Share this article on:

Evidence-Based Practice for the Use of Internal Strategies as a Memory Compensation Technique After Brain Injury: A Systematic Review

O'Neil-Pirozzi, Therese M. ScD, CCC-SLP; Kennedy, Mary R.T. PhD; Sohlberg, McKay M. PhD

Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation: July/August 2016 - Volume 31 - Issue 4 - p E1–E11
doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000181
Original Articles

Objective: To complete a systematic review of internal memory strategy use with people who have brain injury and provide practitioners with information that will impact their clinical work.

Methods: A systematic literature search to identify published intervention studies that evaluated an internal memory strategy or technique to improve memory function of individuals with brain injury. Relevant data from reviewed articles were coded using 4 clinical questions targeting participants, interventions, research methods, and outcomes.

Results: A comprehensive search identified 130 study citations and abstracts. Forty-six met inclusion/exclusion criteria and were systematically reviewed. Visual imagery was most frequently studied, in isolation or in combination with other internal strategies. Despite significant variability in research methods and outcomes across studies, the evidence provides impetus for use of internal memory strategies with individuals following brain injury.

Conclusions: Individuals with traumatic brain injury may benefit from internal memory strategy use, and clinicians should consider internal memory strategy instruction as part of intervention plans. Further research needs to better delineate influences on intervention candidacy and outcomes.

Department of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts (Dr O'Neil-Pirozzi); Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Spaulding Rehabilitation Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts (Dr O'Neil-Pirozzi); Communicative Sciences and Disorders, Chapman University, Orange, California (Dr Kennedy); and Communication Disorders & Sciences, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon (Dr Sohlberg).

Corresponding Author: Therese M. O'Neil-Pirozzi, ScD, CCC-SLP, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northeastern University, 360 Huntington Ave, 70 Forsyth Bldg, Room 103, Boston, MA 02115 (t.oneil-pirozzi@neu.edu).

This project was supported by the Academy of Neurologic Communication Disorders and Sciences.

Supplemental digital content is available for this article. Direct URL citations appear in the printed text and are provided in the HTML and PDF versions of this article on the journal's Web site (www.headtraumarehab.com).

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

THIS ARTICLE is another in a series of publications by the Academy of Neurologic Communication Disorders and Sciences (ANCDS) on evidence-based practice (EBP) in the clinical management of neurogenic communication disorders. The ANCDS EBP project was initiated over a decade ago, with the creation of expert committees charged with reviewing the literature to develop evidence-based clinical practice guidelines for a range of neurogenic communication disorders.1,2 This article provides practice recommendations for the use of internal memory strategies and was generated by members of the ANCDS EBP subcommittee on cognitive-communication disorders associated with traumatic brain injury (TBI). It is a complement to 1 on the use of external aids as a memory compensation technique.3

Multiple systematic reviews and meta-analyses of memory strategy use post-TBI have been published. Some of these analyses are embedded in reviews of comprehensive cognitive rehabilitation therapy.4–12 A recent meta-analysis13 and a recent systematic review14 analyzed studies specific to memory rehabilitation, but both examined impairment-based or restorative therapies rather than strategy training interventions and, thus, were not relevant to the current review. A third recent review by an international group (INCOG) also focused on the memory rehabilitation literature but had a broader inclusion and reviewed studies evaluating the use of internal and external memory strategies as well as restorative techniques to develop clinical practice guidelines for memory intervention.12 They concluded that the integration of internal and external memory strategies taught individually or in groups can be useful to individuals with mild to moderate memory impairments and found weak evidence regarding restorative memory techniques.

Two frequently cited reviews were published by the Institute of Medicine (IOM)10 and Cicerone and colleagues.7 The IOM10 reported on 14 studies that featured internal memory strategies. To be included in their review, studies had to include a majority (50% or more) of participants with TBI and had to be published after 1991. Results revealed that the majority of the studies targeted participants with moderate-severe TBI. They concluded that there were immediate treatment benefits to those who used various internal memory strategies, characterized by improvement on standardized memory tests. However, there was limited evidence that internal memory strategies made positive functional changes that were also maintained over time.

In their 2005 review, Cicerone and colleagues7 reviewed treatment studies published between 1998 and 2002 in cognitive rehabilitation that included memory and the use of internal (eg, visual imagery and rehearsal) and external (eg, planners and alarms) strategies. Criteria for review included clinical studies conducted with participants diagnosed with TBI or stroke. Of the 13 studies evaluating remediation of memory deficits, 1 targeted internal memory strategies, and it was a prospective, randomized controlled trial that investigated the effectiveness of visual imagery training postacquired brain injury.15 On the basis of their 2000 and 2005 reviews, internal and external strategy training was classified as a practice standard for individuals with mild memory impairment. For individuals with moderate or severe memory impairment, the authors recommended external strategy training as a practice guideline. The 2011 review by this group did not include studies of internal memory strategies.8

Why then, this review? First, not all systematic reviews are created equal. Report characteristics vary across reviews and analyses, as do target stakeholder audiences, and so conclusions regarding evidence may vary.16,17 For example, a systematic review to inform the design of a research study would be different from a systematic review to inform a clinical decision to use 1 therapeutic technique versus another. Although research design determines the level of trustworthiness of the clinical conclusions, there can be important clinical lessons in articles with less rigorous methodology. A primary purpose of this article was to provide practitioners with clinically relevant information about the use of internal memory strategies following TBI reported in the research literature. Although the recent INCOG review12 did focus on practice intervention, its purpose was to provide broad practice guidelines for all memory interventions. We aimed at organizing and distilling the evidence in a way that will guide clinician implementation specifically for internal memory strategies.

