Share this article on:

Trends and Predictors of Longitudinal Hearing Aid Use for Children Who Are Hard of Hearing

Walker, Elizabeth A.1; McCreery, Ryan W.2; Spratford, Meredith2; Oleson, Jacob J.3; Van Buren, John3; Bentler, Ruth1; Roush, Patricia4; Moeller, Mary Pat2

doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000208
Research Articles

Objectives: Children who are hard of hearing (CHH) have restricted access to acoustic and linguistic information. Increased audibility provided by hearing aids (HAs) influences language outcomes, but the advantages of appropriately fit HAs can only be realized if children wear their devices on a consistent basis. The purpose of this article was to characterize long-term HA use in CHH, based on parent-report measures, and identify factors that influence longitudinal trends in HA use.

Design: Participants were parents of 290 children with mild to severe hearing loss. At every visit, parents estimated the average amount of time the child used HAs per day during the week and on the weekends. Parent reports of daily HA use were analyzed to determine if different patterns of HA use were observed longitudinally during the study. Independent predictor variables were then related to longitudinal trends in HA use within three age groups (infant, preschool, school age).

Results: On average across multiple visits, parents reported that their children wore their HAs for 10.63 hr per day (SD = 3.29). Data logging values were lower than parent-report measures (M = 8.44, SD = 4.06), suggesting that parents overestimated daily HA use. The majority of children in each age group wore HAs at least 8 hr per day from their first research testing interval to their last, based on parent-report measures. Maternal education level predicted longitudinal trends in HA use for infants and school-age CHH. Degree of hearing loss was related to trends in school-age children only.

Conclusions: These results indicated that the majority of CHH increased HA use over time, but a sizable minority demonstrated a low level of use or decreased use in the time period studied. Maternal education level influenced longitudinal trends in daily HA use. Degree of hearing loss influenced trends in school-age children only. Audiologists and early intervention service providers might aid in improving HA use by providing regular hands-on training with the HAs and individualized problem-based strategies to address the challenges families experience with attaining a high level of use. Families may also benefit from practical demonstrations of the benefits of consistent HA use, such as hearing loss simulations, examples of listening in noise with and without HAs, or listening to malfunctioning HAs.

The primary objective of this study was to examine trends in hearing aid (HA) use over time for children who are hard of hearing (CHH). Additional goals included examining factors that influence longitudinal trends in HA use. The current results indicated that the majority of CHH increased HA use during the early childhood years, while a minority remained limited users or decreased usage over time. Maternal education level appears to play an important role in longitudinal trends in daily HA use.

1Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa, USA; 2Center for Childhood Deafness, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, Nebraska, USA; 3Department of Biostatistics, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa, USA; and 4Department of Otolaryngology, University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, USA.

Supplemental digital content is available for this article. Direct URL citations appear in the printed text and are provided in the HTML and text of this article on the journal’s Web site (www.ear-hearing.com).

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare. The authors had full editorial control of this work and manuscript.

Received December 1, 2014; accepted May 27, 2015.

Address for correspondence: Elizabeth A. Walker, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Iowa, 250 Hawkins Drive, Iowa City, IA 52242. E-mail: elizabeth-walker@uiowa.edu

Back to Top | Article Outline

INTRODUCTION

One of the goals of the Outcomes of Children with Hearing Loss (OCHL) Supplement is to explore the concept that consistent access to the speech spectrum is a prerequisite for successful speech and language development. Children who are hard of hearing (CHH), by nature of their hearing loss, have restricted access to acoustic and linguistic information. (For a Table of Acronyms, see Supplemental Digital Content, Appendix A, http://links.lww.com/EANDH/A202.) Recent studies suggest that increased audibility provided by hearing aids (HAs) influences speech production, grammar and vocabulary outcomes (Stiles et al. 2012; Koehlinger et al. 2013; Tomblin et al. 2014). Presumably, however, the advantages of appropriately fit HAs will only be realized if children wear their devices on a consistent basis. The ways in which amount of audibility and consistency of HA use accrue over time may reveal an effect of cumulative linguistic experience on speech and language development. The purpose of this article is to characterize long-term HA use in CHH, based on parent report, and determine what factors influence consistency of HA use over time. In subsequent articles in this OCHL Supplement, we will seek to determine if amount of HA use influences language outcomes of CHH.

Most studies involving CHH have not addressed the contributions of aided audibility or amount of daily HA use on individual differences in speech perception, speech production, or language (Davis et al. 1981, 1986; Elfenbein et al. 1994; Sininger et al. 2010; Fitzpatrick et al. 2011). There is compelling evidence for a relationship between aided audibility and vocabulary skills (Stiles et al. 2012), as well as syntax (Koehlinger et al. 2013), articulation, and global language (Tomblin et al. 2014), but there is less research on how the amount of daily HA use might influence outcomes. Marnane and Ching (2015) examined HA use as a categorical variable and reported that the majority of 3-year olds were consistent users (i.e., more than 8 hr per day). They also found that children who wore HAs more frequently had higher scores on a parent-report scale of auditory skills, but there was no significant relationship between amount of HA use and functional outcomes after controlling for other demographic variables (maternal education level, additional disabilities). In contrast, other studies indicate that amount of HA use varies substantially across CHH (Fitzpatrick et al. 2010; Walker et al. 2013; Muñoz et al. 2014, 2015), when HA use is considered as a continuous variable as opposed to a categorical variable. Further research is needed to explore the influence of HA use (measured as a continuous variable) on auditory and language outcomes.

