Skip Navigation LinksHome > November 2000 - Volume 11 - Issue 6 > A Pooled Analysis of Magnetic Fields, Wire Codes, and Childh...
Original Articles

A Pooled Analysis of Magnetic Fields, Wire Codes, and Childhood Leukemia

Greenland, Sander1; Sheppard, Asher R.2; Kaune, William T.3; Poole, Charles4; Kelsh, Michael A.5; for the Childhood Leukemia-EMF Study Group

Free Access
Article Outline
Collapse Box

Author Information

From the 1Department of Epidemiology, UCLA School of Public Health, Los Angeles; 2Asher Sheppard Consulting, Redlands, and Department of Physiology, Loma Linda University, CA; 3EM Factors, Richland, WA; 4Department of Epidemiology, University of North Carolina School of Public Health, Chapel Hill, NC; and 5Exponent Health Group, Menlo Park, CA.

This research was supported by Grant R03 ES-08920 from the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences. Southern California Edison Co. supported previous work that contributed to this report.

Submitted September 13, 1999; final version accepted May 30, 2000.

Address correspondence to: Asher Sheppard Consulting, 108 Orange Street, Suite 8, Redlands, CA 92373-4719.

The Childhood Leukemia-EMF Study Group consists of the above authors; data contributors: A. Ahlbom and M. Feychting (Karolinska Institute), R. Coghill (Coghill Research Laboratories), Electric Power Research Institute (EPRI) (magnetic field survey data), J. Dockerty (University of Otago), A. Fajardo-Gutiérrez (Centro Médico Nacional Siglo XXI), J. Fulton (Rhode Island Department of Health), M. Koskenvuo (University of Turku), M. Linet (National Cancer Institute), S. London (National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences), M. McBride (British Columbia Cancer Agency), J. Michaelis (Johannes Gutenberg-University of Mainz), J. Olsen (Danish Cancer Society), J. Peters (University of Southern California), E. Pukkala (Finnish Cancer Registry), D. Savitz (University of North Carolina), J. Schüz (Johannes Gutenberg-University of Mainz), L. Tomenius, T. Tynes (Norwegian Radiation Protection Authority), P. Verkasalo (University of Helsinki), and N. Wertheimer (University of Colorado); and the Pooled Database Assembly Team, which included R. Mrad, B. Smith, and K. Zhao (Exponent Health Group), and M. Atherton (EcoAnalysis, Inc.).

Collapse Box


We obtained original individual data from 15 studies of magnetic fields or wire codes and childhood leukemia, and we estimated magnetic field exposure for subjects with sufficient data to do so. Summary estimates from 12 studies that supplied magnetic field measures exhibited little or no association of magnetic fields with leukemia when comparing 0.1–0.2 and 0.2–0.3 microtesla (μT) categories with the 0–0.1 μT category, but the Mantel-Haenszel summary odds ratio comparing >0.3 μT to 0–0.1 μT was 1.7 (95% confidence limits = 1.2, 2.3). Similar results were obtained using covariate adjustment and spline regression. The study-specific relations appeared consistent despite the numerous methodologic differences among the studies. The association of wire codes with leukemia varied considerably across studies, with odds ratio estimates for very high current vs low current configurations ranging from 0.7 to 3.0 (homogeneity P = 0.005). Based on a survey of household magnetic fields, an estimate of the U.S. population attributable fraction of childhood leukemia associated with residential exposure is 3% (95% confidence limits = –2%, 8%). Our results contradict the idea that the magnetic field association with leukemia is less consistent than the wire code association with leukemia, although analysis of the four studies with both measures indicates that the wire code association is not explained by measured fields. The results also suggest that appreciable magnetic field effects, if any, may be concentrated among relatively high and uncommon exposures, and that studies of highly exposed populations would be needed to clarify the relation of magnetic fields to childhood leukemia.

The question of health effects of extremely low-frequency electromagnetic fields (EMFs) remains an unsettled topic. 1 The National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences funded our research team to conduct a pooled analysis of those studies of EMF and childhood leukemia for which original data could be obtained. We felt that a direct analysis of individual study data would allow a more reliable evaluation of interstudy differences in results (heterogeneity). It also could allow more reliable evaluation of dose-response relations and effects on public health than could a combination of summaries from studies, especially in light of the very different analyses presented in the published reports. The present paper reports our analyses.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Subjects and Methods


From literature searches, we identified 24 studies 2–25 that presented data on household EMF or power-supply wiring information and childhood leukemia. To be eligible for inclusion in our pooled analysis, the study had to have obtained quantitative magnetic field measures for individual subjects or enough information to approximate Wertheimer-Leeper wire codes. 1 Nineteen studies 2–16,22–25 had eligible data. Five articles reporting four studies 22–26 appeared after our initial search in 1998; investigators in two of those studies 22,23 supplied data in time for inclusion here. One study group 16 refused our data request. Two studies 8,15 were conducted using identical methods within the same country and treated as one study. 8 Fulton et al5 and Tomenius 11 published analyses that used residence as the analysis unit, but we used individual-level exposures from their data. We thus had anonymous records on individual subjects from 15 distinct studies.

Table 1 summarizes the studies included here. All are case-control studies. Verkasalo et al13 initially conducted and reported a person-time cohort study. They supplied data from an unpublished case-control study nested within their cohort, based on all cases observed in the cohort plus ten controls for each case, and which obtained additional covariate data; the controls were age-sex matched but otherwise randomly sampled from the cohort. The two Swedish studies 4,11 had a small overlap in source populations and so share a few cases, but this overlap could not be identified from the available data. Most studies had geographic restrictions on their source populations beyond those shown in Table 1; some had restrictions to areas near or crossed by high-voltage lines. 4,12,13

Table 1
Table 1
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Primary Measures

Twelve studies supplied magnetic field exposure estimates for some or all individuals. For four Nordic studies, 4,9,12,13 we used estimates calculated by theoriginal investigators from measured proximity to power lines and historical current-supply data. For eight studies, 2,6–8,10,11,22,23 we used estimates based on direct measurements (measured magnetic fields at the front door of the residence, 11 measured fields in the child’s bedroom, 2,7,8,23 averaged fields in several rooms, 6,10 and averaged personal and house measures 22).