Internal memory strategies are behaviors or techniques designed to help a person gain control over his or her learning and recall ability. Also known as mnemonic strategies, they involve mental manipulations to facilitate memory of targeted stimuli. They may be task specific and used to learn an explicit body of information such as forming an acronym to remember a medication regimen, or they may be generalized strategies designed to enhance memory across information domains such as the use of visual imagery for remembering.18 Internal memory strategies are carried out “internally” or covertly as one thinks in a new or different way to encode material. The use of internal memory strategies may be effective because they (1) encourage a deeper level of processing, which improves recall; (2) can facilitate integration of isolated information; and/or (3) provide built-in retrieval cues.19

The literature exploring the use of internal memory strategies has a long history. Internal memory papers span over 3 decades with more publications in the earlier years, as this memory intervention approach was used more commonly in the past.11 More than one-third of the studies analyzed for this review were published at least 2 decades ago. Although these early studies hold important lessons, the research methodology is not up to current standards, making it difficult to analyze their intervention efficacy.

More recent studies include several well-controlled trials15,20 evaluating and supporting the use of internal memory strategies. Yet, this approach has not been a primary focus in the cognitive rehabilitation literature since the 1990s. Kaschel and colleagues15 suggest several reasons why researchers and clinicians may be reticent to employ internal memory strategies—(1) they may be too complex for people with cognitive impairments; (2) they may be unnatural and difficult to apply to everyday life activities; and (3) their generalized use has not been well documented in people with memory and executive function impairments. The pervasive and devastating effects of memory impairments encourage investigation of all interventions with potential to mitigate the impact of memory deficits. This review attempts to integrate and analyze the available evidence within the context of challenges to their clinical implementation.

Back to Top | Article Outline

METHODS

Searching, gathering, and selecting studies

The literature search was conducted to identify published intervention studies that evaluated an internal memory strategy or technique whose purpose was to improve the memory functioning of individuals with brain injury. As in previous ANCDS systematic reviews of cognitive rehabilitation,3,9,21,22 the following inclusion and exclusion criteria were used to select studies for this review:

1. Studies that had participants with the diagnosis of TBI were included; studies that included other diagnostic groups were acceptable as long as individuals with TBI were included.

2. Peer reviewed, empirical studies were included; chapters, books, dissertations, theoretical articles or studies, and reports without data were excluded.

3. Study designs were considered for inclusion as long as identifiable baselines and posttreatment outcomes were reported.

4. Studies that investigated effects of using internal memory strategies were included. Studies that investigated the use of compensatory external memory aids only were excluded.3,8

5. Within-subject design studies in which participants were presented with alternative methods of encoding in a single trial were excluded because they were not intended as interventions.

Ten databases were searched through March 2015—MEDLINE, PsychInfo, PsychArticles, the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL), ERIC PubMed, CSA Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts, Health Source: Nursing, ISI (which includes Social Science Citation Index and Medline), ScienceDirect, and REHABDATA. Reference lists from prior published reviews were also examined and compared with the studies identified by searching databases. Prior reviews were also identified to make sure that no studies were missed, including the systematic reviews by Cicerone and colleagues,6–8 the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews,23 and the cognitive rehabilitation review by the IOM.10

The initial search was performed using combinations of the following keywords: traumatic brain injury or brain injury or closed head injury or acquired brain injury for the population; memory for the type of deficit; and intervention or treatment or therapy or training or remediation, or rehabilitation or imagery or elaboration or encoding or rehearsal for therapy.

Figure 1 displays the process to search, gather, identify, and select published, peer-reviewed intervention studies. This resulted in more than 2900 citations with abstracts. First, the second author conducted the search and reviewed citations and abstracts. The vast majority were excluded because they were pharmacological studies, were not peer reviewed, were not in English, or did not include a memory intervention. Next, each author independently read 130 abstracts, at which point another 54 were excluded for the same reasons. Thus, 76 studies were read in entirety and reviewed by the authors. There was 100% consensus to exclude an additional 30 studies for reasons listed on Figure 1, which brought the final number of included studies to 46.15,21,24–67

Back to Top | Article Outline

Reviewing and extracting data

Four clinical questions were used as a schema to code the relevant data extracted from each article.3,9,10,21,22 Table 1 lists these questions with the corresponding extracted data. The questions organized the data into the following 3 tables of evidence (TOE): (1) participant characteristics, (2) study design and intervention characteristics, and (3) study outcomes. These 3 tables are available at: http://links.lww.com/JHTR/A146; http://links.lww.com/JHTR/A147; and http://links.lww.com/JHTR/A148. The Participant TOE includes participant data from each study (eg, N, average age, years of education, and diagnosis). The Design and Intervention TOE includes study design, level of evidence, purpose of study, description of the intervention, research stage, and methodological quality indicators. The Outcomes TOE includes intervention outcomes and quality indicators related to outcomes.

