Defining the Nature of Motion Perception Deficits in Glaucoma Using Simple and Complex Motion Stimuli

KARWATSKY, PETER OD, MSc; BERTONE, ARMANDO PhD; OVERBURY, OLGA PhD; FAUBERT, JOCELYN PhD

doi: 10.1097/01.opx.0000225107.38719.0d
Original Article

Purpose. The purpose of this study is to determine the nature of motion perception deficits in primary open-angle glaucoma by measuring the sensitivity of simple (luminance-defined) and complex (texture-defined) motion, the latter requiring supplementary neural processing to be resolved. These findings will help address the possible extent of the cortical damage in glaucoma that has been recently demonstrated by anatomic and physiological studies. They also serve the purpose of establishing which motion paradigms would be most appropriate for assessing glaucoma-related functional loss.

Methods. Direction-identification thresholds for first-order and second-order motion were measured for 26 patients with primary open-angle glaucoma (for both phakic and pseudophakic) and 18 nonglaucomatous observers.

Results. The glaucomatous observers showed significantly increased motion thresholds for both first- and second-order motion conditions when compared with nonglaucomatous observers. However, the relative increase in threshold for first-order motion did not differ significantly from that of second-order motion.

Conclusions. These findings imply that there is no measurable higher-level cortical function damage caused by the glaucomatous process because no greater loss in second-order motion was observed. Based on the results, we suggest that motion paradigms used to assess functional loss in primary open-angle glaucoma should consist of simple, first-order type stimuli to minimize potential confounds such as those introduced by both the normal and pathologic aging process on complex motion processing (i.e., perimetry using complex motion stimuli).

Visual Psychophyiscs and Perception Laboratory, école d'Optométrie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Quebec, Canada (PK, AB, JF) and Sir Mortimer B. Davis Jewish General Hospital, Montréal, Quebec, Canada (OO)

Received December 7, 2005; accepted April 3, 2005.

Article Outline

Recent studies have demonstrated that glaucoma may cause changes in the Lateral Geniculate Nucleus and cortical regions of the brain as demonstrated by cell shrinkage and loss.1,2 There is also evidence from metabolic and neurochemical studies that the central nervous system is affected in glaucoma.3–5 Given these findings, it becomes important to determine whether both low- and higher-level perceptual losses are induced by glaucoma-related damage.

A number of psychophysical studies have focused on the development of early diagnostic methods that assess visual field loss in open-angle glaucoma.6 Such defects associated with glaucoma are presumed to originate from ganglion cell destruction in the retina.7–9 Specifically, such studies indicate that M-ganglion cells, which represent approximately 8% to 10% of the retinal ganglion cells in the retina,10 are lost preferentially during the glaucomatous process, particularly in the early stages of the disease7,8 (see also other references for an alternate view11–13). An attenuation of both the pattern electroretinogram14 and low-contrast visual-evoked potentials,15 both of which have been argued to be stimuli that bias the magnocellular pathway, have been shown in glaucoma and attributed to M-cell loss. Decreased diffuse flicker sensitivity16,17 and temporal modulation visual fields18–20 lowered peripheral displacement thresholds,21 and defects in spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity22 in glaucomatous eyes are further indicators of magnocellular pathway degeneration. Because M-ganglion cells are believed to be involved in the processing of temporal information and to be the retinal origin regarding motion information processing,23,24 the evaluation of motion perception in glaucoma has been used as an investigative predictor of the disease. In effect, several studies have associated motion perception impairments with advancing glaucoma. Direction-discrimination thresholds for global motion cues have been shown to be elevated in patients with primary open-angle glaucoma (POAG) and for persons with ocular hypertension (OHT),25 and motion perimetry has revealed glaucomatous defects that were not observable using conventional perimetry.26,27 Loss of sensitivity to motion-defined form (patients with POAG and those with OHTs28), elevation of perimetric motion thresholds (patients with POAG29), and loss in motion coherence perimetry26 have all been demonstrated. These and other findings suggest motion perception is impaired in the early stage of the disease (see 30 for review).