There is also evidence that the amount of HA use varies widely depending on the listening context, chronological age, and degree of hearing loss of the child, as well as socioeconomic status of the family (Moeller et al. 2009; Walker et al. 2013; Muñoz et al. 2015). Moeller et al. conducted longitudinal interviews with mothers of seven early-identified infants with mild to moderate hearing loss (age range = 11 to 28.5 months). Instead of asking mothers to estimate overall daily HA use, the researchers evaluated consistency of use with a Likert scale for different situations (e.g., in the car, book sharing, at the playground, etc.). Results showed that the infants were inconsistent HA users for most situations before 2 years of age. Only 2 out of the 7 parents were able to establish early, consistent usage across all contexts. Over time, amount of HA use increased, although at the oldest age (28.5 months) there were still some situations that parents found problematic, such as riding in the car. Consistent with the findings of Moeller et al., Muñoz et al. (2014, 2015) have also found that HA management is difficult for families with younger children, particularly in specific contexts. Muñoz et al. (2015) surveyed 37 families about parent-reported challenges with HA use. Muñoz and colleagues noted wide variation in the amount of hours children wore their HAs, with 32% (n = 11) of parents reporting that their child wore HAs less than 5 hr per day. Similar to the results in Moeller et al. (2009), parents also reported that specific situations resulted in difficulty with HA compliance, including riding in the car, playing outside, or leaving the child in the care of alternate caregivers.

The findings by Moeller et al. (2009) and Muñoz et al. (2015) suggest that there are individual differences in HA use time for young children as a result of the listening context, which could contribute to variation in speech and language outcomes in children with hearing loss. One limitation in both studies is that the researchers had to rely on parents as proxy reporters of HA use, because objective measures were not available at the time of data collection. Many current HA models now have data logging, a feature built into the HA which automatically records information about the HAs over time, including average hours of daily use. Jones and Launer (2011) utilized a database for pediatric HA fittings at participating clinics, and collected data logging information on approximately 5000 children, ages birth to 19 years. Consistent with the parent-report measures in previous research (Moeller et al. 2009; Muñoz et al. 2015), they found that the amount of HA use per day increased with age. Unlike the results in Moeller et al., the data logging results indicated that only 10% of children were full-time HA users, defined by Jones and Launer as more than 12 hours per day. Furthermore, 40% of children used their HAs for 4 hr or less per day. The average use time for the entire sample was 5.5 hr per day, although there was wide variation across children. Jones and Feilner (2013) reported on more recent data collected from the Phonak database. They separated use time based on the ages of the children, noting that infants and toddlers wore HAs an average of 5.5 hr and school-age children wore HAs an average of 7 hr per day. These results lend additional support to the premise that there is considerable variability in the amount of daily HA use in children with hearing loss. These daily use practices may interact with the level of aided audibility provided by the HAs, and together these two factors may influence developmental outcomes of CHH. However, further investigation is needed to support this hypothesis.

Because the data logging information in the Jones and Launer (2011) and Jones and Feilner (2013) studies were obtained through a confidential patient database, the authors were unable to describe the demographic characteristics of the children, and therefore, could not identify child- or family-specific factors that influenced individual differences in average daily HA use. Such information has clinical value, because it could help direct audiological and educational counseling practices. Marttila and Karikoski (2006) looked at HA use as a dichotomous variable (nonusers versus users) and reported that chronological age, degree of hearing loss, and age at HA fitting influenced whether or not children wore HAs. Muñoz et al. (2014) examined data logging results from a cross-sectional sample of 29 children. Based on descriptive statistics, they observed that HA use increased with chronological age and severity of hearing loss. Walker et al. (2013) examined predictive factors of average daily HA use within a cross-sectional, multicenter sample of 272 CHH, between the ages of 5 months and 7 years, 3 months. Results from a multiple regression analysis showed that longer daily HA use (based on parent report) was related to older age, poorer hearing, higher maternal education level, and recruitment site.

One drawback of Jones and Launer (2011), Muñoz et al. (2015), and Walker et al. (2013) is that these studies used cross-sectional data to describe daily HA use. Moeller et al. (2009) collected longitudinal data on consistency of HA use across different contexts, but it was with a small number of children (n = 7). Given the trends seen in Moeller et al. and Muñoz et al. (2014), as well as the significant predictor of age in the Walker et al. findings, it is likely that many children start off wearing HA only a few hours a day during infancy, but increase their amount of HA use during preschool. Alternative trends are also possible, however: some children may start off wearing their HAs for the majority of the day and maintain that level of HA use over time. Other children may start off using the HAs frequently but decrease use over time, perhaps due to the influence of degree of hearing loss. For example, children with mild degrees of HL may start to identify situations where they feel they can “get by” without amplification as they get older. All three trends are plausible, but more importantly, there are different implications for counseling depending on which pattern children tend to show.

There is limited clinical evidence regarding the influence of HA use on developmental outcomes. To examine the effects of amplification on outcomes, we first need to understand the longitudinal variations in HA use from infancy to the early school-age years, as well as the variables that predict trends in longitudinal HA use for individual children. These lines of research, when considered in combination with the influence of audibility (McCreery et al., 2015, this issue, pp. 24S–37S) and linguistic input (Ambrose et al., 2015, this issue, pp. 48S–59S), will allow clinicians and researchers to make more informed evidence-based decisions on appropriate interventions for families of young children with hearing loss.

This study addresses the following research questions:

1. What are longitudinal trends in daily HA use among CHH? We predict that most children will increase HA use over time, based on parent report. We also anticipate that trends in longitudinal HA use will vary as a function of chronological age at baseline (i.e., most school-age children will display trends of high HA use over time, while most infants will display trends of low HA use initially, but increased HA use over time).

2. What factors predict trends in daily HA use over time? We predict that greater degree of hearing loss and higher maternal educational level will be predictive of greater daily HA use over time. Gender, age at HA fitting, and recruitment site will not.

Back to Top | Article Outline

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Participants were parents of 290 children (133 females) with hearing loss who wore HAs. At the initial testing visit, the children were between the ages of 5 months and 7 years, 1 month (M = 39.84 months; SD = 21.58 months). Two hundred and twenty children (75.9%) referred from their newborn hearing screen. The mean age at confirmation of hearing loss was 7.34 months (median = 3.0, SD = 12.11) and the mean age at fitting of HAs was 11.20 months (median = 5.75, SD = 14.47) for children who failed the newborn hearing screen.