Some studies 4,6–8,10,22 supplied more than one type of magnetic field measurement. For example, there were normal- or low-power measurements, spot and 24-hour measurements, mean and median values, data from the residence at diagnosis, and data from other residences. There is as yet no measure of magnetic field exposure that is known to be biologically the most relevant. In the absence of such knowledge, it would be best to examine a number of different measures. This was indeed done in several studies, but it raises multiplicity problems that are difficult to deal with statistically in even a single study. For a pooled analysis of the studies here, there would be more than 100 combinations of measures (although we did not have all measures for all of the studies).

To avoid multiplicity issues and to keep our task manageable, we defined our target measure to be a child’s time-weighted average exposure up to 3 months before diagnosis. When we had several measures from a study, we used a measure that, based on earlier work, 27–29 seemed likely to provide the best approximation to this target. In particular, we preferred calculated historical fields or averages of multiple measurements rather than spot measurements when there was a choice. Table 2 summarizes the measurements used from each study. We also conducted analyses of each supplied measure and a limited sensitivity analysis of summaries based on revisions of initial choices.

Table 2
Table 2
Image Tools

All North American studies 3,5–7,10,14,22,24 obtained wire code data. Wire codes from two studies 5,14 were recalculated from original data on distances to type of distribution line. Wire codes from one study 3 were in a unique three-level form.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Other Information

Studies varied considerably in the covariates available for control and in their completeness of exposure and covariate information. One study 11 supplied no covariate data and so was excluded from covariate-adjusted analyses. Several studies 4–9,12,22,23 supplied at least one socioeconomic variable on some or all subjects. One important ecologic covariate available for all studies was location; studies in North America involved 60-Hz fields with 110–125-V home supply, whereas all other studies involved 50-Hz fields with 220–240-V power. Thus, all comparisons of 60-Hz vs 50-Hz fields are also comparisons of 110–125-V vs 220–240-V systems and of North America vs other locations.

There are several discrepancies between the data we report and those in some published reports. 6,7,10,14,22,23 Some differences arose because we did not impose exclusion criteria used by certain authors. For example, we included ten Down-syndrome subjects excluded by Linet et al6 because we could not identify such subjects in other studies and we could not identify any bias that would justify such an exclusion. Other differences arose from postpublication corrections or additions to the study data by the original investigators and from our use of exposure measures and cutpoints different from those used in the original publications; these differences led to especially large upward changes for the Tomenius 11 and McBride et al22 estimates. A few small discrepancies were unresolved; no such discrepancy appeared capable of producing more than negligible differences in summary results.

Coghill et al2 and Linet et al6 restricted their cases to acute lymphoblastic leukemia (ALL). Because about 80% of childhood leukemias are ALL, and because not all datasets distinguished leukemia subtypes, we conducted no analysis restricted to ALL.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Statistical Methods

Data were analyzed using inverse-variance weighted (Woolf), Mantel-Haenszel, and maximum-likelihood (ML) tabular methods, and using ML logistic regression. 30,31 (Inverse variance methods were included because they are common in meta-analysis.) All P-values were derived from score statistics or deviance (log likelihood-ratio) statistics. 30 For magnetic field exposures, dose response was examined using category indicators and splines in logistic models. 31–33 All results were adjusted for study: tabular analyses were always stratified on study, and all regressions included indicators for study.

All magnetic field measurements were converted into units of microtesla (μT). Only two studies 6,7 had more than four cases above 0.4 μT; therefore, for categorical magnetic field analyses, values above 0.3 μT were combined in a single category to ensure cell counts large enough for all statistical procedures. To avoid the trend distortions and power loss associated with percentile-category boundaries, 33,34 we used equally spaced boundaries below the 0.3-μT cutpoint. We combined low-exposure wire codes (UG = underground, VLCC = very low current code, and OLCC = ordinary low current code) into a single “LCC” low-current reference category for comparison with the two high-exposure wire codes (OHCC = ordinary high current, and VHCC = very high current). Previous results indicate that the three low-current categories do not correspond to meaningful differences in EMF exposure or childhood leukemia risk. 3,5–7,10,22,24,35 Furthermore, in three studies, 5,6,14 the proportions of subjects with a UG or VLCC code were too small to yield efficient estimates using those codes as reference category; in another, 6 UG and VLCC were combined in the supplied data; and in another, 3 low-current codes had been combined in data collection.

Complications arose in accounting for the variety of matching protocols used. Most studies matched on certain covariates (typically sex, age or birth date, and some sort of geographic unit). Many studies experienced some failures to match, leading to fewer subjects available for matched analyses than unmatched analyses. Several considerations led us to focus on unmatched analyses with analytic control for matched covariates. First, this choice provided the most subjects for analysis. Second, this choice avoided further efficiency loss due to the type of analysis overmatching documented by Brookmeyer et al.36 Third, this choice also helped avoid small-sample bias away from the null due to sparse matched-set counts in study-specific analyses 37; although we would expect the unmatched analyses to suffer some small bias toward the null, we thought this possibility preferable to a potentially large bias away from the null due to sparse data. Fourth, results from matched analyses were less stable but exhibited the same patterns seen in the unmatched analyses.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Results for Magnetic Fields

Categorical Analyses

Table 3 displays the distribution of magnetic field measurements among the studies supplying such measurements. There are extensive differences among the studies, ranging from Olsen et al9 (which has only 0.5% of cases and controls above 0.1 μT) to Linet et al6 (which has more than one-third of measured subjects above 0.1 μT). Values above 0.3 μT are relatively infrequent in all studies. The differences appear associated chiefly with location rather than with measurement method (direct vs calculated). Distributions in North American studies tend to be much higher than those in European studies, probably reflecting differences in power systems (for example, more overhead wires and lower household voltage in North America), per capita electricity consumption, 38 and grounding practices. The higher distribution in Feychting and Ahlbom 4 compared with the other Nordic studies reflects the fact that the source population was restricted to children dwelling within 300 meters of high-voltage lines 4 (although Verkasalo et al13 imposed a 500-meter limit and Tomenius 11 restricted subjects to census wards with transmission lines).