Although different motion testing paradigms show glaucoma-related loss, they do not solicit identical processing requirements. For instance, global motion (or motion coherence) necessarily requires complex spatiotemporal integration within extrastriate cortical areas,31,32 whereas simple stimuli (luminance-defined or even flicker thresholds) solicit higher-level motion processing to a much lesser extent. If we are to assume that glaucoma is primarily an optic nerve disease, then why use complex motion stimuli to assess retinal–neural function? Using motion paradigms that necessitate complex perceptual processing may reduce the diagnostic specificity of the measures because performance on such tests may be contaminated by higher-level neural changes such as those presented during normal aging33 in addition to glaucoma-related retinal dysfunction. Complex motion processing has been shown to be affected during the normal34 and pathologic aging process35–39 to a greater extent than for simple motion types.34,38,39 On the other hand, using complex motion paradigms might be of interest if we assume that there is higher-level cortical damage in glaucoma. The recent evidence showing that cortical cells primarily associated with the nonglaucomatous eye can also show anatomic changes implies that higher-order mechanisms may also be affected in this disease.2 If this were the case, using complex motion paradigms as opposed to simpler techniques might increase the sensitivity of the measure. Given the demographic of most patients with glaucoma, the selection of proper motion stimuli has important practical and interpretive implications.

The main purpose of this study was to determine whether motion perception deficits in POAG are the consequence of only a low-level retinal V1 system or includes additional higher-level cortical processing; in essence, to investigate the nature of motion perception deficits found in POAG. This was done by measuring direction-identification thresholds to first-order (or luminance-defined) and second-order (texture-defined) motion stimuli. These stimuli were chosen because contemporary motion models differentiate first- and second-order motion processing by the level at which they are first processed along the motion pathway.40–44 Luminance-defined first-order motion processing begins in the retina and is followed by standard motion analysis selective mechanisms operating in the primary visual cortex that process the local luminance variations in the retinal image.45–48 For this reason, first-order information is considered to be a “simple” type of visual information. On the other hand, additional nonlinear processing of the second-order motion signals is required (i.e., signal rectification or response squaring) before standard motion analysis can resolve the direction of this class of motion.41,44 Such additional processing has been attributed to the cortex,49–52 suggesting that second-order motion perception necessarily involves more cortical processing. For this reason, second-order visual information is considered a more “complex” type of visual information because it requires the implication of larger neural circuitry to be perceived. These two classes of visual information were used because a large body of psychophysical evidence has demonstrated that they are initially processed by neural mechanisms varying in complexity40,41,44,53 and are sensitive to subtle neural dysfunctioning of visual information processing.33,34

The techniques used in this article have been shown to be extremely sensitive to subtle cortical alterations. For example, normal aging has been shown to affect second-order thresholds to a significantly greater extent than first-order thresholds, and this, in nonpathologic (healthy) aging where changes, if they exist, must be much more subtle than disease processes.34 This paradigm has also been very useful to demonstrate subtle perceptual effects in autism54,55 and fragile X syndrome.56 Therefore, this method has been proven to be very effective to tease out whether there are condition-specific subtle changes in cortical processes requiring larger neural networks.33

A decrease in the perception of both first- and second-order drifting stimuli is expected in POAG given the primary mechanism for glaucoma, that being retinal ganglion cell loss. If the glaucomatous process governing open-angle glaucoma is in any way related to higher neural dysfunctions, then damage to higher cortical areas is expected, leading to a larger threshold elevation for the perception of second order. Alternatively, if there is no relative difference between the magnitude of loss for first- and second-order stimuli, we must conclude that the glaucoma-related neural damage remains at a low level and that future assessment of glaucoma-related motion sensitivity loss should use testing paradigms that limit cortical processing (simple motion).