Sixty-nine children were identified with hearing loss after the newborn period, due to passing the newborn hearing screen, not being screened, or lack of follow-up after the failed newborn hearing screen (1 child’s newborn hearing screen status was unknown). Of those 69 participants, the mean age at confirmation of hearing loss was 28.48 months (median = 28, SD = 17.15) and the mean age at HA fitting was 31.68 months (median = 30.00, SD = 18.32). Based on a review of medical records and parent report, we were able to confirm that 30.4% of these later-identified children had a congenital hearing loss and 29% had a confirmed postnatal onset of hearing loss as a result of structural abnormalities (e.g., Enlarged Vestibular Aqueduct Syndrome) or acquired etiologies (e.g., ototoxicity, meningitis, assisted ventilation). We were unable to determine the onset of hearing loss for 40.6% of the group.

The mean better-ear pure-tone average (BEPTA) for the whole group at the baseline visit was 48.73 dB HL (SD = 13.48). Sixty-nine participants had a slight/mild hearing loss (16.25 to 39 dB HL), 150 had a moderate hearing loss (40 to 59 dB HL), and 64 had a moderately-severe/severe hearing loss (60 to 82.50 dB HL) at the baseline visit (7 did not have audiometric data at the baseline visit). Twelve percent (34/290) presented with a progressive hearing loss during the course of the study, as determined by more than a 10 dB HL increase in BEPTA (i.e., decline in thresholds) between the first and last visit. Two hundred and eighty-three children were fitted with wide dynamic range compression HAs and 6 had bone conduction HAs. One child had wide dynamic range compression HAs at the first visit and bone conduction HAs at all subsequent visits. Ten children received cochlear implants subsequent to study enrollment. Only preimplantation data are included in this analysis.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Data Collection

As previously described in this OCHL Supplement (Tomblin et al. 2015b, this issue, pp. 14S–23S), children and their families participated in an initial baseline visit, followed by two visits a year for children under age 2 years and one visit a year for children older than 2 years for up to 4 years, or until 9 years. The number of visits varied across children, depending on their age at enrollment and time of study completion. At the initial visit, parents completed an intake interview with an examiner. Intake questions documented demographic characteristics related to the child and family, including predictor variables incorporated in this study (i.e., maternal education level, gender, and age at HA fitting). At every visit, parents participated in a face-to-face interview regarding HA use. To determine long-term trends in HA use, we included data from each visit for parents who completed the HA use questionnaire at more than one visit (number of observations = 915). Seventy parents reported on HA use for two visits, 86 for three visits, 100 for four visits, 21 for five visits, and 2 for six visits.

In addition to parent-report measures of HA use, audiologists obtained data logging information when possible. Families were unaware that the data logging feature was used throughout the data collection period, and the results were not shared with parents due to potentially biasing effects of intervention. In this analysis, 232 children had at least one visit in which data logging and parent-report measures could be compared. Eighty-four children had data logging collected at one visit, 107 children had data logging at two visits, 37 had data logging at three visits, and 4 had data logging at four visits.

Back to Top | Article Outline

HA Use Questionnaire

As part of each visit, an examiner conducted an interview with the caregiver pertaining to pediatric HA use (see Walker et al. 2013, for an example of the HA use questionnaire). Parents estimated the average amount of time the child used HAs per day during the week and on the weekends. To compare the parent-report estimate with data logging, we calculated a weighted HA use measure in which parents’ estimates of weekday use was multiplied by 0.71 (5/7 days of the week) and weekend use was multiplied by 0.29 (2/7 days of the week). The two values were then added together. This weighted measure was used to represent HA use time in all statistical analyses.

Back to Top | Article Outline

HA Data Logging

Audiologists collected values for average use time per day by connecting the HAs to a HiPro box and using the appropriate HA software to obtain data logging values. If the values were different between ears, the larger value was included in data analyses.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Statistical Analyses

The experimental design for the two primary research questions in this study was longitudinal with multiple age cohorts (i.e., infant, preschool, school age). For our first research question, investigating HA use over time, we analyzed clusters based on longitudinal trends in daily HA use. To determine how longitudinal trends tend to cluster, we examined parent report of HA use at the baseline visit in combination with the amount of change between the first and the last visit. Ward’s hierarchical clustering (1963) was used to determine cluster groupings in the longitudinal HA trends.

In our second research question, we examined which independent predictor variables were related to longitudinal trends in HA use. To investigate the relationships among the independent predictor variables and the dependent response variables, we used logistic regression models. Statistical analyses were conducted by separating children into three subgroups based on age at baseline visit: infants (6 months to 2 years, n = 80), preschool (2 years to 5 years, n = 134); and school age (5 years to 7 years, n = 76). Due to low sample sizes and missing data, univariate analyses were performed within each age group. Logistic regression was conducted with the continuous independent variables, age at HA fitting and BEPTA (four-frequency PTA when it was available, three-frequency PTA when it was not). Fisher’s χ2 tests were conducted with the categorical variables: maternal education level, test site, and gender. Because the number of visits differed across children, we also used Fisher’s exact test to evaluate the effect of number of visits that each participant had within each of the clusters. We used Cohen’s (1988) criteria to determine effect sizes for the regression analyses (small = 1.49; medium = 3.45; large = 9).

The independent variables were gender, recruitment site (University of Iowa, Boys Town National Research Hospital, and University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill), maternal education level, BEPTA, age at HA fitting, and number of visits. Maternal education and BEPTA were obtained from the baseline visit. The dependent variables consisted of predetermined dichotomous categories of longitudinal HA use, based on the results of the cluster analysis. Due to the large number of maternal education categories collected, arbitrary nominal levels were introduced in the data analysis. Table 1 displays descriptive statistics summarizing both the independent and dependent variables that were included in the regression analysis.