We defined and coded each study's level of evidence using The American Academy of Neurology's (AAN) classifications I through IV.68,69 Table 2 provides descriptions of these, with their corresponding requirements and clinical recommendations. All types of designs were considered, including randomized control trials (RCTs) with or without assessor blinding, other group designs, and case reports. Because single-subject, multiple baseline studies are not included in the AAN's classification scheme, they were coded as class II evidence, consistent with prior reviews by ANCDS.3,21

Each study was evaluated for its research stage using the AAN68 schema as follows: “discovery” in which a treatment is assessed to show promise of efficacy; “efficacy” in which treatment is tested in an ideal and highly controlled condition/context; “effectiveness' in which treatment is tested in the “real world” and under routine conditions; and “cost benefit/public policy” in which the economic and societal benefits of the effective treatment are assessed. “Efficiency” studies are typically based on effective treatments to determine whether changes in dosage or timing result in changed outcomes. Methodological quality indicators were used to evaluate each study as present (1) or absent (0). Some indicators were applicable for group studies only—presence of a published intervention manual or protocol, assessor blinding, sampling methods description, comparability of groups or participants, and treatment fidelity.

The Outcomes TOE includes intervention outcomes and quality indicators related to outcomes, which were classified as decontextualized or person centered. Decontextualized outcomes are measurements traditionally identified as “impairment measures” based on the World Health Organization International Classification Framework.70 These are measures of underlying cognitive and/or memory functioning and do not capture a behavior, skill, or knowledge directly applicable to participants' daily memory functioning. Examples of decontextualized measures include formal assessments of cognitive or psychological functioning using tests not relevant to memory outside of an environmental context or activity and measurement of performance on tasks where the stimuli are not items needed to be remembered for daily functioning. Person-centered outcomes are those that are functionally relevant to the individual; they are direct or indirect measures of activity or participation (from the World Health Organization International Classification Framework)71 relevant to the participant's actual daily functioning. Examples include changes in functional activities such as remembering names of people in their actual daily life or self-/other reports using questionnaires inquiring about daily memory performance. Timing was identified for each type of outcome; that is, outcomes were described as being immediate after the withdrawal of intervention and after time had lapsed since the withdrawal of intervention, that is, maintenance. The presence of generalization to other untrained stimuli, activities, or tests was noted as well.

Because researchers typically report various outcome measures, we created a scoring system that accounts for the variety. These outcome “scores” ranged from 0 to 3 (0 = no measure reported, 1 = no significant intervention effect on outcome measure(s), 2 = significant intervention effect on some but not all measures [<50%], including primary or secondary measures, and 3 = significant intervention effect on most or all intended measures [≥50%]).

Finally, outcome quality indicators were identified and coded as present (1) or not present (0). These included statistical significance; intervention effect sizes; precision of intervention reported in confidence interval; outcome measures; and statistics that account for participant attrition, that is, intent-to-treat.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Reliability

Each author independently reviewed a subset of the 46 selected studies and extracted data onto TOE, as “the” primary reviewer. A second reviewer (1 of the authors) who was blinded to the primary reviewer's data extraction reviewed 16% or 35% of the studies. The interrater reliability agreement was 95.2% for participant data, 92.8% for design and intervention data, and 97.7% for outcomes data. Overall, the interrater agreement was 95.23%.

Back to Top | Article Outline

RESULTS

The 3 TOE were created to display extracted data from the 44 studies (available at: http://links.lww.com/JHTR/A146; http://links.lww.com/JHTR/A147; and http://links.lww.com/JHTR/A148). For ease in synthesizing data, studies were listed by their AAN classification of evidence, that is, I, II, III, IV,68 and then alphabetized. A narrative summary of the results answering each of the 4 clinical questions guiding this systematic review follows.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Question 1: who were the participants?

A total of 1 143 participants were included in the studies under review. Table 3 provides a summary of participant information, and the TOE: Participants (available at: http://links.lww.com/JHTR/A146) provides details of each study. The number of participants per study varied from 1 to 332, with the majority of studies (65%) having 20 or less, and 24% having 3 or less. Note that an inclusion criterion for this review was that a study had to have at least 1 participant with TBI or closed head injury. Up to 84% of participants across studies had sustained a TBI. Other diagnoses included stroke, anoxic brain injury, and brain tumor. Nearly all studies reported time postinjury onset, although with varied specificity (eg, >6 months; group mean; years and months per participant). Participants in just over half of the studies were greater than 1 year postinjury. Twenty-five studies (54%) had study-specific inclusion/exclusion criteria.

Sex information was provided in 32 of the 46 studies, with 69% of reported participants being male. Nearly all studies reported age at time of study participation, which ranged from 8.1 to 86.0 years of age. A majority of studies reported educational levels.

