JE, Harbourne RT, Kyvelidou A, Stuberg WA, Stergiou N. Nonlinear analysis of sitting postural sway indicates developmental delay in infants. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon). 2009;24:564–570.

33. Harbourne RT, Stergiou N. Nonlinear analysis of the development of sitting postural control. Dev Psychobiol. 2003;42:368–377.

34. Dusing S, Kyvelidou A, Mercer VS, Stergiou N. Infants born preterm exhibit different patterns of center of pressure movement than infants born at term. Phys Ther. 2009;89:1354–1362.

35. Dusing SC, Harbourne RT. Variability in postural control during infancy: implications for development, assessment, and intervention. Phys Ther. 2010;90:1838–1849.

36. Dusing SC, Thacker LR, Stergiou N, Galloway JC. Early complexity supports development of motor behaviors in the first months of life [published online ahead of print May 9, 2012]. Dev Psychobiol. doi: 10.1002/dev.21045.

37. Harbourne R, Deffeyes JE, DeJong SL, Stuberg WA, Kyvelidou A. Nonlinear variables can assist in identifying postural control deficits in infants. J Sport Exerc Psychol (Suppl). 2007;29:S9.

38. Lobo MA, Galloway JC, Savelsbergh GJ. General and task-related experiences affect early object interaction. Child Dev. 2004;75:1268–1281.

39. Kang R, Barnard K, Hammond M, et al. Preterm infant follow-up project: a multi-site field experiment of hospital and home intervention programs for mothers and preterm infants. Public Health Nurs. 1995;12:171–180.

40. Bakewell-Sachs S, Gennaro S. Parenting the post-NICU premature infant. MCN Am J Matern Child Nurs. 2004;29:398–403.

41. Fallang B, Saugstad OD, Hadders-Algra M. Postural adjustments in preterm infants at 4 and 6 months post-term during voluntary reaching in supine position. Pediatr Res. 2003;54:826–833.

42. Fallang B, Oien I, Hellem E, Saugstad OD, Hadders-Algra M. Quality of reaching and postural control in young preterm infants is related to neuromotor outcome at 6 years. Pediatr Res. 2005;58:347–353.

43. Lobo MA, Harbourne RT, Dusing SC, McCoy SW. Grounding early intervention: physical therapy cannot just be about motor skills anymore. Phys Ther. 2013;93:94–103.

arm/physiology; biomechanics; developmental disabilities; early intervention; family caregivers; female; male; motor skills; physical therapy/methods; postural balance; preterm infants; weight-bearing/physiology

© 2013 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Inc.

Login

Article Tools

Images

Share

Follow PED-PT on Twitter

Search for Similar Articles
You may search for similar articles that contain these same keywords or you may modify the keyword list to augment your search.