Back to Top | Article Outline

RESULTS

Descriptive HA Use Data

On average, parents reported that their children wore their HAs for 10.78 hr per day during the week (SD = 3.26) and 10.24 hr per day during the weekends (SD = 3.68). The weighted sum of weekday and weekend use was 10.63 hr per day (SD = 3.29). Data logging indicated 8.44 hr per day (SD = 4.06). These values are longitudinal and include repeated measures, in that there are multiple data points for individual children across visits. Descriptively, the values are slightly higher than the data reported in Walker et al. (2013; 10.48 hr for weekday use, 9.96 hr for weekend use, and 8.2 hr for data logging), which may reflect the inclusion of later visits and an increase in data from older children in this analysis. Figure 1 displays the distributions in the parent-report measure for HA use and data logging (with repeated measures for both parent-report and data logging measures). It is apparent from the figure that the median amounts of HA use increased with age, as reported on parent measures and through data logging measures. This trend supports our prediction that HA use will increase over time.

Figure 2 displays the mean and median values for hours of use, per age group, based on data logging values from the first visit in which data logging was obtained (i.e., no repeated measures). Data logging results indicated that infants (i.e., children with visits between 6 months and 2 years of age) wore HAs an average of 4.36 hr per day (SD = 3.17), preschoolers (i.e., children with visits between 2 and 4 years of age) wore HAs an average of 7.57 hr per day (SD = 4.32), and school-age children (i.e., children with visits between 5 and 9 years of age) wore HAs an average of 10.15 hr per day (SD = 3.33). Figure 3 displays the percentage of children who were routine HA users and limited HA users, based on data logging. We defined routine use as 8 hr or more per day, based on the fact that infants generally sleep an average of 14 hr, which decreases to an average of 10 hr for 9-year olds (Iglowstein et al. 2003). We also took into account times during the day when it is unlikely children would be wearing HAs (e.g., swimming, bath, getting dressed). We acknowledge that this value associated with “routine” is arbitrary and 8 hr per day should not be considered full-time or optimal use for children. We defined limited HA use as less than 4 hr per day, using the rationale that such low use is inconsistent with best practice recommendations for young children who use HAs.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Relationships Between Parent-Report Measures and Data Logging: Cross-Sectional Analysis

To examine the relationship between parent-report measures and data logging, we compared data from the first visit in which both measures were collected (n = 232). Thus, the analysis in this section is cross-sectional rather than longitudinal, to avoid having to account for multiple visits with individual participants. Using a Pearson correlation coefficient, there was a statistically significant correlation between parent-report measures and data logging (r = 0.71; p < 0.001). This correlation was consistent with the relationship between parent-report measures and data logging described in Walker et al. (2013), in which parent report of HA use and data logging showed a significant correlation of 0.76. The correlation between parent report and data logging was generally consistent across age groups (infants: n = 34, r = 0.69, p < 0.001; preschool: n = 98, r = 0.62, p < 0.001; school age: n = 100, r = 0.61, p = 0.001).

The mean difference between parent-report measures and data logging was 2.34 hr (SD = 3.04) for all participants. When children were separated based on age, infants had a mean difference between parent-report measures and data logging of 2.43 hr (SD = 2.45), preschoolers had a mean difference of 2.53 hr (SD = 3.36), and school-age children had a mean difference of 2.13 hr (SD = 2.90). In total, 81% of parents overestimated the amount of time their child wore their HAs, and 19% were accurate or underestimated HA use. Again, this is consistent with the cross-sectional findings in Walker et al. (2013), in which the average overestimation was 2.6 hr (n = 133, SD = 2.52) and 84% of parents overestimated daily HA use. Taken together, the correlational data and mean differences between data logging and parent-report data suggest that the majority of parents overestimate HA use, but the relative difference between parents’ estimations and data logging is consistent across children.

To better understand differences between parent-report measures and data logging, we examined 95% confidence intervals for data logging given increasing amounts of HA use reported by parents for the whole group. If a parent reported 5 hr of daily use, the model estimated data logged time was 3.60 hr with a 95% confidence interval between 2.87 and 4.32 hr. If a parent reported 10 hr of daily use, the estimated data logged time was 7.74 hr with a 95% confidence interval of 7.35 to 8.13 hr. If a parent reported 15 hr of daily use, the estimated data logged time was 11.88 hr with a 95% confidence interval from 11.27 to 12.49 hr.

We conducted a multiple linear regression analysis to determine what factors predicted the amount of overestimation between parent report of HA use and data logging values. Independent variables included maternal education level, child’s chronological age, recruitment site, gender, and BEPTA. The dependent variable was the difference between parent report of HA use and data logging value at the first visit in which parent report and data logging were both available (n = 232). Only BEPTA was a significant predictor of overestimation (β = −0.14; p = 0.03). Parents of children with better hearing were more likely to overestimate HA use compared with parents of children with poorer hearing. These results were inconsistent with results in Walker et al. (2013), in which chronological age was the only significant predictor of the amount of overestimation in parent report (i.e., parents of younger children tended to overestimate more than parents of older children). This discrepancy may be related to the addition of more children with mild hearing loss in the current dataset (54 children, compared with 24 children in the mild hearing loss range in Walker et al. 2013). Parents of children with mild hearing loss may be less aware of how often their child is wearing HAs on a daily basis, perhaps because there are circumstances in which these children do not wear amplification consistently (e.g., after school, on the weekends).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Relationships Between Parent-Report Measures and Data Logging Over Time: Longitudinal

To determine whether or not trends in parent report hours of use would be consistent with trends in data logging, we examined results for 38 children (16 infant, 14 preschool, 8 school age) who had data logging and parent-report measures at both their first baseline visit and last visit. We conducted a two-sample paired t test to assess the differences between parent report and data logging for their baseline scores and change scores (difference between baseline and last visit). There was a significant difference between the baseline scores for parent report and data logging hours (p < 0.001), which is expected given the mean differences presented in the descriptive data for the whole group of children. Paired t tests did not indicate a significant difference in the change values over time (p = 0.15). In other words, the difference between hours of use at the baseline visit compared with the final visit was similar between parent-report measures and data logging. Thus, parent-report data showed the same relative trends as data logging over time.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Research Question 1: What Are Longitudinal Trends in HA Use?