Table 3
Table 3
Image Tools

Table 4 displays odds ratio estimates computed directly from the raw counts underlying Table 3 and summary estimates assuming common odds ratios for each analysis category. The study-specific and summary estimates tend to show little or no association of fields below 0.3 μT with leukemia, but all studies with cases and controls in the >0.3 μT category exhibit positive associations for that category. The differences across studies were within chance variation (deviance P = 0.42 using exposure categories in Table 4), as were differences between studies with different measures [ML odds ratios for >0.3 μT = 1.70 from studies with calculated fields and 1.68 from studies with direct measurement; 95% confidence limits (95% CL) for ratio of odds ratios = 0.46, 2.22] or different field frequencies (ML odds ratios for >0.3 μT = 1.97 from studies with 50-Hz fields and 1.58 from studies with 60-Hz fields; 95% CL for ratio of odds ratios = 0.66, 2.36).

Table 4
Table 4
Image Tools

The Tomenius data 11 included no covariate and so were excluded from covariate-adjusted analyses. The penultimate line of Table 4 shows the age-sex-study-adjusted Mantel-Haenszel estimates. The exclusions and adjustments had negligible effect, and odds ratio differences across age and sex categories (not shown) were within chance variation. Table 5 summarizes categorical analyses upon restriction to subjects with no missing data. Neither restriction nor adjustment for available covariates changed the qualitative result that there was little or no association evident below 0.2 μT, but some positive association was evident above 0.3 μT.

Table 5
Table 5
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Trend Analysis

The final line of Table 4 displays estimated odds ratios from a logistic model fit to individual-level magnetic field data using a quadratic spline for field along with age, squared age, and sex terms. The spline has a single knot at 0.2 μT (the middle category boundary) and so has one linear and two quadratic magnetic field terms; the model thus uses 3 degrees of freedom for field, the same number of degrees of freedom as in the four-category analysis. The spline estimate under each category is the leukemia odds ratio comparing the mean field measure in that category with the mean field measure in the ≤0.1 μT category and is thus a continuous-data analogue of the categorical summary estimate. Unlike the categorical analysis, the spline analysis imposes a smooth dose-response relation between field level and leukemia. Nonetheless, the spline results are similar to the categorical results: there appears to be little or no association below 0.2 μT but some association comparing high with low exposures; furthermore, differences among covariate-specific curves (not shown) were within chance variation.

Figure 1 displays a graph of the “floated” case-control ratios 39 fit by the spline model, along with pointwise confidence limits. This figure is a plot of the fitted odds of being a case vs being a control in our studies. Assuming these odds are proportional to the underlying childhood leukemia rates, this plot is an estimate of the shape of the curve relating leukemia rates to magnetic fields under the spline model. 39 The vertical axis corresponds to geometric mean case-control ratios rather than to odds ratios, but ratios of different points on the curve equal the model-fitted odds ratios 39; for example, the ratio of the curve heights at 0.58 and 0.02 μT (the means of the >0.3 and ≤0.1 μT categories) is 1.65, equal to the final odds ratio in Table 4. We caution against focusing on the central curve, however, because the data are compatible with a wide range of trends, including nonmonotonic, linear, and exponentially increasing shapes. For example, the strictly increasing trend above 0.1 μT is not a statistically stable feature, in that curves that plateau or even decline above 0.6 μT also fit the data well.

Figure 1
Figure 1
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Influence and Sensitivity Analyses

As with covariate adjustment, neither single-study deletions nor alternative choices for the exposure measure altered results qualitatively, nor did deletion of large field values (for example, the five subjects above 2.0 μT, all controls from Tynes and Haldorsen 12). Although the highest-category estimates and the fitted curve varied considerably with category-boundary and model choices, these choices also did not alter the basic qualitative results.

Use of alternatives among the supplied exposure measures produced only small differences in the summaries; we did not have all measures from all studies, however. Missing data varied with choice of measure, and this variation sometimes had more influence on estimates than the choice of measure. Two studies 4,13 supplied calculated yearly exposure of children; we used these data to construct alternative-exposure measures that might arguably approximate more closely our target than the measures used in the original reports and in our analysis above. Use of these alternatives had little effect on the study-specific odds ratios below 0.3 μT but raised the >0.3-vs-≤0.1 odds ratio to 5.9 (95% CL = 2.0, 17) for Feychting and Ahlbom 4 and to 10 (95% CL = 1.4, 74) for Verkasalo et al.13 Some of this increase may only be increased small-sample bias 37 due to reduction in numbers above 0.3 μT. In any event, use of these alternatives changed the summaries by only a few percent.

The calculated-field measures from the Nordic studies were based on high-voltage lines and did not include contributions from sources such as in-home wiring and appliances. 4,9,12,13 The effect of the latter omissions is not straightforward to assess, because fields are vector additive and so may even destructively interfere with one another, depending on the relative orientation and phase of the contributions from different sources. One study 4 supplied spot measurements as well as calculated fields on 24 of 38 cases and 344 of 554 controls. These dual measurements permitted instrumental-variable corrections 40 for estimates from the calculated fields in the Nordic studies. Because these corrections involve strict assumptions and require extensive technical description, 40 they were not used in Tables 3 and 5, and we omit details. The main result was that odds ratio estimates from the Nordic studies 4,9,12,13 were corrected toward the null. Nonetheless, because these studies contributed so few cases at the higher exposure levels, the corrections had only a small effect on the overall summary estimates.

