Skip Navigation LinksHome > May 2014 - Volume 25 - Issue 3 > Obesity Paradox: Conditioning on Disease Enhances Biases in...
Epidemiology:
doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000075
Methods

Obesity Paradox: Conditioning on Disease Enhances Biases in Estimating the Mortality Risks of Obesity

Preston, Samuel H.; Stokes, Andrew

Open Access
Supplemental Author Material
Article Outline
Collapse Box

Author Information

From the Population Studies Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA.

Supported by grant number R01AG040212 from the National Institute on Aging.

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Supplemental digital content is available through direct URL citations in the HTML and PDF versions of this article ( www.epidem.com). This content is not peer-reviewed or copy-edited; it is the sole responsibility of the author.

Correspondence: Samuel H. Preston, Population Studies Center, University of Pennsylvania, 3718 Locust Walk, McNeil Building, Room 239, Philadelphia, PA 19104. E-mail: spreston@sas.upenn.edu.

Collapse Box

Abstract

Background:

Many studies have documented an obesity paradox—a survival advantage of being obese—in populations diagnosed with a medical condition. Whether obesity is causally associated with improved mortality in these conditions is unresolved.

Methods:

We develop the logic of collider bias as it pertains to the association between smoking and obesity in a diseased population. Data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) are used to investigate this bias empirically among persons with diabetes and prediabetes (dysglycemia). We also use NHANES to investigate whether reverse causal pathways are more prominent among people with dysglycemia than in the source population. Cox regression analysis is used to examine the extent of the obesity paradox among those with dysglycemia. In the regression analysis, we explore interactions between obesity and smoking, and we implement a variety of data restrictions designed to reduce the extent of reverse causality.

Results:

We find an obesity paradox among persons with dysglycemia. In this population, the inverse association between obesity and smoking is much stronger than in the source population, and the extent of illness and weight loss is greater. The obesity paradox is absent among never-smokers. Among smokers, the paradox is eliminated through successive efforts to reduce the extent of reverse causality.

Conclusion:

Higher mortality among normal-weight people with dysglycemia is not causal but is rather a product of the closer inverse association between obesity and smoking in this subpopulation.

In a wide variety of disease states, obese persons have been shown to experience lower mortality and better survival than that shown by the nonobese. These states include diabetes,1–3 coronary artery disease,4,5 heart failure,6 peripheral arterial disease,5 hypertension,7 chronic obstructive pulmonary disease,8 lung cancer,9 and esophageal adenocarcinoma.10 Superior survival among the obese patients has also been demonstrated after myocardial infarction,11 coronary revascularization,12 and angiography,13 and among hemodialysis patients.14 Better survival for obese patients in these disease states is considered paradoxical because obesity is associated with higher mortality in the vast majority of studies where it has been investigated.15

In this article, we argue that the obesity paradox is a product of statistical biases. Although these biases are present in most observational cohort studies of the mortality risks of obesity, they are exaggerated when attention is limited to populations that are conditioned on a disease state. We demonstrate these biases through formal reasoning and by application to a population with diabetes and prediabetes.

Back to Top | Article Outline

BIASES IN STUDIES OF THE MORTALITY RISKS OF OBESITY

Confounding by unmeasured or poorly measured variables is probably the most widely recognized bias in analyses of mortality risk factors. In studies of the mortality consequences of obesity, failure to properly control for smoking is a classic example of confounding.16 Because smoking is negatively correlated with body mass index (BMI) in the United States,17 failure to properly control smoking exposure is likely to result in underestimating the mortality risks associated with obesity.18 Crude categorizations and mismeasurement of smoking allow ample opportunity for residual confounding even when smoking is “controlled.”16,19 Stram et al18 cite 6 studies in which the correlation5 between cotinine levels and reported smoking behavior was only 0.40 to 0.70. A frequent suggestion aimed at minimizing confounding by smoking is to restrict a study to never-smokers,16 although such restrictions may produce only small changes in the shape of the relation between obesity and mortality.20,21