For the current longitudinal cluster analysis (question 1) and the subsequent logistic regression analysis (question 2), we relied on parent-report measures rather than data logging. Although objective data logging measures may have been preferable, such an analysis would have been limited because of the lower number of repeated measures for data logging, particularly for children with three or more visits (41 children had data logging at three or more visits, compared with 209 children with parent-report data). Furthermore, the correlation between data logging and parent report was strong and the relative trends were similar over time. Given the much larger number of participants with long-term parent report data compared with data logging, parent report of HA use allowed us to examine long-term trends with sufficient power to conduct regression analyses.

For question 1, we predicted that longitudinal trends in HA use might vary as a function of chronological age at baseline. To further investigate that prediction, children were separated into three groups by age at the baseline visit: infant, preschool, and school age. Data from each age group were clustered based on the amount of HA use at the initial visit (high, low) and direction of growth between the initial and final visit (increasing, decreasing, no change). Because participants varied in the number of visits, final visits did cross into the older age groups for some participants (i.e., a child might start the study at 2 years of age and be followed over four visits so that their final visit was at 6 years of age). In these cases, the age cohort was based on the child’s age at the start of the study. We arbitrarily divided higher and lower amounts of HA use at 8 hr, using the rationale stated earlier in this manuscript.

For longitudinal HA use, infants were clustered into categories of (1) high daily use increasing over time, (2) high daily use decreasing over time, (3) low daily HA use increasing over time, and (4) low daily use decreasing over time (Table 2 and Fig. 4). Children in the first group (high increase) made up 42% of the group, whereas 22% showed the high-decreasing trend. Both of these groups showed an early pattern of high HA use, even though they showed different trajectories in HA use over time. These two groups were combined for the statistical analysis, because of their similar amounts of long-term HA use. There was a subgroup of infants who were low HA users, but increased their use with time (27%). Finally, there was a group of infants (10%) who showed low amounts of HA use at baseline, and remained low users over time. The latter two groups were analyzed separately, because they demonstrated different patterns of use over time.

Preschoolers were clustered into groups that were similar to the infants: (1) high daily use increasing over time, (2) high daily use decreasing over time, (3) low daily use increasing over time, and (4) low daily use decreasing over time (Table 2 and Fig. 4). When the high-increase and high-decrease clusters are combined, 79% of the preschool-age children could be viewed as wearing their HAs more than 8 hr per day over time. Again, these two groups were combined for the logistic regression, because both trends represented high HA use from the baseline to the final visit. Eleven percent wore HAs at low levels at the first visit, but increased use time by their last visit, while 10% demonstrated low amounts of HA use over time. Like the infants, the latter two groups were analyzed separately.

Data for school-age children clustered into slightly different longitudinal trends compared with the infants and preschoolers. One trend included children who demonstrated high daily HA use initially, which increased over time, while another trend included children with high daily HA use and no change over time (Table 2 and Fig. 4). These two groups were combined, because both trends represented high HA use from the baseline to the final visit. Together, these groups made up 84% of the school-age children. Six children displayed high HA use initially, but decreased daily use over time. Another six children displayed low HA use initially, and increased use by the final visit, but still not to a level that would be considered high level of HA use based on the average amount of daily use for school-age children (M = 11.49). These two groups were also combined, because both trends represented low usage over time. Together, these two groups made up 16% of the school-age children.

By examining parent report of HA use as a function of age, the majority of children in each age group wore HAs more than 8 hr per day from their first OCHL study visit to their last. As predicted, the percentage of children in this particular cluster increased with age, from 64% of the infants to 84% of the school-age group. Interestingly, even though there was no overlap in participants across the three groups, the percentage of children who demonstrated low HA use over time was fairly constant across the three age groups (10% in the infant and preschool groups; 16% in the school-age group). These data suggest that there is a fairly constant percentage of children who maintain low HA use as they get older.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Research Question 2: What Factors Predict Trends in Longitudinal HA Use?

Because chronological age did appear to influence longitudinal HA patterns, both in the trends that were displayed and the percent of children who fell into those clusters, we used regression analyses within each age group, as opposed to combining the entire group. This allowed us to identify the factors that predict which children fell into the longitudinal cluster groups, as a function of age.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Infants

The first analysis of infants combined the start high-increasing group with the start high-decreasing group (based on the rationale that both groups showed a trend of more than 8 hr of HA use over time). This combined start high group was compared with the start low-increasing group. Pairwise comparisons for maternal education levels revealed significant differences between levels, with large effect sizes (i.e., odds ratios [OR]). The odds of being in the start low-increasing group instead of the start high group were nine times higher for mothers with a high school diploma compared with mothers with some college education or mothers with a postgraduate degree (OR = 8.99, p = 0.03; OR = 9.01, p = 0.04, respectively), and 18 times higher for mothers with a high school degree compared with mothers with a bachelor’s degree (OR = 18.15, p = 0.01). Table 3 displays a summary of significant variables and ORs for the infant cohort. There were no other significant pairwise comparisons in maternal education level (p > 0.05). Recruitment site, age at HA fitting, gender, BEPTA, and number of visits were not significant predictors of cluster groups (all p values > 0.18).

The second analysis of infants compared the start low-increasing group to the start low-decreasing group. No factors predicted which children fell into these groups, possibly because of small sample sizes (n = 8 for children in the start low-decrease group and n = 19 for children in the start low-increase group).