The dip in the curve in Figure 1 below 0.1 μT is mostly attributable to the Danish data, 9 in which exposures below 0.1 μT were effectively set to 0 when calculating averages, and which contributed about one-quarter of the subjects in the ≤0.1 μT category. When this study was deleted, the dip disappeared, but the curve remained mildly sigmoidal.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Noncontributing Studies

Myers et al16 reported only one case and two controls for “non-solid tumors” above 0.1 μT; exclusion of this study could not have influenced our results to an important degree. Most of the data from the much larger study by Green et al24 were neither presented in categories that could be combined directly with our categories nor broken into analysis categories above 0.15 μT; the estimates in this study varied considerably with the measure and adjustment used, but all had wide confidence intervals and were statistically compatible with our results. Crude data from a personal-monitoring substudy by Green et al25 produced odds ratios of 1.20 (95% CL = 0.59, 2.41), 1.76 (95% CL = 0.82, 3.80), and 0.71 (95% CL = 0.18, 2.88) comparing 0.1–0.2, 0.2–0.3, and >0.3 μT with ≤0.1 μT, reflecting the small numbers in this substudy. The U.K. Childhood Cancer Study group 26 reported birthdate-sex-socioeconomic status-adjusted odds ratios for total leukemia of 0.78 (95% CL = 0.55, 1.12), 0.78 (95% CL = 0.40, 1.52), and 1.68 (95% CL = 0.40, 7.10) comparing categories of 0.1–0.2, 0.2–0.4, and >0.4 μT with ≤0.1 μT; our pooled data yielded age-sex-study-adjusted ML estimates of 1.01 (95% CL = 0.84, 1.21), 1.25 (95% CL = 0.96, 1.61), and 1.60 (95% CL = 1.03, 2.48) using the same categories.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Attributable-Fraction Analysis

We estimated the excess fraction of U.S. childhood leukemia incidence that would be attributable to magnetic field exposures above 0.05 μT, under the assumption that the dose-response estimate in Figure 1 represents the causal effects of fields. To estimate the U.S. population distribution of field exposure, we used data from a utility-based cluster-sampled survey conducted by the Electric Power Research Institute (EPRI). 41 The data we obtained (Table 6) comprised spot field measurements averaged across rooms within each of 987 homes sampled from residences served by 301 EPRI utilities, which together served about 67% of U.S. homes. 41

Table 6
Table 6
Image Tools

When these data were combined with the spline function in Figure 1 using a model-based attributable-fraction formula, 42 we obtained a population attributable-fraction estimate of 3% for the effect of magnetic fields greater than 0.05 μT (95% CL = −2%, 8%). The estimate is nearly the same if one uses any reference level up to 0.15 μT (rather than 0.05 μT), reflecting the fact that 90% of surveyed homes are in the 0–0.2 μT range, in which the fitted ratios exhibit little variation. The wide confidence interval reflects the uncertainty about the distribution of exposure, as well as the considerable uncertainty about dose response. We further caution that our estimate refers only to effects of ambient residential fields and excludes effects of unmeasured personal field sources such as electric blankets.

We did not have survey data for Europe, but given the low Northern European exposures seen in Table 3, we would expect a correspondingly lower attributable-fraction estimate for Northern Europe.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Results for Wire Codes

Table 7 displays the distribution of wire codes among the studies supplying such codes, as well as data from Table V of Green et al.24 As with fields, there are extensive differences among the studies, ranging within the U.S. from 15% with OHCC or VHCC codes in Linet et al6 to nearly 50% with those codes in London et al.7 These differences reflect well-documented differences in power-grid architecture within the United States. 1,41

Table 7
Table 7
Image Tools

Table 8 displays odds ratio estimates computed directly from the raw counts underlying Table 7, and the corresponding covariate-adjusted estimates. Summary estimates are omitted because of the extensive unexplained heterogeneity among the study-specific results; for example, the VHCC odds ratios are less than 1 in three studies and more than 2 in three others (homogeneity P = 0.005). We found no covariate that accounted for the large variation in results, but deletion of Wertheimer and Leeper 14 increased the homogeneity P-value to 0.11; no other single-study deletion increased the homogeneity P-value above 0.04. Eliminating Wertheimer and Leeper 14 and Fulton et al5 (the two earliest studies) yielded summary ML odds ratios of 1.02 (95% CL = 0.87, 1.22) for OHCC and 1.50 (95% CL = 1.17, 1.92) for VHCC based on 1,457 cases and 1,962 controls from six studies 3,6,7,10,22,24 (deviance P = 0.005 for wire code; homogeneity P = 0.15).

Table 8
Table 8
Image Tools

As with fields, confounder adjustment had little effect on the wire code results beyond reducing the number of subjects, resulting in less stable estimates and more pronounced heterogeneity. For example, adjustment changed the Savitz et al10 estimate of the VHCC odds ratio from 2.6 (95% CL = 0.92, 7.5) to 3.8 (95% CL = 1.2, 12); this change was entirely due to the deletion of 15 cases and 23 controls without covariate data. Adjusted results in our three-level format could not be computed from Green et al, 24 but their own adjustment produced little change in their estimates. 24 Fajardo-Gutiérrez supplied additional data on wiring configurations that allowed one of us (W. T. K.) to construct an alternative approximation to the Wertheimer-Leeper wire code in this study. 3 This alternative coding produced OHCC and VHCC (vs LCC) odds ratios of 1.5 (95% CL = 0.80, 2.9) and 1.2 (95% CL = 0.80, 1.9), which appear less consistent with other studies than the odds ratios from the original coding (Table 8).

Four studies 6,7,10,22 recorded both magnetic fields and wire codes, allowing us to examine these exposures together (Table 9). Because these analyses involve only a fraction of all subjects and because fields and codes are strongly associated (mean fields of 0.09 for LCC, 0.13 for OHCC, and 0.19 for VHCC), the results are even more unstable. Nonetheless, the associations seen with fields and codes entered into the same model were similar to the associations seen with separate models for the measures.