Back to Top | Article Outline

METHODS

Observers

A total of 44 observers participated in this study (mean age, 70.5 ± 5.37 years; range, 61–78 years), all of which were recruited from among the patients of the Ophthalmology Department of the Sir Mortimer B. Davis Montreal Jewish General Hospital. Observers were considered to have glaucoma after a diagnosis or confirmation of diagnosis by the examining ophthalmologist at the patient's most recent visit. All observers with glaucoma were undergoing treatment. The observers were placed into one of four experimental groups: POAG/phakic (n = 15; mean age, 70.20 ± 5.85 years), POAG/pseudophakic (n = 11; mean age, 71.09 ± 6.09 years), nonglaucomatous/phakic (n = 10; mean age, 70.00 ± 4.29 years), or nonglaucomatous/pseudophakic (n = 8; mean age, 71.25 ± 4.86 years). Both phakic and pseudophakic observers with and without glaucoma were tested to evaluate whether lenticular senescence would contribute to loss in motion sensitivity irrespective of glaucoma. Control participants were defined as having no ocular pathologies and taking no long-term ocular medication. Mild sclerosis of the lens was permitted as long as the subject met the minimal visual acuity requirements. Minimal corrected visual acuity was 20/40 and all of the observers had normal color vision as measured by Farnsworth D-15 color plates. The pupil sizes of all observers were measured. Testing was done monocularly with the observers' best correction in place. All participants were naïve to the purpose of the study and none were experienced psychophysical observers. In accordance with the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki, informed consent was obtained from each participant before testing began.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Apparatus and Display

The motion stimuli were presented using a Power Macintosh 7300/200 computer and presented on a standard Seiko Instruments CM 1445 screen (refresh rate of 67 Hz, GEAC Canada Ltd, Ontario, Canada) that was gamma-corrected using a color look-up table. The screen resolution was 640 × 480 pixels and its mean luminance was 38 cd/m2 in which Lmin and Lmax were 3.6 cd/m2 and 80 cd/m2, respectively. Stimuli generation and presentation were controlled using the VPixx graphics program (www.vpixx.com). Luminance and chromaticity measurements were made using a Minolta Chromameter (CS-100, Folio Instruments Inc., Ontario, Canada).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Stimuli

All motion stimuli were presented to observers within a circular region at the center of the display subtending a visual angle of 10° in diameter when viewed from a distance of 67 cm. The stimuli consisted of first- and second-order translating patterns. The first-order motion stimuli were luminance-modulated noise patterns produced by adding static grey-scale noise to modulating sine waves, in this case a vertical sinusoid. The noise consisted of dots (1 pixel × 1 pixel measuring approximately 2.3 arc min) with individual luminances randomly assigned as a function of sin (x) in which (x) ranged from 0 to 2Π. The contrast (luminance modulation depth) of the first-order patterns was manipulated by varying the amplitude of the modulating sine wave. The amplitude of the luminance modulation for the first-order patterns could be varied from 0.0 to 0.5 defined as:

where Lmax and Lmin refer to the average highest and lowest local luminances in the pattern. The first-order luminance modulation levels used in the constant stimuli presentations were 0.04, 0.02, 0.01, 0.005, 0.0025, and 0.00,125. These levels were chosen based on previous studies using similar stimuli.54

The second-order stimuli were texture-modulated noise patterns produced by multiplying rather than summing the same modulating sine waves to the grey-scale noise. The depth of the texture modulation (contrast modulation depth) was manipulated by varying the amplitude of the modulating sine wave. The amplitude of the sinusoid therefore defined the contrast of the pattern and could be varied within a range of 0.0 and 1.0 defined as:

where Cmax and Cmin are the maximum and minimum local contrasts in the pattern (Fig. 1). Second-order contrast modulation levels used during the constant stimuli procedures were 1.0, 0.333, 0.143, 0.111, and 0.059. The spatial and drift frequency was 1 cycle per degree (cpd) and 2 Hz, respectively.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Procedure

Participants were tested individually in a dimly lit room and viewed the monitor monocularly using the eye with best visual acuity. The procedure was explained to them and a practice session followed to familiarize the participants with the procedure and confirm that they were able to complete the task. During the actual testing session, each participant was presented with trials consisting of first- and second-order stimuli moving either to the left or to the right for 750 ms. The method of constant stimuli was used to measure direction-identification thresholds for each motion condition that included six levels of luminance modulation (ranging from 0.04 to 0.00,125) and five levels of contrast modulation (ranging from 0.5 to 0.03,125). For each motion condition, the stimuli were presented 10 times in either direction (left and right) at each level of modulation for a total of 20 trials at each level of modulation. Participants were asked to identify the direction of motion by pressing either of two buttons on a keypad (two alternative forced choices). All stimuli were presented foveally at the same spatial location on the monitor. Participants were instructed to keep their gaze on a central fixation point throughout experimentation. Finally, Weibull57 functions were fitted to the data to calculate direction-identification thresholds at 75% correct level of performance.