Back to Top | Article Outline

Preschool

The first analysis of the preschool group combined the start high-increasing cluster with the start high-decreasing cluster, after the same rationale used with the infants. This combined start high group was compared with the start low-increasing cluster. None of the predictor variables were significant (all p values > 0.07).

The second analysis compared the start low-increasing group with the start low-decreasing group. There were no significant predictors of group membership between these two comparisons, except for the number of visits (p < 0.01). The low-decreasing group had fewer overall visits than the low-increasing group. All of the preschool children in the low-decreasing group had two or three visits while the majority of the low-increasing group had four visits. This resulted in a greater proportion of children who were 5 years or older (79%) in the low-increasing group at the last visit compared with the low-decreasing group (33%). Thus, it is possible that if we had been able to continue following the children in the low-decreasing group, the trends may have reversed upon school entry.

Back to Top | Article Outline

School Age

For school-age children, we combined the start high-increasing group with the start high-no change group. This group represented routine HA users who wore HAs more than 8 hr per day over time. We also combined the start low-increasing group with the start high-decreasing group. This group represented low HA users over time. These two groups were then compared with one another. Maternal education level was a significant predictor of group (p = 0.05), with large effect sizes. Children whose mothers had a bachelor’s degree were approximately 14 times more likely to be routine users, compared with mothers who had some college education (OR = 13.89; p = 0.04). Children whose mothers had a postgraduate degree were 20 times more likely to be in routine users, compared with mothers with some college education (OR = 19.96; p = 0.02). No other pairwise comparisons were significant for maternal education level. BEPTA was also a significant predictor of cluster group (OR = 1.10; p = 0.02), with small effect sizes. In other words, children with better hearing were more likely to use HAs less frequently over time, while children with poorer hearing were more likely to be in clusters using HAs 8 hr or more over time. Age at HA fitting, recruitment site, gender, and number of visits were not significant predictors of cluster group (all p values > 0.36). Table 3 summarizes the significant results from the regression analyses.

In summary, the results of the regression analysis indicate that maternal education level is a significant predictor of longitudinal trends in HA use for infants and school-age CHH. None of the variables were significant predictors for preschool-age children, but these findings appear to have been influenced by the number of visits per cluster for the preschoolers. Degree of HL was a significant predictor of longitudinal HA use trends for elementary school-age children only.

Back to Top | Article Outline

DISCUSSION

The primary goals of this study were to describe longitudinal trends in HA use and examine the factors that influenced longitudinal trends. Results showed that CHH vary in the amount of time they wear HAs, which is consistent with previous findings from the OCHL cohort and others (Moeller et al. 2009; Walker et al. 2013; Muñoz et al. 2014, 2015). Specific factors appeared to influence long-term trends in HA use, but interestingly, these predictors differed depending on the age range in question.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Research Question #1: What Are Longitudinal Trends in Daily HA Use?

The results for our first research question were consistent with our predictions. We expected to see most of the children show a pattern of increasing HA use over time and the results supported this hypothesis. Many of the children within each of the three age cohorts were routine HA users who wore HAs 8 hr per day or more to start with and maintained that level of use over time. It should be acknowledged that these data may be an overestimation of the percentage of children who might be considered routine HA users, because it was based on parent report data rather than data logging. An important future direction for this line of research will be to investigate longitudinal trends in HA use using data logging as an objective measure of use time.

Not surprisingly, the infant group demonstrated the smallest percentage of children who wore HAs more than 8 hr per day over time (64%). Although this still represents a majority of the infants, a substantial minority (36%) of parents reported very limited HA use, at least initially. It is encouraging to note, however, that 27% of the infants showed a trend of moving from limited to more routine HA use with age. This cluster within the infant age group is important to recognize, because it shows families that it is possible to overcome challenges to HA use for CHH during the infant and toddler years. These findings highlight a need for continued support for families who are dealing with HA compliance issues in young children. In our previous study, parents recounted child-related challenges as causes for less-than-optimal HA use consistency, although the report of these challenges decreases by age 2 years (Walker et al. 2013). Muñoz et al. (2014) investigated the issue of child-related challenges in more detail, and reported that parents had concerns about losing the HAs in public places or when the child was in the care of others. Furthermore, because individual differences in language abilities are greater for younger children than older children (Fenson et al. 2000), parents of infants may not see the benefit of wearing HAs as readily as parents of children who are older. Nevertheless, it is important for service providers to remind parents of infants of the cumulative nature of the benefit of HA use. Parents who report that they are confident in their ability to manage their child’s HAs also perceive that their child wears their device more often and feel that they are more involved in their child’s development of speech and language, compared with parents who report feelings of low self-efficacy with regards to HA management (Desjardin 2005). Audiologists and early intervention service providers might aid in improving HA use consistency, especially for families with low socioeconomic status, by providing regular hands-on training with the HAs or providing a parent–parent mentor. Families may also benefit from practical demonstrations of the benefits of consistent HA use, such as hearing loss simulations, examples of listening in noise with and without HAs, or listening with malfunctioning HAs. To date, however, there is no evidence-based research to indicate what strategies are most effective for improving HA compliance in young children. Future research might explore the efficiency of different counseling practices in improving HA compliance in infants, for audiologists, speech-language pathologists, and early interventionists.

These findings also indicate that a fairly stable percentage of infant, preschool, and school-age children maintained lower HA use over time (10 to 16% across the age cohorts), even though the participants in these age cohorts did not overlap. This may be an underestimation of the proportion of children with low levels of HA use over the long term, as the analyses were performed with parent report data instead of data logging, and parent report has been found to regularly overestimate data logging values. Bearing this in mind, these results indicate a need for support and continued counseling by service providers regarding the importance of consistent device use, even after children have worn HAs for several years. In this context, a discussion of the results of data logging (currently available in most HA models) with parents and children could play a role in counseling efforts during regular intervention visits. It is also likely to be helpful to continue to discuss context-specific barriers to HA use with parents. For example, as children enter late-elementary and middle school, they may be less inclined to wear HAs in social settings. Children may encounter challenges when they are in situations where they must wear a helmet (e.g., bike riding, sports). These dataset only includes children in the early elementary school years; therefore, it is unclear what potential barriers may arise as children get older. We are continuing to follow this cohort of children into later elementary grades to explore this issue in a renewal of the original OCHL grant.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Research Question #2: What Factors Predict Trends in Daily HA Use Over Time?