A second potential bias producing an underestimation of the mortality risks of obesity has been referred to as reverse causation. Low weight can be caused by loss of appetite or increased metabolic demands from manifest or occult disease which may in turn raise the risk of death.16,22 Among those with diabetes, unintentional weight loss commonly accompanies poor glucose control.23 The most serious condition associated with unintentional weight loss is cachexia, which is an involuntary loss of muscle mass as a result of an increase in circulating inflammatory cytokines.24 Many diseases can initiate this process, including chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, cancer, heart failure, and chronic renal failure.24,25 This condition is common; approximately 5 million Americans experience from cachexia.25 Although these processes have been clearly described, their empirical significance for estimating the relation between obesity and mortality has been questioned.26

Back to Top | Article Outline

INCREASED BIAS AMONG PERSONS SELECTED INTO A DISEASE STATE

Reverse causation and statistical confounding by smoking may become greater threats to accurate identification of the mortality risks of obesity when a study is confined to people who have entered a disease state. Consider first the threat of statistical confounding of obesity risks by smoking. Figure 1 presents three 2 × 2 tables. Figure 1A is the joint distribution of a population on 2 risk factors, which we have labeled Obese (Yes/No) and Smoker (Yes/No). Figure 1B represents the relative risk of developing diabetes for each combination of exposures on these two risk factors. Each cell in Figure 1C is the product of the equivalent cells in Figure 1A and B, so this table represents the joint distribution of the number of new cases of diabetes with respect to smoking and obesity exposure.

FIGURE 1.
FIGURE 1.
Image Tools

Now compare the relation between risk factors among persons with diabetes with the equivalent relation in the population as a whole (the source population). In the source population, the ratio of the odds of smoking among nonobese persons to the odds of smoking among obese persons is:

(1)
(1)
Image Tools

Among people with diabetes, the equivalent odds ratio of smoking among nonobese persons to smoking among obese persons is:

(2)
(2)
Image Tools

Thus, the ratio of the odds ratios in the two populations is:

(3)
(3)
Image Tools

A ratio of OD/OP > 1.00 would indicate that the relative prevalence of smoking among nonobese persons compared with the obese persons is greater among those with diabetes than it is in the source population.

Suppose that the risks of developing diabetes associated with smoking and obesity are multiplicative, ie, that the relative risk of developing diabetes if one is a smoker is the same whether or not one is obese. Then,

(4)
(4)
Image Tools

In this case, OP = OD, and the joint distribution of risk factors among those with diabetes will simply replicate that in the population. But if,

(5)
(5)
Image Tools

then OD > OP. Condition (5) means that the risk of developing diabetes is less than a multiplicative function of being obese and being a smoker. That is, if one is both obese and smoker, the relative risk of developing diabetes is less than the product of the smoking-relative risk for nonobese persons and the obesity-relative risk for nonsmokers. Such a circumstance would apply, for example, when risks are additive rather than multiplicative. Supermultiplicativity would reverse the inequality sign in (5).

If condition (5) prevails, then exposures that are independently distributed in the population become negatively correlated among the diseased. When exposures are negatively associated in the source population, as in the case of obesity and smoking, the negative association will become stronger among those in the diseased state.

The change in the association between two variables on entry into a disease state is a special case of what has been referred to as “conditioning on an effect,” “selection bias,” “collider bias,” and “event index bias.”27–31 The relations among exposures and mortality observed in the source population are conditioned on the “effect” of being diabetic. This effect may change the relation between the two exposures (smoking and obesity) relative to their relation in the source population. The magnitude and direction of any bias induced by conditioning on diabetes is unpredictable without additional information. What is unusual about the application to relations between smoking and obesity is that the two risk factors are negatively correlated in the source population. When condition (5) prevails, this negative correlation becomes exaggerated in the diseased population, enhancing the threat that the mortality risks of obesity will be underestimated because of a failure to fully control the effects of smoking.

Consider now the bias associated with reverse causation. The strength of reverse causal pathways should be greater in a diseased population because the frequency of disease-associated weight loss should be higher. Among people with diabetes, those losing weight have higher mortality than those with stable weight.32 We expect that smoking will play a key role in patterns of weight loss among those with dysglycemia. A meta-analysis concluded that smokers were 44% more likely than nonsmokers to develop diabetes.33 Smoking is associated with many diseases that may cause cachexia, including chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, cancer, heart failure, and chronic renal failure. Therefore, we expect that the incidence of weight loss, and the associated biases in estimating the mortality effects of obesity, will be accentuated in the population with diabetes.