Back to Top | Article Outline

RESULTS

Because first- and second-order absolute motion thresholds are defined by different attributes (i.e., luminance and texture modulation, respectively), comparisons of the mean differences between the two motion classes is not informative nor is the direct comparison of first- and second-order thresholds for the same patient. For this reason, first- and second-order motion-identification thresholds were analyzed separately.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Effect of Lenticular Senescence on Motion Sensitivity

As already mentioned, both phakic and pseudophakic participants with and without glaucoma were evaluated to determine whether lenticular senescence would contribute to loss in motion sensitivity independently of the presence of glaucoma. Figure 2A shows the mean direction-identification thresholds for phakic (white bars) and pseudophakic (black bars) observers with and without glaucoma for the first-order motion condition. A two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) (group × lenticular senescence) revealed no significant difference in first-order direction-identification thresholds between phakic and pseudophakic observers in either the glaucoma (F1,24 = 0.428, p > 0.05) or the control group (F1,16 = 0.454, p > 0.05). A separate two-way ANOVA was used to compare the direction-identification thresholds between phakic and pseudophakic observers for the second-order motion condition. As was found for the first-order motion type, phakic and pseudophakic observers showed no significant difference in direction-identification thresholds for either glaucomatous (F1,24 = 0.432, p > 0.05) or control participants (F1,16 = 0.834, p > 0.05) for the second-order motion condition (Fig. 2B). Because the direction-identification thresholds were not affected by lenticular senescence for either glaucomatous and control observers for both motion conditions, the thresholds were collapsed across lens condition and averaged. These results demonstrate that, unlike minimum motion color thresholds,58,59 lenticular senescence does not affect motion sensitivity in our conditions for either luminance- or texture-defined motion information for observers with and without glaucoma.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Effect of Glaucoma on First- and Second-Order Motion Sensitivity

After the data were collapsed across lens condition, 26 observers fell into the glaucoma category (mean age, 70.56 ± 5.85 years; range, 61–78 years) and 18 nonglaucomatous observers served as age-matched control subjects (mean age, 70.56 ± 4.46 years; range, 63–78 years). Figure 3 illustrates the direction-identification thresholds for first-order (left panel) and second-order (right panel) motion conditions for both glaucomatous and control participants (phakic and pseudophakic patients included in each experimental group). Statistical analysis of the grouped data showed that direction-identification thresholds were significantly higher for the observers with glaucoma when compared with normal observers for both the first (F1,42 = 19.114, p = < 0.05) and second-order (F1,42 = 12.787, p < 0.05) motion conditions. More importantly, the magnitude of the threshold elevation was similar for both motion conditions; 1.56 times greater for the first-order class (from 0.005 to 0.0078) and 1.40 times greater for the second-order class (from 0.1787 to 0.2506).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Effect of Retinal Illumination on First- and Second-Order Motion Sensitivity

Optical factors such as reduced retinal illumination caused by decreased pupil size contribute to age-related decline in contrast sensitivity.60 To ensure that differences found in motion sensitivity were not a consequence of reduced retinal illumination, the pupil size of each observer comprising the different experimental groups was measured and compared. On average, each experimental group had comparable mean pupil sizes: POAG/phakic group (n = 15; mean pupil size, 3.20 ± 0.68 mm), POAG/pseudophakic group (n = 11; mean pupil size, 3.00 ± 0.63 mm), nonglaucomatous/phakic group (n = 10; mean pupil size, 3.30 ± 0.67 mm), and nonglaucomatous/pseudophakic group (n = 8; mean pupil size, 3.00 ± 0.53 mm). Additional statistical analysis demonstrated that pupil size (4 mm, n = 12; 3 mm, n = 26; and 2 mm, n = 6) did not significantly affect direction-identification thresholds for either the first-order (F2,41 = 0.971, p > 0.05) or second-order (F2,41 = 1.824, p > 0.05) motion, irrespective to which group the patient belonged to.