We hypothesized that two factors—poorer hearing and higher maternal education level—would predict more consistent HA use over time. We based the first two predictions on findings from our previous research (Walker et al. 2013), in which BEPTA and maternal education level were significant predictors of amount of daily HA use.

Our predictions were correct, in that degree of HL and maternal education level both influenced parent-reported trends in HA use over time. We did not predict that these variables would have a different effect depending on the age of the child, although this finding is not surprising. Maternal education level was a significant predictor for the youngest age group and the oldest age group: infants and school-age children whose mothers had more education were more likely to start off wearing the HAs more than 8 hr per day, and maintain that level of use over time, while children whose mothers had less education tended to fall into a pattern of low initial HA use, increasing with time. Within the preschool group, however, maternal education level did not influence trends in HA use. This may be because of the fact that by this age, parents and children, regardless of socioeconomic status, have had sufficient time to appreciate the level of benefit the child receives from amplification, or be more cognizant of lack of progress in speech and language skills. At this stage in development, family-specific factors such as maternal education level may start to play less of a role in the amount of daily HA use (Walker et al. 2013), although it is not clear from this analysis if child-specific factors play more of a role.

With school-age children, both maternal education level and severity of hearing loss appeared to play a role in higher versus lower levels of HA use. This shift in influential factors over time is practical information for service providers because it highlights a change in emphasis of counseling, depending on the age of the child. It is also important to note that the school-age children with more severe degrees of hearing loss tended to be more routine HA users, whereas children with milder degrees of hearing loss tended to show less regular HA use. The influence of degree of hearing loss is consistent with our previous findings (Walker et al. 2013). In that study, children with BEPTAs of more than 50 dB HL wore HAs more often than children with BEPTA of less than 50 dB HL, based on ratings of consistency of use both in public and at school. Fitzpatrick et al. (2010) also reported decreased HA use in children with mild hearing loss. They examined a population-based, retrospective chart review of 255 children with mild or unilateral hearing loss and found that less than two-thirds of those children wore amplification on a regular basis. An older study by Vesterager and Parving (1995) also found more frequent HA use (>8 hr per day) in children with severe hearing loss compared with mild hearing loss. In this study, trends in HA use were not influenced by degree of hearing loss in the preschool-age group, whereas the school-age cohort did show such an effect. It is possible that there is less oversight for HA use as children with mild hearing loss enter elementary grades. It is also possible that school-age children with mild hearing loss (and their parents) may begin to identify situations in which they feel they do not need amplification, such as on the weekends, after school, or in social settings. It is unclear what effect limited HA use has on functional outcomes for school-age children with mild hearing loss or unilateral loss, given their significant amount of residual hearing. Future directions should address this gap in the research literature.

Back to Top | Article Outline

Limitations and Future Directions

Although this is the first article to examine longitudinal trends in HA use in a large sample of CHH, there are several limitations that should be highlighted. Potentially, progression of hearing loss could be a factor that contributes to increased HA use, given the finding that poorer hearing relates to increased HA use (Walker et al. 2013; Muñoz et al. 2014). We did not examine progression of hearing loss as a predictor of trends in HA use, however, because of the low number of children who demonstrated a significant decline in thresholds from the first to last visit. Dividing the low number of children with progressive hearing loss into age groups would further decrease statistical power and limit interpretability of results. Future studies could include a closer examination of children with progressive hearing loss, to determine trends in HA use within this specific population.

Another limitation is that the cluster analysis and logistic regression relied on parent-report measures of HA use, rather than the objective information supplied by data logging, because the sample had an insufficient amount of data logging across all visits. This article clearly demonstrates that most parents overestimate the amount of time their child wears HAs compared with data logging. Objective data logging measures would appear to be preferable when recording amount of daily HA use. Future research should incorporate data logging measures in investigations of longitudinal HA use, and whenever possible, audiologists and related professionals should rely on data logging as a measure of daily HA use.

A third limitation is that we are unable to ascertain the effects of intervention (e.g., audiology, speech-language pathology, early intervention) on HA use. We collected online surveys from the children’s managing audiologists, speech-language pathologists, and teachers of the deaf/hard of hearing, but we did not query the service providers about their general strategies with regards to HA compliance. We also did not contact children’s educational audiologists, who may have been able to provide additional information regarding HA compliance issues. Thus, we were unable to determine how intervention influences daily HA use, or how specific service providers may influence HA compliance in the pediatric population. Future research should address intervention strategies for counseling parents about HA use, particularly those families who are struggling with HA compliance.

Finally, a fourth limitation is that families in the OCHL study volunteered to participate in this longitudinal project. Like many longitudinal samples, this sample tended to be biased toward higher socioeconomic status and was not representative of income or educational status of the US population as a whole. This suggests the possibility that our findings of a strong effect of maternal education levels on longitudinal HA use actually underestimate these influences in the greater population. There remains a need for more research on alternative counseling and support strategies for families of diverse economic backgrounds, as these findings suggest that current approaches may not be working well for less-advantaged families.