We summarize our causal model in the directed acyclic graph (DAG) shown in Figure 2. Both obesity (represented by “body mass index”) and smoking are seen as causally related to the development of diabetes. The two exposures “collide” in diabetes, a condition that raises the risk of death. Smoking is seen as producing lower weight. This effect works through two pathways: smoking increases rates of metabolism and reduces appetite, producing the negative cross-sectional association between BMI and smoking found in the United States17; and smoking increases the incidence of diseases that produce weight loss and cachexia. The first of these pathways represents a standard version of statistical confounding, whereas the second is typically referred to as reverse causation. Smoking increases the risk of death apart from the pathway operating through diabetes.

FIGURE 2.
FIGURE 2.
Image Tools

On the basis of this DAG and the reasoning and evidence that support it, we hypothesize the following:

  • (1) The collision of obesity and smoking among those diagnosed with diabetes will strengthen the negative association between obesity and smoking.
  • (2) The high prevalence of smoking among those diagnosed with diabetes will increase the incidence of illness and weight loss relative to the incidence in the source population.
  • (3) Because of the two processes hypothesized in (1) and (2), the relatively low mortality of nonobese persons in the source population will be muted or even reversed (producing an “obesity paradox”) in the diabetic population.
  • (4) Among people with diabetes who never smoked, the normal relation of higher mortality among the obese will be maintained. The DAG appropriate for those who never smoked is easily envisioned by eliminating all arrows from smoking in Figure 2. Among never-smokers, there is no collision between smoking and obesity, and the sources of bias associated with smoking are eliminated.
Back to Top | Article Outline

EMPIRICAL APPLICATION

To examine the biases produced by conditioning on diabetes, we require data on the population with diabetes as well as data on the population from which persons with diabetes are selected. Such data are provided by the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES), a nationally representative survey of the noninstitutionalized US population. We investigate the obesity paradox among people with diabetes, a state in which the obesity paradox has been repeatedly demonstrated.1–3 To increase the number of observations, we include people with prediabetes, a group that we refer to as having “dysglycemia,” as well as those with diabetes.

We construct the sample using data from NHANES III (1988–1994) and from NHANES continuous waves (1999–2004). After linkage to the National Death Index through 2006,34 the period used for mortality analysis is 1988–2006. Mean follow-up is 9.2 years. NHANES provides clinical data on glycemia, as well as measured data on height and weight. We defined diabetes using the hemoglobin A1c (HbA1c) test, which we adopted over other diagnostic markers (such as fasting plasma glucose) because HbA1c exhibits greater stability and lower intraindividual variation.35 To construct our group with dysglycemia, we adopt an HbA1c threshold of 5.7%, in accordance with the most recent national American Diabetes Association guidelines.36 We include as dysglycemic those persons who report a prior diagnosis of diabetes even if their HbA1c value is less than 5.7%. We eliminate persons whose reported age at the time of diagnosis was less than 25 years to reduce the number with type 1 diabetes. For the analysis, we focus on people aged 35 to 74 at baseline. After eliminating missing observations on HbA1c, BMI, smoking, education, and mortality status, the final sample consists of 4642 dysglycemic persons, among whom 1162 died during follow-up.

We operationalize weight status using a dichotomous construct of obesity consistent with that used in some previous studies.1,9,11 We classify people as either normal weight (18.5–24.9 kg/m2) or overweight/obese (≥25 kg/m2). We estimate age-standardized mortality rates by weight and smoking status using the US 2000 census as the standard. We additionally apply Cox proportional hazards models to adjust for covariates. Attained age is used for analysis time. Covariates include sex, race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic black, Hispanic, or other), educational attainment (less than high school, high school or equivalent, or some college or greater), HbA1c, and detailed categories of smoking intensity (never smoker, former smoker <1 pack/day, former smoker 1 to <2 packs/day, former smoker 2+ packs/day, smoker <1 pack/day, current smoker 1 to <2 packs/day, or current smoker 2+ packs/day).