Back to Top | Article Outline

DISCUSSION

Simple and Complex Motion Perception in Glaucoma

The purpose of this study was to investigate the nature of motion perception impairment in POAG by measuring the sensitivity of motion stimuli varying with respect with the amount of neural processing involved in resolving their motion direction. Our general findings are consistent with previous studies demonstrating decreased motion perception sensitivity for patients with advancing glaucoma.21,26,28,29,61 In the present study, direction-identification thresholds for both simple (first-order) and complex (second-order) motion types were significantly elevated for patients with glaucoma. One implication of this finding is that the decreased sensitivity to luminance-defined first-order motion stimulus, which is processed in part by the retina, is in accordance with M-cell pathway damage in glaucoma.15,17,21 Increased first-order motion thresholds suggest ganglion cell loss and damage within the retina itself. Our results also demonstrated a significant increase for second-order motion thresholds for the observers with glaucoma. However, this difference in threshold did not exceed the difference in threshold measured between the glaucoma and normal observers for the first-order motion condition. The finding that the complex-motion perception (that is mediated by a more complex neural network) was not selectively affected by glaucoma implies that there is no measurable higher-order cortical damage caused by the glaucomatous process, at least at early corticovisual areas (areas V2 and V3) where second-order motion is believed to solicit more processing than first-order mechanisms.40,50,52

Back to Top | Article Outline

Aging and Its Relation to Motion Perception in Glaucoma

Aging affects both peripheral and central aspects of visual information processing.33,62,63 For example, gradual decline of temporal modulation visual fields,64 form-from-motion processing,65 and temporal processing speed66 have been shown to be associated with an increasing age, as has global motion perception.35 Previous studies using motion stimuli similar to those used in the present study have demonstrated that aging (i.e., in the absence of eye disease) affects the perception of complex motion stimuli to a greater extent.34 Together, these findings suggest less efficient neural processing of complex visual motion information in the elderly. The present finding that the relative first- and second-order motion loss is similar in our glaucoma subjects suggests that these findings are not the consequence of normal aging, which would have resulted in a greater loss for the more complex, second-order stimuli. At least for the luminance and texture-based motion stimuli used in the present study, both lenticular senescence and retinal illumination had no significant effect on motion direction discrimination. Although the mean pupil size of our experimental groups was very similar, no correlation was found between pupil size and motion sensitivity when analyzed irrespective of group inclusion. Similarly, lenticular senescence did not affect sensitivity to the motion stimuli, whether they were defined by spatiotemporal variations in luminance (first-order) or texture (second-order). Based on these findings, we suggest that the decreased motion sensitivity demonstrated between the control and glaucoma groups were not confounded by either lenticular senescence and/or retinal illumination.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Clinical Application of Findings

The interpretation of results from studies using existing psychophysical tests used to detect early stages of glaucoma has been questioned, particularly on their ability to selectivity isolate either M or P system functioning.67,68 The present study does not attempt to do so. Rather, by using two different motion stimuli differentiated only by the neural processing involved in resolving their motion direction (i.e., same spatial and temporal characteristics), an assessment of M pathway functioning at two different levels was possible, the second-order motion condition implicating increased neural processing. The finding that glaucomatous individuals were equally less sensitive to first-order and second-order motion stimuli has important clinical considerations regarding the type of stimuli to be used in glaucoma studies. Although both simple and complex motion analysis is initially based on the efficient spatiotemporal functioning of retinal ganglion cells, complex motion analysis (i.e., second-order motion, global motion) usually involves neurointegrative processing beyond the primary visual cortex (i.e., V2/V3 in the case of second-order motion and the medial temporal [MT] area in the case of global motion perception). Therefore, motion testing using complex motion stimuli like random dot or global motion25–27,29 is dependent on both simple and complex neural analysis. If this is the case, there is an increased probability that elevated random dot/global motion may be the result of neural dysfunction, if present, in addition to any ganglion loss caused by glaucoma. As already mentioned at the beginning of this article, an example of such a scenario would be a glaucomatous person with dementia (i.e., Alzheimer disease). Complex motion perception has been shown to correlate with age34 and significantly elevated for persons with dementia.35–39 Given the similar demographics shared by persons with dementia and presenting with glaucoma, the sensitivity of global motion may be confounded by neural dysfunction resulting in, at the least, a less specific diagnostic method for assessing POAG.