Table 9
Table 9
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline


For brevity and on scientific grounds, we restricted this report to analyses specified as a priori relevant to the main study question: Are magnetic fields or wire codes consistently associated with childhood leukemia? Our prior restrictions were meant to avoid analyses that “capitalize on chance” (small numbers and unstable estimates) either to reinforce or refute a particular hypothesis. Such restrictions are especially important in dose-response analyses of magnetic fields because of suggestions that the entire topic of EMF research is a product of unconstrained data dredging. 43

Purely categorical dose-response analyses (that is, those conducted without regard to ordering, spacing, or smoothness constraints) can almost always be made to yield nonmonotone patterns by using categories small enough so that category-specific estimates become unstable. To avoid such problems, we supplemented our initial categorical analyses with smooth regression analyses (splines) rather than with smaller categories. We believe that dose-response modeling is important in the present context because, even upon pooling, there are still too few data to reject any plausible dose-response shape, especially above 0.2 μT. In particular, the data appear to be statistically consistent with anything from curves that are nearly flat to curves that rise and then fall at high exposures to curves that rise faster than exponentially.

We had planned to use available information to impute magnetic field values for subjects having only wire codes, on the basis of information relating codes to field measurements. 10,35 Nonetheless, because of the heterogeneity among wire code results and doubts about the accuracy of the imputation, we decided to forego those analyses.

One interesting result from our analysis is resolution of an apparent “wire code paradox.” It has been remarked that wire codes show more consistent associations with childhood cancers across studies than do magnetic fields. The paradoxical element arose in part from the presumption that wire codes were a proxy for fields and thus should show less consistent associations if fields have an effect. An examination of our tables suggests that, after allowing for statistical variability, wire codes in fact show less consistent associations with childhood leukemia than do magnetic fields. Nonetheless, adjustment for measured fields does not reduce the association of wire codes with childhood leukemia (Table 9). Perhaps only fields are biologically relevant, but errors in the field measures are so large that wire codes pick up much of the field effect; another possibility is that both measures only reflect effects of some biologically relevant exposure that is missing from our data.

One can of course raise many criticisms of the individual studies, which would increase the already large uncertainty in our results. For example, confounding effects of socioeconomic status, residential mobility, residence type, viral contacts, and traffic density have been raised as possible explanations for the observed associations. 44–51 These confounding hypotheses are themselves problematic. First, a confounding explanation requires the confounder to have an effect considerably larger than the observed association, as well as a strong association with exposure. 30 These attributes have not yet been demonstrated for the hypothesized confounders across the different populations that display positive associations. Adjustment for recorded socioeconomic and housing factors produced only small changes in the field-leukemia association, but our data on such factors are incomplete and we have only limited data on other potential confounders. Some results suggest that traffic-density effects may be large enough to partly explain the associations seen here. 44–47 We thus recommend that future studies obtain data on traffic density and ambient pollution levels, as well as details of socioeconomic status and residence history.

Biases due to measurement errors are undoubtedly present in and vary across all of the studies, but their assessment is not wholly straightforward. One problem is that there is no agreed-upon definition of the target exposure, although it is often thought of as some sort of average or cumulative exposure during some biologically relevant time before leukemia diagnosis. Only under fairly restrictive conditions 40,52 can one be certain that the net bias due to such error will be toward the null. Unfortunately, there is little or no evidence to establish such detailed attributes of the errors, and there is no basis for assuming such attributes are the same across studies and measures. For example, although some U.S. studies have found clear associations between fields measured at the front door, average magnetic fields in the home, and personal exposure to children 27,53 and another U.S. study found some repeatability of spot measures over extended time periods, 54 these associations are not large enough to ensure that the measures would tend to exhibit similar associations with childhood leukemia. Furthermore, the associations are imperfect enough to indicate that probably all of the measures suffer considerable error as proxies for any biologically relevant exposure measure (if one exists). One study suggested that electric rather than magnetic fields may be the relevant exposure. 2 Other studies conflict with this suggestion, however, insofar as the electric-field associations with childhood leukemia reported in those studies tended to be null or smaller than the reported magnetic-field associations. 7,10,23,25

Selection biases may be present in the studies, but for most there is little evidence that would establish their magnitude or even their direction with any certainty. Some studies reported low response rates (for example, field measurements were obtained on only half the identified potential controls in McBride et al22), and accurate response rates cannot be determined for all studies. Whether such problems have led to serious bias remains a matter of speculation; the limited evidence from U.S. studies appears conflicting (for example, contrast Savitz et al10 with Hatch et al51 and Savitz and Kaune 55).

Given the preceding considerations, it seems reasonable to suppose that measurement and validity differences are responsible for some of the variation in study-specific results. Those considerations also raise a serious criticism of our analysis, in that we pooled different magnetic field measures without demonstrating that all of the measures are comparable or combinable. Indeed, it is highly implausible that the measures we used (or any other choices among available measures) reflect common underlying exposure and error distributions. Furthermore, our criteria for choosing measures when we had a choice are not compelling (for example, minimize missing data), and one could reasonably argue in favor of other choices 56 (although not without dispute 57,58). We expected that measure heterogeneity would lead to extra variation among the study-specific results, so we are all the more surprised that the observed variation was limited. We caution, however, that other choices could lead to very different degrees of variation; our results may not even be typical of what would be seen upon trying all defensible choices (although exploring the full range of choices would not indicate which choice is most valid). These problems should further expand the considerable uncertainty apparent in our results.

Another meta-analytic issue is that of publication bias. Because of the publicity surrounding the topic, we speculate that the data in small unpublished studies (if any exist) would have little influence on the results, and that all large studies of this topic get published. Unfortunately, there are as yet too few published studies of fields or wire codes and childhood leukemia to support a reliable analysis of this bias, 59,60 and current methods for analyzing the bias are not well suited for relations that require several degrees of freedom to summarize.

Our attributable-fraction estimate is subject to further criticism through its dependence on the EPRI survey. 41 The survey measurements are of residential fields and therefore exclude sources such as school exposures and electric blankets; this exclusion error probably increases with age, especially upon school entry. Furthermore, selection bias could have been introduced because the survey homes were not limited to homes with children. Nonetheless, we think our estimate shows that any population effect of fields is probably much too small to detect via ecologic or time-trend studies; large ecologic variation or trends in leukemia rates would more likely be due to ecologic or temporal confounding than to real EMF effects.