Back to Top | Article Outline

SUMMARY

In 2007, the Joint Committee on Infant Hearing advocated for more research directed toward improving the effectiveness of intervention for children with hearing loss. One logical area of research is to identify malleable factors that may influence developmental outcomes. Malleable factors include any family- or child-specific variables that are amenable to change as part of intervention. In many previous studies, researchers focused on factors, such as age at identification, age at amplification, or type of intervention. In this article, we focused on HA use time as a malleable factor, an issue that has received little attention in past research. Results indicated that the majority of CHH increase HA use over time, but a sizable minority remain limited users and some actually decrease usage over time. Family-specific variables influenced trends in longitudinal HA use in younger children (i.e., maternal education level) and child-specific variables influenced trends in longitudinal HA use in school-age children (i.e., degree of hearing loss). Another article in this OCHL Supplement examines the influence of longitudinal HA use on language outcomes. As shown in this subsequent article (Tomblin et al. 2015a, this issue, pp. 76S–91S), children with greater amounts of HA use achieve better developmental outcomes, because of enhanced cumulative linguistic experience.

Back to Top | Article Outline

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research was supported by the following grants from the National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders: R01DC009560 and R01DC013591.

Back to Top | Article Outline

REFERENCES

Ambrose S. E., Walker E. A., Berry L. M. U., et al. Quantity and quality of caregivers’ linguistic input to 18-month and 3-year-old children who are hard of hearing. Ear Hear. (2015);36:48S–59S
Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. (1988) Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers
Davis J. M., Shepard N. T., Stelmachowicz P. G., et al. Characteristics of hearing-impaired children in the public schools: Part II–psychoeducational data. J Speech Hear Disord. (1981);46:130–137
Davis J. M., Elfenbein J., Schum R., et al. Effects of mild and moderate hearing impairments on language, educational, and psychosocial behavior of children. J Speech Hear Disord. (1986);51:53–62
Desjardin J. L.. Maternal perceptions of self-efficacy and involvement in the auditory development of young children with prelingual deafness. J Early Interv. (2005);27:193–209
Elfenbein J. L., Hardin-Jones M. A., Davis J. M.. Oral communication skills of children who are hard of hearing. J Speech Hear Res. (1994);37:216–226
Fenson L., Bates E., Dale P., et al. Measuring variability in early child language: Don’t shoot the messenger. Child Dev. (2000);71:323–328
Fitzpatrick E. M., Durieux-Smith A., Whittingham J.. Clinical practice for children with mild bilateral and unilateral hearing loss. Ear Hear. (2010);31:392–400
Fitzpatrick E. M., Crawford L., Ni A., et al. A descriptive analysis of language and speech skills in 4- to 5-yr-old children with hearing loss. Ear Hear. (2011);32:605–616
Iglowstein I., Jenni O. G., Molinari L., et al. Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: Reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics. (2003);111:302–307
Joint Committee on Infant Hearing.Joint Committee on Infant Hearing.. Year 2007 position statement: Principles and guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs. Pediatrics. (2007);120:898–921
Jones C., Launer S.Seewald R. C., Bamford J. M.. Pediatric fittings in 2010: The Sound Foundations Cuper project. In A Sound Foundation through Early Amplification: Proceedings of the 2010 International Conference. (2011) Chicago, IL Phonak AG:187–192
Jones C., Feilner M.R. C. Seewald, J. M. Bamford. What do we know about the fitting and daily life usage of hearing instruments in pediatrics? In A Sound Foundation through Early Amplification: Proceedings of the 2013 International Conference. (2013) Chicago, IL Phonak AG:97–103
Koehlinger K. M., Van Horne A. J., Moeller M. P.. Grammatical outcomes of 3- and 6-year-old children who are hard of hearing. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2013);56:1701–1714
Marnane V., Ching T. Y.. Hearing aid and cochlear implant use in children with hearing loss at three years of age: Predictors of use and predictors of changes in use. Int J Audiol. (2015);54:544–551
Marttila T. I., Karikoski J. O.. Hearing aid use in Finnish children–impact of hearing loss variables and detection delay. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. (2006);70:475–480
McCreery R. W., Walker E., Spratford M., et al. Longitudinal predictors of aided speech audibility in infants and children. Ear Hear. (2015);36:24S–37S
Moeller M. P., Hoover B., Peterson B., et al. Consistency of hearing aid use in infants with early-identified hearing loss. Am J Audiol. (2009);18:14–23
Muñoz K., Preston E., Hicken S.. Pediatric hearing aid use: How can audiologists support parents to increase consistency? J Am Acad Audiol. (2014);25:380–387
Muñoz K., Olson W. A., Twohig M. P., et al. Pediatric hearing aid use: Parent-reported challenges. Ear Hear. (2015);36:279–287
Sininger Y. S., Grimes A., Christensen E.. Auditory development in early amplified children: Factors influencing auditory-based communication outcomes in children with hearing loss. Ear Hear. (2010);31:166–185
Stiles D. J., Bentler R. A., McGregor K. K.. The speech intelligibility index and the pure-tone average as predictors of lexical ability in children fit with hearing AIDS. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2012);55:764–778
Tomblin J. B., Oleson J. J., Ambrose S. E., et al. The influence of hearing aids on the speech and language development of children with hearing loss. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (2014);140:43–49
Tomblin J. B., Harrison M., Ambrose S. E., et al. Language outcomes in young children with mild to severe hearing loss. Ear Hear. (2015a);36:76S–91S
Tomblin J. B., Walker E.A., McCreery R.W., et al. Outcomes of children with hearing loss: Data collection and methods. Ear Hear. (2015b);36:14S–23S
Vesterager V., Parving A.. Rehabilitation of hearing-impaired children: Intervention and outcome. Audiology. (1995);34:207–217
Walker E. A., Spratford M., Moeller M. P., et al. Predictors of hearing aid use time in children with mild-to-severe hearing loss. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. (2013);44:73–88
Ward J. H. Jr.. Hierarchical grouping to optimize an objective function. J Am Statist Assoc. (1963);58:236–244

Audibility; Children; Hard of hearing; Hearing aids

Supplemental Digital Content

Back to Top | Article Outline
Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.