Back to Top | Article Outline

CONCLUSIONS

Because our findings point to glaucoma as not involving higher-order motion sensitive cortical mechanisms, motion testing using simple stimuli that are processed at the eye and in the primary visual cortex (i.e., simple motion) may be an advantageous alternative with respect to its specificity for the early detection of the glaucomatous process.

Back to Top | Article Outline

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was supported by a Canadian Optometric Education Trust Fund to PK, a CIHR graduate scholarship to AB, and CIHR grant to JF.

Jocelyn Faubert

Visual Psychophysics and Perception Laboratory

École d'Optométrie, Université de Montréal

C.P. 6128, succursale Centre-Ville

Montréal, H3C 3J7

Québec, Canada

e-mail: jocelyn.faubert@umontreal.ca

Back to Top | Article Outline

REFERENCES

1. Gupta N, Yucel YH. Brain changes in glaucoma. Eur J Ophthalmol 2003;13 (suppl 3):S32–5.
2. Yucel YH, Zhang Q, Weinreb RN, Kaufman PL, Gupta N. Effects of retinal ganglion cell loss on magno-, parvo-, koniocellular pathways in the lateral geniculate nucleus and visual cortex in glaucoma. Prog Retin Eye Res 2003;22:465–81.
3. Crawford ML, Harwerth RS, Smith EL 3rd, Mills S, Ewing B. Experimental glaucoma in primates: changes in cytochrome oxidase blobs in V1 cortex. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2001;42:358–64.
4. Crawford ML, Harwerth RS, Smith EL 3rd, Shen F, Carter-Dawson L. Glaucoma in primates: cytochrome oxidase reactivity in parvo- and magnocellular pathways. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2000;41:1791–802.
5. Vickers JC, Hof PR, Schumer RA, Wang RF, Podos SM, Morrison JH. Magnocellular and parvocellular visual pathways are both affected in a macaque monkey model of glaucoma. Aust N Z J Ophthalmol 1997;25:239–43.
6. Stewart WC, Chauhan BC. Newer visual function tests in the evaluation of glaucoma. Surv Ophthalmol 1995;40:119–35.
7. Glovinsky Y, Quigley HA, Dunkelberger GR. Retinal ganglion cell loss is size dependent in experimental glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1991;32:484–91.
8. Quigley HA, Dunkelberger GR, Green WR. Chronic human glaucoma causing selectively greater loss of large optic nerve fibers. Ophthalmology 1988;95:357–63.
9. Quigley HA, Dunkelberger GR, Green WR. Retinal ganglion cell atrophy correlated with automated perimetry in human eyes with glaucoma. Am J Ophthalmol 1989;107:453–64.
10. Livingstone M, Hubel D. Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science 1988;240: 740–9.
11. Morgan JE, Uchida H, Caprioli J. Retinal ganglion cell death in experimental glaucoma. Br J Ophthalmol 2000;84:303–10.
12. Morgan JE. Retinal ganglion cell shrinkage in glaucoma. J Glaucoma 2002;11:365–70.
13. Yucel YH, Zhang Q, Gupta N, Kaufman PL, Weinreb RN. Loss of neurons in magnocellular and parvocellular layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus in glaucoma. Arch Ophthalmol 2000;118:378–84.
14. Bielik M, Zwas F, Shin DH, Tsai CS. PERG and spectral sensitivity in ocular hypertensive and chronic open-angle glaucoma patients. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 1991;229:401–5.
15. Klistorner AI, Graham SL. Early magnocellular loss in glaucoma demonstrated using the pseudorandomly stimulated flash visual evoked potential. J Glaucoma 1999;8:140–8.
16. Tyler CW. Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1981;20:204–12.
17. Velten IM, Korth M, Horn FK, Budde WM. Temporal contrast sensitivity with peripheral and central stimulation in glaucoma diagnosis. Br J Ophthalmol 1999;83:199–205.
18. Faubert J, Balazsi GA, Brussell EM, Overbury O. Multi-flash campimetry and other psychophysical tests in glaucoma. Doc Ophthalmol Proc Ser 1987;49:425–32.