Regression models are implemented under various sample-restriction criteria designed to successively attenuate reverse-causation bias. These measures include delaying onset of exposure by 3 years to reduce the influence of those who were ill at baseline and who may have lost weight as a consequence; eliminating persons with a prior diagnosis of emphysema or a smoking-related cancer; and excluding persons who have lost significant weight in the 10-year period before the survey. The category of smoking-related cancers is based on relative risks in a recent large study37 and includes cancers of the lung, larynx, mouth/tongue/lip, esophagus, bladder, kidney, and pancreas. Weight loss was assessed using BMI 10 years before baseline, based on recalled weight in combination with measured height at baseline. To reduce bias that may result from inaccurate recall of past weight, we applied a correction factor based on the proportionate error between measured and self-reported BMI at baseline. The threshold for exclusion was 2.2 BMI units.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Demonstration of Greater Potential Biases Among the Dysglycemic Population

Table 1 demonstrates differences in the joint distribution of obesity and smoking status between the source population and the dysglycemic population. Values are age- and sex-standardized to the US 2000 census using 5-year age groups between ages 35 to 74 years.

TABLE 1.
TABLE 1.
Image Tools

The table shows that the joint distribution of smoking and obesity is quite different in the dysglycemic population from that in the US population as a whole. In the source population, the ratio of the odds of being a current smoker among nonobese persons to that among obese persons is 1.58. Among those with dysglycemia, the odds ratio increases to 2.88. Clearly, the inverse association between smoking and obesity has greatly strengthened in the dysglycemic population, confirming hypothesis (1) and the operation of condition (5). Such strengthening also occurs for the categories of heavy smoker and long-duration smoker. This stronger inverse association between smoking and obesity increases the risk of statistical confounding in the dysglycemic population relative to the risk in the source population.38

Table 2 investigates reverse causation, the second source of bias in studies of the mortality risks of obesity. Persons with dysglycemia have higher rates of illness than the source population, providing support for hypothesis (2). Higher rates of illness are observed not only for dysglycemia itself but also for emphysema, heart attacks, strokes, and self-rated health. The only exception is cancers that are not smoking-related. The greater extent of illness in the dysglycemic population should produce more weight loss than that in the source population. Using retrospectively reported data on height and weight 10 years before baseline, we define substantial weight loss as the top 50th percentile of weight loss in the dysglycemic population during this period. This criterion translates into a loss of 2.2 BMI units. Using this criterion, 13% of the dysglycemic population lost weight compared with 8% of the source population. To the extent that weight loss is a signal of disease, the greater extent of weight loss among persons with dysglycemia increases the risk of reverse causation relative to the risk in the source population.

TABLE 2.
TABLE 2.
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Effect of Biases on Excess Mortality of Overweight/Obesity Among Persons with Dysglycemia

Table 3 demonstrates that the obesity paradox is present among the dysglycemic population. On the basis of the age-standardized death rates, the relative risk of death for normal-weight people compared with that for overweight/obese people is 1.31 (95% confidence interval [CI] = 1.05–1.58). Next, we divide the dysglycemic population into ever-smokers and never-smokers. The table shows that the obesity paradox is strengthened among ever-smokers, with relative risks of death among normal-weight people rising to 1.51 (1.17–1.84). However, the obesity paradox is eliminated among never-smokers, among whom people of normal weight experience a lower death rate compared with that in overweight/obese people (relative risk = 0.66 [95% CI = 0.41–0.91]).

TABLE 3.
TABLE 3.
Image Tools

Table 3 controls only age. In Table 4, we introduce Cox proportional hazards models with various model specifications (varying across the rows) and various exclusions designed to reduce the impact of reverse causation (varying down the columns). The main independent variable in each regression model is a dichotomous indicator of BMI status (normal weight vs. overweight/obese). Model 1 examines the risk of mortality as a function of BMI status, adjusting for sex, race/ethnicity, education, and HbA1c; Model 2 additionally adjusts for detailed smoking intensity; and Model 3 introduces an interaction term between normal weight and an indicator for ever-smoking (former and current smoker).

TABLE 4.
TABLE 4.
Image Tools

Using all data and before introducing variables related to smoking, mortality among normal-weight people is substantially higher than that among overweight/obese people (hazard ratio [HR] = 1.28 [95% CI = 1.08–1.52]). When smoking status is introduced in Model 2, the excess risk associated with normal weight declines to 13% (HR = 1.13 [0.93–1.37]).