Just over half of the studies reported injury severity. Thirteen of the 27 studies (48%) used traditional labels of “mild,” “moderate,” and “severe” injury severity. One study characterized participants as having mild-moderate injury severity; 4 studies characterized participants as having severe injuries; and 7 studies included all severity levels to characterize injuries.

Impairment severity was reported in 25 of the studies. To assess cognition in general, or memory in particular, many of these studies used standardized tests (eg, California Verbal Learning Test,70), rating scales (eg, Dementia Rating Scale72), and questionnaires (eg, Everyday Memory Questionnaire73). Twenty-one of the studies used “mild,” “moderate,” and “severe” impairment labels, with participants in 4 of these studies (19%) being described as presenting with mild impairments, in 5 of these studies (24%) as presenting with mild-moderate impairments, in 6 studies (28.5%) as presenting with severe impairments, and in 6 (28.5%) as presenting with a range of impairments. Despite the use of these severity labels by 21 studies, only 3 of them15,20,31 reported using normative data to determine how standardized test scores converted to impairment severity.

Five studies reported that participants had received previous or simultaneous memory-related treatment, and 3 reported that participants did not. Thus, 89% did not report treatment history.

In summary, studies were appraised for 12 participant characteristics. The range of documented characteristics was between 1 and 11 (mean = 7.74; standard deviation = 1.97). Participant characteristics reported in most studies included the number of participants, age, diagnosis, time postonset, and injury severity. Characteristics reported in the least number of studies included comorbidities, therapy history, etiology of injury, and education. Studies reported some participant characteristics differently, and this variability prevented identification of clear participant-related trends.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Question 2: what comprised the intervention?

To synthesize the interventions, internal memory strategies had to be identified first. One author reviewed strategy descriptions and sorted the studies into the following 8 categories: (1) Unspecified Internal Memory Strategies; (2) Imagery/Unspecified Computer Program; (3) Imagery Story/Method of Loci; (4) Metacognitive Reading Strategy PQRST; (5) Verbal Elaboration/Semantic Association; (6) Rehearsal; (7) Combined Visual Imagery and Rehearsal/Association; and (8) Combined Verbal/Written Rehearsal and Elaboration or Altered Format. To establish validity of the categories and reliability of strategy assignment, each of the other 2 authors reviewed half of the studies and placed them in the identified categories. Up to 95% interrater agreement was achieved initially; differences between coders on 2 studies were discussed and resolved to reach 100% agreement.

Table 4 lists the strategies and the number of studies that used them. See the Design and Intervention TOE available at: http://links.lww.com/JHTR/A147 for details of each study. The most frequently evaluated specific strategy was visual imagery. Fourteen studies evaluated efficacy of improving memory for targeted information using visual imagery by creating story, location, or concept images with and without computer delivery. Twenty-two studies evaluated different combinations of strategies, representing the largest group of strategies. Of these combined strategies, those that added imagery to a type of association or rehearsal strategy (eg, chaining) accounted for the largest strategy group. Thus, imagery was involved in 32 (69%) of the 46 studies.

A key characteristic of strategy intervention involves identifying how participants are introduced to and taught individual strategies. A review of the studies suggested the following 4 categories of instruction: (1) systematic instruction in which the authors used explicit teaching procedures designed to teach strategy use such as errorless learning; (2) verbal instruction, which included a script or verbal description of what was said to the participant regarding strategy implementation; (3) practice in which the authors indicated there was opportunity to practice the strategy on nontest stimuli; and (4) unspecified instruction in which there was no indication of strategy instruction. The largest category was unspecified instruction found in 30 (65%) of the 46 studies. Eight studies employed systematic instruction to explicitly teach the strategies; 3 employed verbal instruction, and 5 employed practice.

Some of the studies that were coded as having unspecified instruction were delivered in ways that would not require instruction because the strategy was embedded in the nature of the task. For example, organizing a word list to be remembered using a taxonomic category clustering42 or providing the paired associate for a learning task31,41 does not require internally generated strategy use by the learner and instead depends on the researcher to deliver the strategy.

The intensity (ie, dosage) of interventions varied widely and depended on the rationale for the study. Studies that compared specific types of strategies using list learning paradigms tended to have few evaluation sessions, whereas those studies that were evaluating a memory intervention program conducted more sessions over a longer duration. Study duration ranged from 11 months to 1 week. The mode duration for studies evaluating a memory strategy training intervention was 6 weeks with 1 to 3 sessions per week. For these treatment studies, the type of intervention was split equally between therapies using memory groups and 1-on-1 interventions. Of the 1-on-1 interventions, 4 studies evaluated computer packages.

In summary, the most commonly evaluated strategy is visual imagery. The majority of the literature base did not describe replicable instructional procedures indicating how to introduce strategies that participants would need to independently implement them to be functional.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Question 3: what comprised the research methodology?

The strength of a body of evidence is determined by examining the design and methodology of each study. Features summarized here include study design and level of evidence, research stage, and quality indicators (eg, fidelity and blinded assessors). Table 5 provides this information across the 46 studies. Details of each study can be found on the Design and Intervention TOE (see Supplemental Digital Content, available at: http://links.lww.com/JHTR/A147).