19. Faubert J, Balazsi AG, Muermans M, Brussell EM, Kasner OP. Multi-flash campimetry and optic nerve structure in early chronic open angle glaucoma. In: Heiji A, ed. Perimetry Update 1988/89: Proceedings of the VIIIth International Perimetric Society Meeting; Vancouver, Canada; May 9–12, 1988. Amsterdam: Kugler Publications; 1999:349–58.
20. Casson EJ, Johnson CA, Nelson-Quigg JM. Temporal modulation perimetry: the effects of aging and eccentricity on sensitivity in normals. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1993;34:3096–102.
21. Westcott MC, Fitzke FW, Crabb DP, Hitchings RA. Characteristics of frequency-of-seeing curves for a motion stimulus in glaucoma eyes, glaucoma suspect eyes, and normal eyes. Vision Res 1999;39:631–9.
22. Neima D, LeBlanc R, Regan D. Visual field defects in ocular hypertension and glaucoma. Arch Ophthalmol 1984;102:1042–5.
23. Merigan WH. P and M pathway specialization in the macaque. In: Valberg A, Lee BB, eds. Pigments to Perception: Advances in Understanding Visual Processes. New York: Plenum Press; 1991:117–25.
24. Merigan WH, Katz LM, Maunsell JH. The effects of parvocellular lateral geniculate lesions on the acuity and contrast sensitivity of macaque monkeys. J Neurosci 1991;11:994–1001.
25. Silverman SE, Trick GL, Hart WM Jr. Motion perception is abnormal in primary open-angle glaucoma and ocular hypertension. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1990;31:722–9.
26. Joffe KM, Raymond JE, Chrichton A. Motion coherence perimetry in glaucoma and suspected glaucoma. Vision Res 1997;37:955–64.
27. Wall M, Jennisch CS. Random dot motion stimuli are more sensitive than light stimuli for detection of visual field loss in ocular hypertension patients. Optom Vis Sci 1999;76:550–7.
28. Giaschi DE, Trope GE, Kothe AC, Hong XH. Loss of sensitivity to motion-defined form in patients with primary open-angle glaucoma and ocular hypertension. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 1996;13:707–15.
29. Bosworth CF, Sample PA, Weinreb RN. Perimetric motion thresholds are elevated in glaucoma suspects and glaucoma patients. Vision Res 1997;37:1989–97.
30. Shabana N, Cornilleau Peres V, Carkeet A, Chew PT. Motion perception in glaucoma patients: a review. Surv Ophthalmol 2003;48:92–106.
31. Movshon JA, Newsome WT. Visual response properties of striate cortical neurons projecting to area MT in macaque monkeys. J Neurosci 1996;16:7733–41.
32. Watamaniuk SN, Sekuler R, Williams DW. Direction perception in complex dynamic displays: the integration of direction information. Vision Res 1989;29:47–59.
33. Faubert J. Visual perception and aging. Can J Exp Psychol 2002;56:164–76.
34. Habak C, Faubert J. Larger effect of aging on the perception of higher-order stimuli. Vision Res 2000;40:943–50.
35. Trick GL, Silverman SE. Visual sensitivity to motion: age-related changes and deficits in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neurology 1991;41:1437–40.
36. Silverman SE, Tran B, Zimmerman KM, Feldon SE. Dissociation between the detection and perception of motion in Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1994;44:1814–8.
37. Gilmore GC, Wenk HE, Naylor LA, Koss E. Motion perception and Alzheimer's disease. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1994;49:52–7.
38. O'Brien HL, Tetewsky SJ, Avery LM, Cushman LA, Makous W, Duffy CJ. Visual mechanisms of spatial disorientation in Alzheimer's disease. Cereb Cortex 2001;11:1083–92.
39. Mapstone M, Steffenella TM, Duffy CJ. A visuospatial variant of mild cognitive impairment: getting lost between aging and AD. Neurology 2003;60:802–8.
40. Chubb C, Sperling G. Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. J Opt Soc Am (A) 1988;5:1986–2007.
41. Wilson HR, Ferrera P, Yo C. A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception. Vis Neurosci 1992;9:79–97.