The third model introduces an interaction term between ever-smoking and weight status. A substantial effect modification emerges: among never-smokers, the HR associated with normal weight becomes 0.69 (95% CI = 0.45–1.06). This result is consistent with hypothesis (4). Among ever-smokers, however, relative mortality is much higher among normal-weight persons (HR = 1.33 [95% CI = 1.07–1.66]). If we did not control for smoking history and intensity, the HR for the nonobese relative to the HR for obese among smokers would be even higher at 1.59.

We now introduce the restrictions aimed at reducing the intensity of reverse causal pathways. The restrictions are cumulative. In row 2, we exclude exposure time in the first 3 years after baseline. In row 3, we additionally exclude people with emphysema and smoking-related cancers. In row 4, we additionally exclude those in the top 50th percentile of weight loss during the 10 years before baseline.

After these restrictions are imposed in Model 2, normal-weight people have 16% lower mortality compare with that in overweight/obese people (HR = 0.84 [95% CI = 0.60–1.18]). Results from the third model are especially revealing and underscore the importance of smoking-associated reverse causation. The introduction of successive restrictions designed to reduce the impact of reverse causation has a minor effect on the HR of normal-weight nonsmokers, which declines from 0.69 to 0.61. However, it has a larger effect on the relative risk of normal-weight smokers (HR declines from 1.33 to 0.95). The larger effect of these exclusions among smokers than among never-smokers suggests that, as expected, reverse causation is a more common source of bias among smokers than among nonsmokers.

Note that coefficients in Model 3 in Table 4 were estimated using an interaction term between smoking status and obesity. If we had instead stratified the sample into ever-smokers and never-smokers, results would be virtually identical. The HR for nonobesity among never-smokers would be 0.69 instead of 0.68, and for ever-smokers it would be identical at 1.33.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Sensitivity Analyses

We have performed 3 additional analyses to test the robustness of our conclusions (eAppendix; http://links.lww.com/EDE/A769). In the first, we have tightened the criterion for being dysglycemic from a reading of 5.7% to 6.0%. The second sensitivity analysis removes those in the overweight range (BMI between 25 and 30 kg/m2) from the overweight/obese group. These two sensitivity analyses appear in eTable 1 ( http://links.lww.com/EDE/A768). Third, we have repeated all analyses using data from the National Health Interview Survey (eTables 2–5; http://links.lww.com/EDE/A768). In all cases, basic results are unchanged: an obesity paradox is present before statistical controls are imposed, it is eliminated among never-smokers, and it is attenuated among ever-smokers by the introduction of restrictions designed to reduce reverse causation. Using data from NHIS rather than NHANES, we show that conditioning on dysglycemia enhances the risk of bias from improper control of smoking and from reverse causation (eTables 2–3; http://links.lww.com/EDE/A768).

Back to Top | Article Outline

DISCUSSION

The threat of downward biases in estimating the mortality risks of obesity is widely discussed. That the threats are magnified in studies of populations diagnosed with a disease has received little attention. Banack and Kaufman31 were among the first to suggest that the obesity paradox may be attributable to collider bias. They hypothesize that a collision between obesity and “unmeasured risk factors” occurs in the state of heart failure. At least in the case of diabetes (and we suspect elsewhere), the salient additional risk factor seems to be smoking. We demonstrate both theoretically and empirically that the inverse association between smoking and obesity is strengthened by conditioning on diabetes. We also demonstrate that a variety of forms of illness, as well as weight loss, are more prevalent among persons with dysglycemia, raising the threat of reverse causation.

In multivariate analyses, we identify an obesity paradox for people with dysglycemia. The paradox is attenuated when smoking behavior is controlled, and it is eliminated completely among those who have never smoked. Among smokers, successive restrictions designed to reduce the strength of reverse causal pathways eliminate the obesity paradox. This evidence supports the hypothesis that illness-associated weight loss is more prevalent—and hence a greater source of bias—among smokers than among never-smokers.

Our study has some limitations. One is the relatively small number of persons with dyglycemia in our NHANES study population. Another is that, to construct our weight-change variable, we used recall data on weight to calculate BMI 10 years before baseline. This may have introduced error. However, validation studies of 10-year weight recall support their validity for use in epidemiologic studies.39 An additional limitation is that, apart from the mortality follow-up, data are cross-sectional and do not permit sequential analysis of intervening pathways of disease, recovery, and weight change that would help illuminate the sources of the obesity paradox.