No studies were classified as level I RCTs that met all quality indicators. Of the 20 level II studies, 9 were RCTs. The 4 most commonly omitted quality indicators were assessor blinding, fidelity measurement, effect size, and intent to treat statistics. Eight of the level II studies were “other group designs,” and 3 were single-subject, multiple baseline studies. The 17 level II group design studies reported between 0 and 4 quality indicators. Most of these were efficacy studies, followed by studies of discovery, studies of effectiveness, and 1 study of efficiency.

Of the 15 level III studies, 9 were discovery studies, and 6 were efficacy studies. Quality indicators varied depending on the design. Four level III studies did not report any quality indicators, whereas the remaining 11 studies reported between 1 and 4 indicators.

The 11 level IV studies were single case reports or case series. With the exception of 1, all were discovery studies. Four of these 11 studies did not report any quality indicators, whereas the other 7 reported 1 or 2 indicators.

In summary, many of the studies lacked intervention detail, yet, according to the AAN's 201168 methodology and indicators, the collective evidence base would be sufficient to generate a Practice Guideline for the use of internal memory strategies if the outcomes are positive.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Question 4: what comprised the types of outcome measures used and what did they demonstrate?

Types of intervention outcomes

Forty-one of the 46 studies (85%) investigated decontextualized outcomes; 19 studies (41%) investigated person-centered outcomes; and 15 studies (33%) investigated both types of intervention outcomes (see the Outcomes TOE available at: http://links.lww.com/JHTR/A148). All 46 studies (100%) reported immediate intervention outcomes, and nearly half (48%) reported delayed intervention outcomes. Table 6 summarizes the breakdown of intervention outcome types across the 46 studies.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Intervention outcomes and findings

Most of the studies that investigated intervention impact on immediate decontextualized outcomes used multiple measures. Standardized tests, especially ones assessing memory, were frequently used, including the California Verbal Learning Test,70 the Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test,74 and the Wechsler Memory Scales.75 Additional “control” measures were standardized tests of attention, reading comprehension, and verbal fluency. Some studies used psychological tests to measure anxiety and depression outcomes. Other types of decontextualized outcome measures included performance on intervention tasks, such as the number of stimuli recalled, the number of trials to reach criterion, the number of cues provided, strategies used, the number of strategies used, knowledge about memory quiz scores, and demographic and injury-related outcome predictors.

Thirty-seven of the 41 studies (90%) that investigated immediate decontextualized outcomes reported some degree of postintervention improvement or positive difference/change on at least 1 outcome measure. Six of the 17 studies (35%) investigating delayed decontextualized outcomes reported some delayed postintervention improvement or positive difference/change on at least 1 measure. Seven of these studies (41%) reported that at least some immediate decontextualized intervention improvements or positive differences/changes were maintained. Four studies (24%) reported that at least some immediate decontextualized improvements or positive differences/changes were not maintained.

Most of the studies that investigated intervention impact on immediate person-centered outcomes used multiple measures. Many of these measures were nonstandardized tools used for specific, functional purposes. Some standardized outcome measurement tools were used, including the Memory Compensation Questionnaire,75 the Prospective-Retrospective Memory Questionnaire,76 the Satisfaction With Life Scale,77 and the Subjective Memory Questionnaire.78

All 17 of the studies (100%) that investigated immediate person-centered outcomes reported some degree of postintervention improvement or positive difference/change on at least 1 outcome measure. Of the 8 studies that investigated intervention impact on delayed person-centered outcomes, 7 of them (88%) reported some delayed postintervention improvement or positive difference/change in at least 1 measure.

In summary, the outcomes reported in the literature suggested that persons with memory impairments following brain injury benefit from internal memory strategy training. However, the majority of the evidence-base investigated decontextualized outcomes of interventions, and a minority investigated person-centered outcomes. Of those that did investigate functionally relevant outcomes, 100% reported participant improvement. All of the studies investigated immediate intervention outcomes, and only half investigated delayed intervention outcomes. With the emphasis of rehabilitation on maximizing person-centered, functional outcomes and quality of life,70 establishing the reliability and validity of current and/or developing new patient centered outcomes needs to be a priority. Increased number of studies of internal memory strategies is needed to investigate both their immediate and long-term impacts on decontextualized AND functional outcomes and to identify the most efficacious intervention approaches to achieve and maintain positive outcomes of both kinds.

It is important to consider outcomes with respect to the quality of research methods. Experimental rigor of studies has improved over time. This review used a strict criteria of 10 quality indicators. Four RCTs15,20,40,45 were missing 3 to 5 of the 10 quality indicators; thus, the evidence base is close to being sufficient to generate a Practice Standard.

