Skip Navigation LinksHome > July/August 2014 - Volume 35 - Issue 4 > Bilateral Versus Unilateral Cochlear Implants in Children: A...
Ear & Hearing:
doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000022
Research Articles

Bilateral Versus Unilateral Cochlear Implants in Children: A Study of Spoken Language Outcomes

Sarant, Julia1; Harris, David2; Bennet, Lisa1; Bant, Sharyn1

Open Access
Supplemental Author Material
Article Outline
Collapse Box

Author Information

1Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia; and 2Department of Econometrics and Business Statistics, Monash University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia.

Supplemental digital content is available for this article. Direct URL citations appear in the printed text and are provided in the HTML and text of this article on the journal’s Web site ( www.ear-hearing.com).

This research was funded by an Australian Research Council Linkage Grant (Grant number LP0989391), and Cochlear Ltd.

The authors declare no other conflict of interest.

Address for correspondence: Julia Sarant, Audiology, Hearing and Speech Sciences, 550 Swanston Street, The University of Melbourne, Victoria 3010, Australia. E-mail: jsarant@unimelb.edu.au

Collapse Box

Abstract

Objectives:

Although it has been established that bilateral cochlear implants (CIs) offer additional speech perception and localization benefits to many children with severe to profound hearing loss, whether these improved perceptual abilities facilitate significantly better language development has not yet been clearly established. The aims of this study were to compare language abilities of children having unilateral and bilateral CIs to quantify the rate of any improvement in language attributable to bilateral CIs and to document other predictors of language development in children with CIs.

Design:

The receptive vocabulary and language development of 91 children was assessed when they were aged either 5 or 8 years old by using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (fourth edition), and either the Preschool Language Scales (fourth edition) or the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (fourth edition), respectively. Cognitive ability, parent involvement in children’s intervention or education programs, and family reading habits were also evaluated. Language outcomes were examined by using linear regression analyses. The influence of elements of parenting style, child characteristics, and family background as predictors of outcomes were examined.

Results:

Children using bilateral CIs achieved significantly better vocabulary outcomes and significantly higher scores on the Core and Expressive Language subscales of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (fourth edition) than did comparable children with unilateral CIs. Scores on the Preschool Language Scales (fourth edition) did not differ significantly between children with unilateral and bilateral CIs. Bilateral CI use was found to predict significantly faster rates of vocabulary and language development than unilateral CI use; the magnitude of this effect was moderated by child age at activation of the bilateral CI. In terms of parenting style, high levels of parental involvement, low amounts of screen time, and more time spent by adults reading to children facilitated significantly better vocabulary and language outcomes. In terms of child characteristics, higher cognitive ability and female sex were predictive of significantly better language outcomes. When family background factors were examined, having tertiary-educated primary caregivers and a family history of hearing loss were significantly predictive of better outcomes. Birth order was also found to have a significant negative effect on both vocabulary and language outcomes, with each older sibling predicting a 5 to 10% decrease in scores.

Conclusions:

Children with bilateral CIs achieved significantly better vocabulary outcomes, and 8-year-old children with bilateral CIs had significantly better language outcomes than did children with unilateral CIs. These improvements were moderated by children’s ages at both first and second CIs. The outcomes were also significantly predicted by a number of factors related to parenting, child characteristics, and family background. Fifty-one percent of the variance in vocabulary outcomes and between 59 to 69% of the variance in language outcomes was predicted by the regression models.

Back to Top | Article Outline

INTRODUCTION

Although many children with unilateral cochlear implants (CIs) have excellent speech perception abilities in a controlled listening environment such as a quiet room or sound-proof booth (Sarant et al. 2001; Leigh et al. 2008) these environments do not represent listening conditions in the real world. In more difficult listening conditions, such as noisy classrooms or playgrounds and the family home, children with a unilateral CI and a severe to profound or profound hearing loss in the contralateral ear will experience significant difficulties, which will reduce the amount and quality of speech they are exposed to. Understanding speech that is soft, speech in background noise, and locating sound sources such as speakers in a group conversation are examples of such difficulties. With these perceptual limitations, it is less likely that children with unilateral CIs will have the ability to learn incidentally through “overhearing,” as do children with normal hearing, which limits their acquisition of language, world knowledge, and social skills. Although many children with CIs have been able to develop spoken language and other skills that would not have been possible with conventional hearing aids, it has been well-documented for many years through to the present time that many children with unilateral CIs show delays in the development of language (Blamey et al. 2001: Geers 2002, Nittrouer et al. 2012), speech production (Tobey et al. 2003; Connor et al. 2006; Spencer et al. 2011), literacy (Crosson & Geers 2001; Marschark et al. 2007; Geers & Hayes 2011), academic (Spencer et al. 2003; Mukari et al. 2007) and social skills (Bat-Chava et al. 2005; Hintermair 2006). Although a number of children with unilateral CIs have been able to achieve age-appropriate development in many of these areas (Spencer et al. 2004; Percy-Smith et al. 2008; Duchesne et al. 2009), for a significant number of these children developmental delays have been maintained or increased through to adulthood (Moeller et al. 2007; Mukari et al. 2007; Uziel et al. 2007; Geers et al. 2008). For this reason, the efficacy of bilateral CIs is being investigated, and bilateral cochlear implantation is becoming the standard of care for children with severe to profound hearing loss in developed countries around the world (National Institute on Deafness and other Communication Disorders, 2011).