Clinical recommendations depend on whether the obesity paradox is a product of real biological processes or is produced by statistical biases. In this regard, it is noteworthy that never-smokers do not exhibit an obesity paradox in any of the specifications we have considered, including sensitivity analyses. The absence of a paradox among never-smokers implies that the paradox is not a product of physiological protection afforded by a high BMI unless that protection itself is somehow invoked by smoking. No plausible biological mechanisms have been offered for such a relation. We conclude that the apparent protective effect of obesity among persons with diabetes and prediabetes is not causal.

Back to Top | Article Outline

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We are grateful to Jere Behrman, Virginia Chang, Irma Elo, Douglas Ewbank, Neil Mehta, Sari Stenholm, and Lucia Tiererova, for helpful suggestions.

Back to Top | Article Outline

REFERENCES

1. Carnethon MR, De Chavez PJ, Biggs ML, et al. Association of weight status with mortality in adults with incident diabetes. JAMA. 2012; 308:581–590

2. Doehner W, Erdmann E, Cairns R, et al. Inverse relation of body weight and weight change with mortality and morbidity in patients with type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular co-morbidity: an analysis of the PROactive study population. Int J Cardiol. 2012; 162:20–26

3. Jackson CL, Yeh HC, Szklo M, et al. Body-mass index and all-cause mortality in US adults with and without diabetes. J Gen Intern Med. 2014; 29:25–33

4. Romero-Corral A, Montori VM, Somers VK, et al. Association of bodyweight with total mortality and with cardiovascular events in coronary artery disease: a systematic review of cohort studies. Lancet. 2006; 368:666–678

5. Lavie CJ, Milani RV, Ventura HO. Obesity and cardiovascular disease: risk factor, paradox, and impact of weight loss. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2009; 53:1925–1932

6. Oreopoulos A, Padwal R, Kalantar-Zadeh K, Fonarow GC, Norris CM, McAlister FA. Body mass index and mortality in heart failure: a meta-analysis. Am Heart J. 2008; 156:13–22

7. Carman WJ, Barrett-Connor E, Sowers M, Khaw KT. Higher risk of cardiovascular mortality among lean hypertensive individuals in Tecumseh, Michigan. Circulation. 1994; 89:703–711

8. Landbo C, Prescott E, Lange P, Vestbo J, Almdal TP. Prognostic value of nutritional status in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1999; 160:1856–1861

9. Attaran S, McShane J, Whittle I, Poullis M, Shackcloth M. A propensity-matched comparison of survival after lung resection in patients with a high versus low body mass index. Eur J Cardiothorac Surg. 2012; 42:653–658

10. Yoon HH, Lewis MA, Shi Q, et al. Prognostic impact of body mass index stratified by smoking status in patients with esophageal adenocarcinoma. J Clin Oncol. 2011; 29:4561–4567

11. Bucholz EM, Rathore SS, Reid KJ, et al. Body mass index and mortality in acute myocardial infarction patients. Am J Med. 2012; 125:796–803

12. Oreopoulos A, Padwal R, Norris CM, Mullen JC, Pretorius V, Kalantar-Zadeh K. Effect of obesity on short- and long-term mortality postcoronary revascularization: a meta-analysis. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2008; 16:442–450

13. Angerås O, Albertsson P, Karason K, et al. Evidence for obesity paradox in patients with acute coronary syndromes: a report from the Swedish Coronary Angiography and Angioplasty Registry. Eur Heart J. 2013; 34:345–353

14. Schmidt DS, Salahudeen AK. Obesity-survival paradox-still a controversy? Semin Dial. 2007; 20:486–492

15. Whitlock G, Lewington S, Sherliker P. Prospective Studies Collaboration Body-mass index and cause-specific mortality in 900 000 adults: collaborative analyses of 57 prospective studies. Lancet. 2009; 373:1083–1096

16. Hu F. Hu FB. Obesity and mortality. Obesity Epidemiology. 2008; New York, NY Oxford University Press 216–233

17. Chiolero A, Faeh D, Paccaud F, Cornuz J. Consequences of smoking for body weight, body fat distribution, and insulin resistance. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008; 87:801–809