Back to Top | Article Outline

CONCLUSION

The evidence base provides encouragement for clinicians and researchers to explore use of internal memory strategies with persons with brain injury. However, a number of limitations make clinical interpretation of the evidence base challenging; hence, clinicians need to carefully collect patient-specific outcome data to evaluate treatment response that can guide ongoing therapy decisions. The significant variability in participant characteristics makes it difficult to identify relevant candidacy factors for matching internal memory strategies to particular patient profiles. For example, the authors held a hypothesis that internal memory strategies would be most useful for participants with mild memory impairment, which was a finding in the INCOG review.12 Yet, this review showed no clear indication that only those with mild memory impairment benefit from these strategies, and participant profiles were not sufficiently defined to test this hypothesis across studies. However, it was reassuring that the vast majority of research participants were indeed individuals who had sustained traumatic injuries.

Although the scientific rigor of studies has improved over time, this review suggests several methodological issues that should be addressed in future studies. As noted, participant profiles need to be better defined and compared. Experimental designs need to include assessor blinding and treatment fidelity measures, in addition to effect size and intent-to-treat statistics. It is concerning that the majority of the literature base does not offer replicable instructional procedures to teach strategies that would optimize participants' independence and generalize to their daily lives. In addition, it is critical that both decontextualized and person-centered outcome measures are used to evaluate the impact of strategy training.

Overall, the literature base, which spans decades, supports the efficacy of internal memory strategy training. Perhaps, the most clear and clinically relevant trend in the literature was that all of the studies that used systematic instruction and incorporated practice in using strategies reported positive outcomes. When participants with brain injury are taught methods designed to elaborate stimuli to be remembered or deepen the processing while learning information, it increases the likelihood they will be able to recall the information. Given the uniformity of positive reports including findings from 9 RCTs, strategy instruction should be part of a clinician's treatment repertoire. The strongest evidence is in support of visual imagery training; however, visual imagery was also the most commonly evaluated internal memory strategy. Of note to clinicians is that both group and individual instruction were found to be efficacious. Although positive effects of strategy training are reported across different strategy types and participant profiles, it is difficult to directly compare studies given the divergent outcome measures and implementation procedures. Further research investigating the impact of internal memory strategy training on functional outcomes is needed.

One of the goals of this article was to distill the research evidence to guide clinical practice. A description of the clinical implementation of internal memory strategies as a memory management approach may be found under Clinical Implementation at: http://links.lww.com/JHTR/A149.