In light of the above problems, the inconclusiveness of our results seems inescapable; resolution will have to await considerably more data on high electric and magnetic-field exposures, childhood leukemia, and possible bias sources. It also appears to us that, if an effect exists below 0.2 μT, it is probably too small to reach consensus about it via epidemiologic investigation alone. In contrast, both our categorical and trend analyses indicate that there is some association comparing fields above 0.3 μT to lower exposures, although there are as yet insufficient data to provide more than a vague sense of its form and its possible sources. We believe individual-level studies that focus on highly exposed populations would be needed to clarify this association. Such populations might be found in densely settled areas of some industrialized countries, such as Japan. 61 Even in these countries, efficiency might be improved by restricting the source population to locales containing transmission lines, as was done in some Scandinavian studies.

Back to Top | Article Outline


We thank Jack Sahl for scientific contributions at the inception of this project; A. Ahlbom, K. Ebi (EPRI), A. Fajardo-Gutiérrez, M. Feychting, L. Kheifets, R. R. Neutra, J. M. Robins, D. Savitz, J. Schüz, P. Verkasalo, and N. Wertheimer for helpful reviews; and the referees for their criticisms.

Back to Top | Article Outline


1. Portier CJ, Wolfe MS, eds. Health Effects from Exposure to Power-Line Frequency Electric and Magnetic Fields. NIH Pub. No. 9:9-4493, 1999. Research Triangle Park, NC: National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences.

2. Coghill RW, Steward J, Philips A. Extra low frequency electric and magnetic fields in the bedplace of children diagnosed with leukemia: a case-control study. Eur J Cancer Prev 1996; 5: 153–158.

3. Fajardo-Gutiérrez A, Velásquez-Pérez L, Martínez-Méndez J, Martínez-García C. Exposición a campos electromagnéticos y su asociación con leucemia en ninõs residentes de la ciudad de México. México DF: Unidad de Investigación Médica en Epidemiología Clínica Hospital de Pediatria Centro Médico Nacional Siglo XXI, 1997.

4. Feychting M, Ahlbom A. Magnetic fields and cancer in children residing near Swedish high-voltage power lines. Am J Epidemiol 1993; 138: 467–481.

5. Fulton JP, Cobb S, Preble L, Leone L, Forman E. Electrical wiring configurations and childhood leukemia in Rhode Island. Am J Epidemiol 1980; 111: 292–296.

6. Linet MS, Hatch EE, Kleinermann RA, Robison LC, Kaune WT, Friedman DR, Severson RK, Haines CM, Hartsock CT, Niwa S, Wacholder S, Tarone RE. Residential exposure to magnetic fields and acute lymphoblastic leukemia in children. N Engl J Med 1997; 337: 1–7.

7. London SJ, Thomas DC, Bowman JD, Sobel E, Cheng T-C, Peters JM. Exposure to residential electric and magnetic fields and risk of childhood leukemia. Am J Epidemiol 1991; 134: 923–937.

8. Michaelis J, Schüz J, Meinert R, Zemann E, Grigat JP, Kaatsch P, Kaletsch U, Miesner A, Brinkman K, Kalkner W, Karner H. Combined risk estimates for two German population-based case-control studies on residential magnetic fields and childhood leukemia. Epidemiology 1998; 9: 92–94.

9. Olsen JH, Nielsen A, Schulgen G. Residence near high voltage facilities and risk of cancer in children. BMJ 1993; 307: 891–895.

10. Savitz DA, Wachtel H, Barnes FA, John EM, Tvrdik JG. Case-control study of childhood cancer and exposure to 60-Hz magnetic fields. Am J Epidemiol 1988; 128: 21–38.

11. Tomenius L. 50-Hz electromagnetic environment and the incidence of childhood tumors in Stockholm County. Bioelectromagnetics 1986; 7: 191–207.

12. Tynes T, Haldorsen T. Electromagnetic fields and cancer in children residing near Norwegian high-voltage power lines. Am J Epidemiol 1997; 145: 219–226.

13. Verkasalo PK, Pukkala E, Hongisto MY, Valjus JE, Järvinen PJ, Heikkila KV, Koskenvuo M. Risk of cancer in Finnish children living close to power lines. BMJ 1993; 307: 895–899.

14. Wertheimer N, Leeper E. Electrical wiring configurations and childhood cancer. Am J Epidemiol 1979; 109: 273–284.

15. Michaelis J, Schüz J, Meinert R, Menger M, Grigat JP, Kaatsh P, Kaletsch U, Miesner A, Stamm A, Brinkman K, Karner H. Childhood leukemia and electromagnetic fields: results of a population-based case-control study in Germany. Cancer Causes Control 1997; 8: 167–174.

16. Myers A, Clayden AD, Cartwright RA, Cartwright SC. Childhood cancer and overhead power lines: a case-control study. Br J Cancer 1990; 62: 1008–1014.

17. Coleman MP, Bell CMJ, Taylor HL, Primic-Zakelj M. Leukemia and residence near electricity transmission equipment: a case-control study. Br J Cancer 1989; 60: 793–798.

18. Lin SR, Liu PY. An epidemiologic study of childhood cancer in relation to residential exposure to electromagnetic fields (Abstract). Program of the U.S. Department of Energy/Electric Power Research Institute Contractors Meeting, Portland, OR, 1989. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Energy, 1989.

19. Lowenthal RM, Balkle MJ, Lickiss JN. Exposure to high tension power lines and childhood leukaemia: a pilot study (Letter). Med J Aust 1991; 155: 347.

20. Lin RS. Risk of childhood leukemia in areas passed by high power lines. Rev Environ Health 1994; 10: 97–103.

21. Petridou E, Trichopoulos D, Kravaritis A, Pourtsidis A, Dessypyris N, Skalkidis Y, Kogevinas M, Kalmanti M, Koliouskas D, Kosmidis H, Panagiotou JP, Piperopoulou F, Tzortzatou F, Kalapothaki V. Electrical power lines and childhood leukemia: a study from Greece. Int J Cancer 1997; 73: 345–348.