18. Stram DO, Huberman M, Wu AH. Is residual confounding a reasonable explanation for the apparent protective effects of beta-carotene found in epidemiologic studies of lung cancer in smokers? Am J Epidemiol. 2002; 155:622–628

19. Renehan AG, Leitzmann MF, Zwahlen M. Re: body mass index and risk of lung cancer among never, former, and current smokers. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2012; 104:1680–1681

20. Durazo-Arvizu RA, Cooper RS. Issues related to modeling the body mass index-mortality association: the shape of the association and the effects of smoking status. Int J Obes (Lond). 2008; 32:(suppl 3)S52–S55

21. Cooper RS. Which factors confound or modify the relationship between body weight and mortality? Int J Obes (Lond). 2008; 32:(suppl 3)S47–S51

22. Habbu A, Lakkis NM, Dokainish H. The obesity paradox: fact or fiction? Am J Cardiol. 2006; 98:944–948

23. Pi-Sunyer FX. Weight loss and mortality in type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2000; 23:1451–1452

24. Braun TP, Zhu X, Szumowski M, et al. Central nervous system inflammation induces muscle atrophy via activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. J Exp Med. 2011; 208:2449–2463

25. Morley JE, Thomas DR, Wilson MM. Cachexia: pathophysiology and clinical relevance. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006; 83:735–743

26. Flegal KM, Graubard BI, Williamson DF, Cooper RS. Reverse causation and illness-related weight loss in observational studies of body weight and mortality. Am J Epidemiol. 2011; 173:1–9

27. Ferreira I, Stehouwer CD. Obesity paradox or inappropriate study designs? Time for life-course epidemiology. J Hypertens. 2012; 30:2271–2275

28. Dahabreh IJ, Kent DM. Index event bias as an explanation for the paradoxes of recurrence risk research. JAMA. 2011; 305:822–823

29. Hernán MA, Hernández-Díaz S, Robins JM. A structural approach to selection bias. Epidemiology. 2004; 15:615–625

30. Cole SR, Platt RW, Schisterman EF, et al. Illustrating bias due to conditioning on a collider. Int J Epidemiol. 2010; 39:417–420

31. Banack HR, Kaufman JS. The “obesity paradox” explained. Epidemiology. 2013; 24:461–462

32. Gregg EW, Gerzoff RB, Thompson TJ, Williamson DF. Trying to lose weight, losing weight, and 9-year mortality in overweight U.S. adults with diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2004; 27:657–662

33. Willi C, Bodenmann P, Ghali WA, Faris PD, Cornuz J. Active smoking and the risk of type 2 diabetes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA. 2007; 298:2654–2664

34. National Center for Health Statistics; Office of Analysis and pidemiology The National Health Interview Survey (1986–2004) Linked Mortality Files, Mortality Follow-up Through 2006: Matching Methodology, May 2009. 2009; Hyattsville, MD

Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/datalinkage/matching_methodology_nhis_final.pdf.


35. Kirkman MS, Kendall DM. Hemoglobin A1c to diagnose diabetes: why the controversy over adding a new tool? Clin Chem. 2011; 57:255–257

36. American Diabetes Association Diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care. 2010; 33:(suppl 1)S62–S69

37. Pirie K, Peto R, Reeves GK, Green J, Beral V. The 21st century hazards of smoking and benefits of stopping: a prospective study of one million women in the UK. Lancet. 2012; 6736:1–9

38. Fewell Z, Davey Smith G, Sterne JA. The impact of residual and unmeasured confounding in epidemiologic studies: a simulation study. Am J Epidemiol. 2007; 166:646–655

39. Perry GS, Byers TE, Mokdad AH, Serdula MK, Williamson DF. The validity of self-reports of past body weights by U.S. adults. Epidemiology. 1995; 6:61–66

Back to Top | Article Outline

Supplemental Digital Content

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-Non Commercial-No Derivatives 3.0 License, where it is permissible to download and share the work provided it is properly cited. The work cannot be changed in any way or used commercially.

Copyright © 2014 by Lippincott Williams & Wilkins

Twitter  Facebook

Login

Article Tools

Images

Share