Back to Top | Article Outline

REFERENCES

1. Golper L, Wertz RT, Frattali C, Yorkston K, Myers P, Katz R. Evidence-based guidelines for the management of communication disorders in neurologically impaired individuals: project introduction. http://www.ancds.org/assets/docs/EBP/practiceguidelines.pdf. Accessed March 2014.
2. Yorkston KM, Spencer KA, Duffy JR, et al. Evidence-based medicine and practice guidelines: application to the field of speech-language pathology. J Med Speech-Lang Pathology. 2001;9(4):243–256.
3. Sohlberg MM, Kennedy M, Avery J, et al. Evidence-based practice for the use of external aids as a memory compensation technique. J Med Speech-Lang Path. 2007;15(1):xv–li.
4. Cappa SF, Benke T, Clarke S, Rossi B, Stemmer B, van Heugten CM. EFNS guidelines on cognitive rehabilitation: report of an EFNS Task Force. Eur J Neurol. 2003;10:11–23.
5. Cappa SF, Benke T, Clarke S, Rossi B, Stemmer B, van Heugten CM. EFNS guidelines on cognitive rehabilitation: report of an EFNS task force. Eur J Neurol. 2005;12:665–680.
6. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Kalmar K, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: recommendations for clinical practice. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2000;81(12):1596–1615.
7. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Malec JF, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 1998 through 2002. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2005;86(8):1681–1692.
8. Cicerone KD, Langenbahn DM, Braden C, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation updated review of the literature from 2003 through 2008. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2011;92(4):519–530.
9. Ehlhardt LA, Sohlberg MM, Kennedy M, et al. Evidence-based practice guidelines for instructing individuals with neurogenic memory impairments: what have we learned in the past 20 years? Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2008;18(3):300–342.
10. Institute of Medicine. Cognitive Rehabilitation Therapy for Traumatic Brain Injury: Evaluating the Evidence. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2011.
11. Richardson JTE. The efficacy of imagery mnemonics in memory remediation. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33(11):1345–1357.
12. Velikonja D, Tate R, Ponsford J, McIntyre A, Janzen S, Bayles M. INCOG recommendations for management of cognition following traumatic brain injury, part V: memory. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2014;29(4):369–386.
13. Elliott M, Parente F. Efficacy of memory rehabilitation therapy: a meta-analysis of TBI and stroke cognitive rehabilitation literature. Brain Inj. 2014;28(12):1610–1616.
14. Spreij LA, Visser-Meily JMA, van Heugten CM, Nijboer TCW. Novel insights into the rehabilitation of memory post acquired brain injury: a systematic review. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:993–1012.
15. Kaschel R, Della Sala S, Cantagallo A, et al. Imagery mnemonics for the rehabilitation of memory: a randomised group controlled trial. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2002;12(2):127–153.
16. Schlosser RW, Wendt O, Sigafoos J. Not all systematic reviews are created equal: considerations for appraisal. Evid-Based Commun Assess Interv. 2007;1:138–150.
17. Turkstra LS, Kennedy MRT. Traumatic brain injury and cognitive rehabilitation: current approaches to research, reimbursement, and clinical treatment. ASHA Leader. 2008;13:10–13.
18. Sohlberg MM, Turkstra LS. Optimizing Cognitive Rehabilitation: Effective Instructional Methods. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2011.
19. Wilson BA. Management and remediation of memory problems in brain injured adults. In:Baddeley A, Wilson B, Watts FN, eds. Handbook of Memory Disorders. New York, NY: Wiley; 1995:451–479.
20. O'Neil-Pirozzi TM, Strangman GE, Goldstein R, et al. A controlled treatment study of internal memory strategies (I-MEMS) following traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2010;25(1):43–51.
21. Kennedy MRT, Coelho C, Turkstra L, et al. Intervention for executive functions after traumatic brain injury: a systematic review, meta-analysis and clinical recommendations. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2008;18(3)257–299.
22. Ylvisaker M, Turkstra L, Coelho C, Kennedy MRT, Sohlberg MM, Yorkston KM. Behavioral interventions for individuals with behavior disorders after traumatic brain injury: a systematic review. Brain Inj. 2007;21(8):243–256.
23. Linden M, Hawley C, Blackwood B, Evans J, Anderson V. Technological aids for the rehabilitation of memory and executive functioning in children and adolescents with acquired brain injury (protocol). Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2014;3:1–12.
24. Barko-Collo SL. Initial evaluation of a structured group format memory rehabilitation program. J Cogn Rehabil. 2000;18(2):16–21.
25. Berg IJ, Koning-Haanstra M, Deelman BG. Long-term effects of memory rehabilitation: a controlled study. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 1991;1(2):97–111.
26. Cancelliere AE, Moncada C, Reid DT. Memory retraining to support educational reintegration. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1991;72(2):148–151.
27. Dirette DK, Hinojosa J, Carnevale GJ. Comparison of remedial and compensatory interventions for adults with acquired brain injuries. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 1999;14(6)595–601.
28. Dou ZL, Man DW, Ou HN, Zheng JL. Computerized errorless learning-based memory rehabilitation for Chinese patients with brain injury: a preliminary quasi-experimental clinical design study. Brain Inj. 2006;20(3):219–225.
29. Downes JJ, Kalla T, Davies A, Flynn A, Ali H, Mayes AR. The pre-exposure technique: a novel method for enhancing the effects of imagery in face-name association learning. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 1997;7(3):195–214.
30. Freeman MR, Mittenberg W, Dicowden M, Bat-Ami M. Executive and compensatory memory retraining in traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 1992;6(1):65–70.
31. Hux K, Manasse N, Wright S, Snell J. Effect of training frequency on face-name recall by adults with traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2000;14(10):907–920.
32. Jennet SM, Lincoln NB. An evaluation of the effectiveness of group therapy for memory problems. Int Disabil Stud. 1991;13(3):83–86.
33. Kerner MJ, Acker M. Computer delivery of memory retraining with head injured patients. Cogn Rehabil. 1985;3(6):26–31.
34. Manasse NJ, Hux K, Snell J. Teaching face-name associations to survivors of traumatic brain injury: a sequential treatment approach. Brain Inj. 2005;19(8):633–641.
35. Niemann H, Ruff RM, Baser CA. Computer-assisted attention retraining in head-injured individuals: a controlled efficacy study of an outpatient program. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1990;58(6):811–817.
36. Potvin M-J, Rouleau I, Sénéchal G, Giguère J-F. Prospective memory rehabilitation based on visual imagery techniques. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2011;21(6):899–924.