22. McBride ML, Gallagher RP, Thériault HG, Armstrong BG, Tamaro S, Spinelli JJ, Deadman JE, Fincham S, Robson D, Choi W. Power-frequency electric and magnetic fields and risk of childhood leukemia in Canada. Am J Epidemiol 1999; 149: 831–842.

23. Dockerty JD, Elwood JM, Skegg DCG, Herbison GP. Electromagnetic field exposures and childhood cancers in New Zealand. Cancer Causes Control 1998;9:299–309 (Correction: Cancer Causes Control 1999;10:641).

24. Green LM, Miller AB, Villeneuve PJ, Agnew DA, Greenberg ML, Li J, Donnelly KE. A case-control study of childhood leukemia in Southern Ontario, Canada, and exposure to magnetic fields in residences. Int J Cancer 1999; 82: 161–170.

25. Green LM, Miller AB, Agnew DA, Greenberg ML, Li J, Villeneuve PJ, Tibshirani R. Childhood leukemia and personal monitoring of residential exposures to electric and magnetic fields in Ontario, Canada. Cancer Causes Control 1999; 10: 233–243.

26. U.K. Childhood Cancer Study Investigators. Exposure to power-frequency magnetic fields and the risk of childhood cancer. Lancet 1999; 354: 1925–1931.

27. Kleinerman RA, Linet MS, Hatch EE, Wacholder S, Tarone RE, Severson RK, Kaune WT, Friedman DR, Haines CM, Muirhead CR, Boice JD Jr, Robison LL. Magnetic field exposure assessment in a case-control study of childhood leukemia. Epidemiology 1997; 8: 575–583.

28. Zaffanella LE, Kalton G. Survey of personal magnetic field exposure: phase I, pilot study and design of phase II. Oak Ridge, TN: DOE EMF Rapid Program, Engineering Project No 6, February 1998.

29. Kheifets LI, Kavet R, Sussman SS. Wire codes, magnetic fields, and childhood cancer. Bioelectromagnetics 1997; 18: 99–110.

30. Breslow NE, Day NE. Statistical Methods in Cancer Research, vol 1. The Analysis of Case-Control Studies. IARC Scientific Pub. No. 32. Lyon: International Agency for Research on Cancer, 1980.

31. Greenland S. Introduction to regression models. In: Rothman KJ, Greenland S, eds. Modern Epidemiology, Chapter 20. 2nd ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven, 1998; 359–399.

32. Hastie T, Tibshirani R. Generalized Additive Models. New York: Chapman and Hall, 1990.

33. Greenland S. Dose-response and trend analysis: alternatives to category-indicator regression. Epidemiology 1995; 6: 356–365.

34. Greenland S. Avoiding power loss associated with categorization and ordinal scores in dose-response and trend analysis. Epidemiology 1995; 6: 450–454.

35. Tarone RE, Kaune WT, Linet MS, Hatch EE, Kleinerman RA, Robison LL, Boice JD, Wacholder S. Residential wire codes: reproducibility and relation with measured magnetic fields. Occup Environ Med 1998; 55: 333–339.

36. Brookmeyer R, Liang K-Y, Linet M. Matched case-control designs and overmatched analyses. Am J Epidemiol 1986; 124: 643–701.

37. Greenland S. Small-sample bias and corrections for conditional maximum-likelihood odds-ratio estimators. Biostatistics 2000; 1: 113–122.

38. Swanson J, Kaune WT. Comparison of residential power-frequency magnetic fields away from appliances in different countries. Bioelectromagnetics 1999; 20: 244–254.

39. Greenland S, Michels KB, Robins JM, Poole C, Willet WC. Presenting statistical uncertainty in trends and dose-response relations. Am J Epidemiol 1999; 149: 1077–1086.

40. Carroll RJ, Ruppert D, Stefanski LA. Measurement Error in Nonlinear Models. New York: Chapman and Hall, 1995.

41. High Voltage Transmission Research Center. Survey of Residential Magnetic Field Sources (in two volumes). Palo Alto, CA: Electric Power Research Institute, 1993.

42. Greenland S. Estimation of population attributable fractions from fitted incidence ratios and exposure survey data. Biometrics 2001;57 (in press).

43. Taubes G. Fields of fear. Atlantic Monthly 1994; 274: 94–108.

44. Savitz DA, Feingold L. Association of childhood cancer with residential traffic density. Scand J Work Environ Health 1989; 15: 360–363.

45. Ebi KL. Traffic Density as a Risk Factor for Childhood Cancer in Denver and Los Angeles: EPRI Report TR-114321. Palo Alto, CA: Electric Power Research Institute, 1999.

46. Bracken MB, Belanger K, Hellebrand K, Adesso K, Patel S, Triche E, Leaderer BP. Correlates of residential wiring code used in studies of health effects of residential electromagnetic fields. Am J Epidemiol 1998; 148: 467–474.

47. Feychting M, Ahlbom A. Exposure to motor vehicle exhaust and childhood cancer. Scand J Work Environ Health 1998; 24: 8–11.

48. Jones TL, Shih CH, Thurston DH, Ware BJ, Cole P. Selection bias from differential residential mobility as an explanation for associations of wire codes with childhood cancer. J Clin Epidemiol 1993; 46: 545–548.

49. Sahl J. Viral contacts confound studies of childhood leukemia and high-voltage transmission lines. Cancer Causes Control 1994; 5: 279–283.

50. Gurney JG, Davis S, Schwartz SM, Mueller BA, Kaune WT, Stevens RG. Childhood cancer occurrence in relation to powerline configurations: a study of potential selection bias in case-control studies. Epidemiology 1995; 6: 31–35.