42. Nishida S, Ledgeway T, Edwards M. Dual multiple-scale processing for motion in the human visual system. Vision Res 1997;37:2685–98.
43. Bertone A, Faubert J. How is complex second-order motion processed? Vision Res 2003;43:2591–601.
44. Allard R, Faubert J. Same calculation efficiency but different internal noise for luminance- and contrast-modulated stimuli detection. J Vision 2006;6:322–34. Available at: http://journalofvision.org/6/4/3/. Accessed April 3, 2006.
45. van Santen JP, Sperling G. Temporal covariance model of human motion perception. J Opt Soc Am (A) 1984;1:451–73.
46. Adelson EH, Bergen JR. Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. J Opt Soc Am (A) 1985;2:284–99.
47. van Santen JP, Sperling G. Elaborated Reichardt detectors. J Opt Soc Am A 1985;2:300–21.
48. Watson AB, Ahumada AJ Jr. Model of human visual-motion sensing. J Opt Soc Am A 1985;2:322–41.
49. Baker CL Jr. Central neural mechanisms for detecting second-order motion. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1999;9:461–6.
50. Baker CL Jr, Mareschal I. Processing of second-order stimuli in the visual cortex. Prog Brain Res 2001;134:171–91.
51. Dumoulin SO, Baker CL Jr, Hess RF, Evans AC. Cortical specialization for processing first- and second-order motion. Cereb Cortex 2003;13:1375–85.
52. Smith AT, Greenlee MW, Singh KD, Kraemer FM, Hennig J. The processing of first- and second-order motion in human visual cortex assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). J Neurosci 1998;18:3816–30.
53. Cavanagh P, Mather G. Motion: the long and short of it. Spat Vis 1989;4:103–29.
54. Bertone A, Mottron L, Jelenic P, Faubert J. Motion perception in autism: a ‘complex’ issue. J Cogn Neurosci 2003;15:218–25.
55. Bertone A, Mottron L, Jelenic P, Faubert J. Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain 2005;128:2430–41.
56. Kogan CS, Bertone A, Cornish K, Boutet I, Der Kaloustian VM, Andermann E, Faubert J, Chaudhuri A. Integrative cortical dysfunction and pervasive motion perception deficit in fragile X syndrome. Neurology 2004;63:1634–9.
57. Weibull W. A statistical distribution function of wide applicability. J Appl Mech 1951;18:292–7.
58. Nguyen-Tri D, Overbury O, Faubert J. The role of lenticular senescence in age-related color vision changes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2003;44:3698–704.
59. Karwatsky P, Overbury O, Faubert J. Red–green chromatic mechanisms in normal aging and glaucomatous observers. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2004;45:2861–6.
60. Weale RA. The Aging Eye. London: HK Lewis; 1963.
61. Wu J, Coffey M, Reidy A, Wormald R. Impaired motion sensitivity as a predictor of subsequent field loss in glaucoma suspects: the Roscommon Glaucoma Study. Br J Ophthalmol 1998;82:534–7.
62. Weale RA. Aging and vision. Vision Res 1986;26:1507–12.
63. Spear PD. Neural bases of visual deficits during aging. Vision Res 1993;33:2589–609.
64. Faubert J, Bellavance F. Temporal modulation visual fields, normal aging, Parkinson's disease and methyl-mercury in the James Bay Cree: a feasibility study. Ar Qbras Oftalmol 2003;66:77–91. Available at: http://www.abonet.com.br/abo/665/abo66509.htm. Accessed April 3, 2006.
65. Wist ER, Schrauf M, Ehrenstein WH. Dynamic vision based on motion-contrast: changes with age in adults. Exp Brain Res 2000; 134:295–300.
66. Mendelson JR, Wells EF. Age-related changes in the visual cortex. Vision Res 2002;42:695–703.
67. Ansari EA, Morgan JE, Snowden RJ. Glaucoma: squaring the psychophysics and neurobiology. Br J Ophthalmol 2002;86:823–6.
68. Willis A, Anderson SJ. Effects of glaucoma and aging on photopic and scotopic motion perception. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2000;41:325–35.
Keywords:

cortical damage; glaucoma; motion; first-order; second-order; motion perimetry

© 2006 American Academy of Optometry