37. Ruff R, Mahaffey R, Engel J, Farrow C, Cox D, Karzmark P. Efficacy study of THINKable in the attention and memory retraining of traumatically head injured patients. Brain Inj. 1994;8(1):3–14.
38. Ryan TV, Ruff RM. The efficacy of structured memory retraining in a group comparison of head trauma patients. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1988;3(2):165–179.
39. Tam SF, Man WK. Evaluating computer-assisted memory retraining programmes for people with post-head injury amnesia. Brain Inj. 2004;18(5):461–470.
40. Thickpenny-Davis KL, Barker-Collo SL. Evaluation of a structured group format memory rehabilitation program for adults following brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2007;22(5):303–313.
41. Twum M, Parenté R. Role of imagery and verbal labeling in the performance of paired associates tasks by persons with closed-head injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1994;16(4):630–639.
42. Baddeley AD, Warrington EK. Memory coding and amnesia. Neuropsychologia. 1973;11(2):159–165.
43. Benedict R, Wechsler FS. Evaluation of memory retraining in patients with traumatic brain injury: two single-case experimental designs. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 1992;7(4):83–92.
44. Evans JJ, Wilson BA. A memory group for individuals with traumatic brain injury. Clin Rehabil. 1992;6(1):75–81.
45. Franzen KM, Roberts MA, Schmits D, Verduyn W, Manshadi F. Cognitive remediation in pediatric traumatic brain injury. Child Neuropsychol. 1996;2(3):176–184.
46. Goldstein G, Beers SR, Longmore S, McCue M. Efficacy of memory training: a technological extension and replication. Clin Neuropsychol. 1996;10(1):66–72.
47. Grilli MD, McFarland CP. Imagine that: self-imagination improves prospective memory in memory-impaired individuals with neurological damage. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2011;21(6):847–859.
48. Harris JR. Verbal rehearsal and memory in children with closed-head injury: a quantitative and qualitative analysis. J Commun Disord. 1996;29(2):79–93.
49. Hildebrandt H, Bussmann-Mork B, Schwendemann G. Group therapy for memory impaired patients: a partial remediation is possible. J Neurol. 2006;253(4):512–519.
50. Huckans M, Pavawalla S, Demadura T, et al. A pilot study examining effects of group-based Cognitive Strategy Training treatment on self-reported cognitive problems, psychiatric symptoms, functioning, and compensatory strategy use in OIF/OEF combat veterans with persistent mild cognitive disorder and history of traumatic brain injury. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2010;47(1):43–60.
51. Kovner R, Mattis S, Pass R. Some amnesic patients can freely recall large amounts of information in new contexts. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1985;7(4):395–411.
52. Lewinsohn PM, Danaher BG, Kikel S. Visual imagery as a mnemonic aid for brain-injured persons. J Consult Clin Psyschol. 1977;45(5):717–723.
53. Malec EA, Goldstein G, McCue M. Predictors of memory training success in patients with closed-head injury. Neuropsychol. 1991;5(1):29–34.
54. Milders M, Deelman B, Berg I. Rehabilitation of memory for people's names. Memory. 1998;6(1):21–36.
55. Parenté R, Kolokowsky S, Elliott A. Retraining rehearsal after traumatic brain injury. Cogn Tech. 1998;3(2):33–38.
56. Thoene AI, Glisky EL. Learning of name-face associations in memory impaired patients: a comparison of different training procedures. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1995;1(1):29–38.
57. Crosson B, Buenning W. An individualized memory retraining program after closed-head injury: a single-case study. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1984;6(3):287–301.
58. Crovitz HF, Harvey MT, Horn RW. Problems in the acquisition of imagery mnemonics: three brain damaged cases. Cortex. 1979;15(2):225–284.
59. Glasgow RE, Zeiss RA, Barrera M Jr, Lewinsohn PM. Case studies on remediating memory deficits in brain damaged individuals. J Clin Psychol. 1977;33(4):1049–1054.
60. Godfrey HP, Knight RG. Memory training and behavioral rehabilitation of a severely head injured adult. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1988;69(6):458–460.
61. Goldstein G, McCue M, Turner SM, Spanier C, Malec EA, Shelly C. An efficacy study of memory training for patients with closed-head injury. Clin Neuropsychol. 1988;2(3):251–259.
62. Laatsch L. Development of a memory training program. Cogn Rehabil. 1983;1(4):15–18.
63. Lawson MJ, Rice DN. Effects of training in use of executive strategies on a verbal memory problem resulting from closed-head injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1989;11(6):842–854.
64. Malec J, Questad K. Rehabilitation of memory after craniocerebral trauma: case report. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1983;64(9):436–438.
65. Molloy M, Rand E, Brown W. Memory retraining: a study of four cases. Aust Occup Ther J. 1984;31(1):20–27.
66. Oberg L, Turkstra LS. Use of elaborative encoding to facilitate verbal learning after adolescent traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 1998;13(3):44–62.
67. Zencius A, Wesolowski MD, Burke WH. A comparison of four memory strategies with traumatically brain injured clients. Brain Inj. 1990;4(1):33–38.
68. American Academy of Neurology. Clinical Practice Guideline Process Manual, 2011 ed. St Paul, MN: The American Academy of Neurology; 2011.
69. Kennedy MRT. Evidence-based practice and cognitive rehabilitation therapy. In:McDonald S, Togher L, Code C, eds. Social and Communication Disorders Following Traumatic Brain Injury. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2013:282–306.
70. Delis DC, Kramer KH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1987.
71. World Health Organization. ICIDH-2: International Classification of Impairments, Disabilities, and Handicaps. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2001.
72. Mattis S. Dementia Rating Scale. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 1988.
73. Sunderland A, Harris JE, Baddeley AD. Do laboratory tests predict everyday memory? A neuropsychological study. J Verb Learn Verb Be. 1983;22(3):341–357.
74. Wilson BA, Cockburn J, Baddeley A, Hiorns R. The Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test-II Supplement Two. Bury St Edmunds, England: Thames Valley Test Company; 2003.
75. Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1987.
76. Smith G, Della Sala S, Logie RH, Maylor EA. Prospective and retrospective memory in normal ageing and dementia: a questionnaire study. Memory. 2000;8(5):311–321.
77. Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. J Pers Assess. 1985;49(1):71–75.
78. Sunderland A, Watts K, Baddeley AD, Harris JE. Subjective memory assessment and test performance in the elderly. J Gerontol. 1983;41:376–384.

brain injury; evidence-based practice; memory; rehabilitation; systematic review

Supplemental Digital Content

Back to Top | Article Outline
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.