51. Hatch EE, Kleinerman RA, Linet MS, Tarone RE, Kaune WT, Auvinen A, Baris D, Robison LL, Wacholder S. Do confounding or selection factors of residential wiring codes and magnetic fields distort findings of electromagnetic field studies? Epidemiology 2000; 11: 189–198.

52. Weinberg CR, Umbach DM, Greenland S. When will nondifferential misclassification preserve the direction of a trend? Am J Epidemiol 1994; 140: 565–571.

53. Friedman DR, Hatch EE, Tarone R, Kaune WT, Kleineman RA, Wacholder S, Boice JD Jr, Linet MS. Childhood exposure to magnetic fields: residential area measurements compared to personal dosimetry. Epidemiology 1996; 7: 151–155.

54. Dovan T, Kaune WT. Repeatability of measurements of residential magnetic fields and wire. Bioelectromagnetics 1993; 14: 145–159.

55. Savitz DA, Kaune WT. Childhood cancer in relation to modified residential wire code. Environ Health Perspect 1993; 101: 76–80.

56. Jaffa KC, Kim H, Aldrich TE. The relative merits of contemporary measurements and historical calculated fields in the Swedish childhood cancer study. Epidemiology 2000; 11: 353–356.

57. Feychting M, Ahlbom A. With regard to the relative merits of contemporary measurements and historical calculated fields in the Swedish childhood cancer study. Epidemiology 2000; 11: 357–358.

58. Jaffa KC, Kim H, Aldrich TE. Measuring electromagnetic fields. Epidemiology 2000; 11: 359–360.

59. Begg CB, Mazumdar M. Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics 1994; 50: 1088–1101.

60. Greenland S. A critical look at some popular meta-analytic methods. Am J Epidemiol 1994; 140: 290–296.

61. Repacholi MH, Ahlbom A. Link between electromagnetic fields and childhood cancer unresolved. Lancet 1999; 354: 1918–1919.

Cited By:

This article has been cited 17 time(s).

Childhood Leukemia and Magnetic Fields in Infant Incubators
Söderberg, KC; Naumburg, E; Anger, G; Cnattingius, S; Ekbom, A; Feychting, M
Epidemiology, 13(1): 45-49.

PDF (62)
The Effect of Missing Values for Covariates
Whitlock, G; Clark, T
Epidemiology, 13(1): 113.

Health Effects of Electric and Magnetic Fields: Are We Done Yet?
Savitz, DA
Epidemiology, 14(1): 15-17.

Magnetic Fields and Acute Leukemia in Children With Down Syndrome
Mejia-Arangure, JM; Fajardo-Gutierrez, A; Perez-Saldivar, ML; Gorodezky, C; Martinez-Avalos, A; Romero-Guzman, L; Campo-Martinez, MA; Flores-Lujano, J; Salamanca-Gomez, F; Velasquez-Perez, L
Epidemiology, 18(1): 158-161.
PDF (168) | CrossRef
Residential Magnetic Field Exposure and Childhood Brain Cancer: A Meta-Analysis
Mezei, G; Gadallah, M; Kheifets, L
Epidemiology, 19(3): 424-430.
PDF (391) | CrossRef
Pooled Analysis of Magnetic Fields, Wire Codes, and Childhood Leukemia
Jaffa, KC
Epidemiology, 12(4): 472-473.

Pooled Analysis of Magnetic Fields, Wire Codes, and Childhood Leukemia
Greenland, S; Sheppard, AR; Kaune, WT; Poole, C; Kelsh, MA
Epidemiology, 12(4): 473-474.

Attributable Fractions: Bias from Broad Definition of Exposure
Greenland, S
Epidemiology, 12(5): 518-520.

PDF (44)
Authors' Response
Greenland, S; Gago-Dominguez, M; Esteban Castelao, J
Epidemiology, 15(5): 527-528.
PDF (91) | CrossRef
European Journal of Cancer Prevention
Epidemiological research on cancer with a focus on Europe
La Vecchia, C; Franceschi, S; Levi, F
European Journal of Cancer Prevention, 12(1): 5-14.

PDF (153)
European Journal of Cancer Prevention
Trends in Childhood Cancer Incidence and Mortality in Catalonia, Spain, 1975–1998
Gonzalez, J; Fernandez, E; Toledo, Jd; Galceran, J; Peris, M; Gispert, R; Borràs, J
European Journal of Cancer Prevention, 13(1): 47-51.

PDF (87)
Health Physics
A Comparison of Important International and National Standards for Limiting Exposure to Emf Including the Scientific Rationale
Roy, CR; Martin, LJ
Health Physics, 92(6): 635-641.
PDF (338) | CrossRef
Health Physics
Implications From Epidemiologic Studies on Magnetic Fields and the Risk of Childhood Leukemia on Protection Guidelines
Schüz, J
Health Physics, 92(6): 642-648.
PDF (240) | CrossRef
Health Physics
Exposure Guidelines for Low-Frequency Electric and Magnetic Fields: Report From the Brussels Workshop
Sheppard, AR; Kavet, R; Renew, DC
Health Physics, 83(3): 324-332.

PDF (56)
Health Physics
Health Effects Relevant to the Setting of Emf Exposure Limits
Bailey, WH
Health Physics, 83(3): 376-386.

PDF (72)
Health Physics
Candidate Sites of Action for Microdosimetry Associated With Exposure to Extremely-Low-Frequency Magnetic Fields, Electric Fields and Contact Currents
Sastre, A; Kavet, R
Health Physics, 83(3): 387-394.

PDF (56)
Health Physics
Residential Magnetic Fields and Measures of Neutral-to-Earth Voltage: Variability Within and Between Residences
Kavet, R; Hooper, HC
Health Physics, 97(4): 332-342.
PDF (1696) | CrossRef
Back to Top | Article Outline

childhood neoplasms; electromagnetic fields; environmental exposure; leukemia; magnetic fields; wire codes

© 2000 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Inc.

Twitter  Facebook 


Article Tools



Article Level Metrics

Search for Similar Articles
You may search for similar articles that contain these same keywords or you may modify the keyword list to augment your search.