Back to Top | Article Outline
Perceptual Benefits of Bilateral CIs

Bilateral CIs offer additional benefits over a unilateral CI through the mechanisms of binaural redundancy (speech perception is improved with 2 ears, as the brain has 2 opportunities to process the signal), binaural summation (the signal when combined from 2 ears is slightly louder than from 1 ear), and the head-shadow effect (the head acts as a physical barrier to the sound, such that the signal will be softer at the ear that is farthest from the sound source). The benefits of bilateral CIs for speech perception in children have been evaluated in both noisy and quiet listening conditions. In noise, many studies have reported a significant improvement in children’s abilities to perceive speech (Litovsky et al. 2006b; Galvin et al. 2008; Johnston et al. 2009; Lovett et al. 2010). In quiet listening conditions, improved speech perception has also been reported (Scherf et al. 2007; Zeitler et al. 2008). Advantages of bilateral CIs for sound localization are not quite as clear, with some children reported to localize sound well (Litovsky et al. 2006a; Lovett et al. 2010), and others demonstrating more limited localization ability (i.e., left–right lateralization, rather than true localization (Galvin et al. 2008; Grieco-Calub & Litovsky 2010). Many other children, particularly older children, have shown no ability to localize sound (Galvin et al. 2007). Further benefits have also been documented, with parents in some studies frequently reporting superior performance using bilateral CIs in everyday life, in situations such as group conversations, background noise, and hearing at a distance (Sparreboom et al. 2012; Galvin et al. in press). There is also objective evidence that for some children with bilateral CIs, listening effort is reduced, suggesting that more attention can then be paid to the learning process (Hughes & Galvin 2013). Despite the above-cited evidence of benefit for children from bilateral CIs, it is yet to be determined whether these perceptual benefits facilitate significantly better broader outcomes in children with bilateral CIs, and if so, to quantify the degree of benefit received by children based on factors such as age at second implant.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Effect of Bilateral CIs on Language Outcomes

Until recently, most of the research on outcomes for children with bilateral CIs was focused on speech perception and sound localization benefits. Evidence regarding whether bilateral CIs significantly improve broader outcomes such as language, literacy, academic and social skills, and overall quality of life is lacking, particularly regarding longer-term outcomes (Johnston et al. 2009; Sparreboom et al. 2010). The results of the few earlier studies comparing language outcomes for children with unilateral and bilateral CIs did not show a significant benefit to language development from bilateral implantation. One of the first studies to investigate the effect of bilateral CIs on vocabulary, receptive and expressive language of children assessed at 3.5 years of age (15 unilateral, 26 bilateral) concluded that the reported perceptual benefits from bilateral CI use “may not extend to generative language”(Nittrouer & Chappman 2009). A further study of language outcomes in preschool-age children implanted by 5 years of age (60 unilateral, 31 bilateral; average age 2 to 5 years) reported similarly that children with bilateral CIs did not receive a significant benefit to either receptive or expressive language development over children with unilateral CIs (Niparko et al. 2010). It was noted that this outcome may have been a result of the brief period of time that had elapsed between implantation of the second CI and follow-up. A third study of vocabulary and language development in children who had just completed preschool (13 unilateral, 14 bilateral) and were up to 6 years of age also reported that having bilateral CIs had no effect on language outcomes (Nittrouer et al. 2012). In considering the results of these three studies it is worth noting that most of the children were of preschool age and many had sequential CIs, and therefore had not had a long time to use their bilateral hearing to develop language. Two of the three studies also had relatively small sample sizes, which makes detecting a significant difference in performance (if it exists) difficult, given the large variance in language outcomes commonly reported (Spencer et al. 2003; Connor et al. 2006; Sarant et al. 2009).

Two recent reports from the same population of children contradict the results of the first three studies. A retrospective study of 288 children implanted by 5 years of age examined language outcomes each year over 3 years post-CI for up to 29 children with bilateral CIs, compared with up to 85 children with unilateral CIs, and up to 62 children with a CI and a hearing aid (Boons et al. 2012a). It was concluded that contralateral stimulation (with bilateral CIs or unilateral CI plus hearing aid) contributed to significantly improved language outcomes. An unspecified post hoc analysis separated the bilateral CI and hearing aid effects, with the finding that bilateral CIs led to better outcomes than unilateral CIs, and also than bimodal hearing (CI plus hearing aid). This study differed from two previous studies in that the children were slightly older (up to 6 years of age), although the number of children with bilateral CIs was not greater. A limitation of the study was that participating children used different CIs (Cochlear Ltd., Sydney, Australia or Advanced Bionics, Valencia, CA), which have recently been reported to give significantly different perceptual results (Lazard et al. 2012) and could therefore have affected language outcomes. Although the effect of child age at first CI was considered, with children implanted before the 2 years of age performing significantly better on all measures, the effect of age at second CI was not investigated.

The same researchers also compared the spoken language outcomes of a smaller sample of children selected from the larger retrospective study (25 unilateral and 25 bilateral; Boons et al. 2012b). The children were matched for several auditory features, sex, implantation age, lack of additional disabilities, a monolingual family background with normal-hearing parents, and educational setting. As for the larger study, it was reported that the performance of children with bilateral CIs on spoken language comprehension and expression was superior.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Contribution of This Study

Evidence of the impact of bilateral implantation on language outcomes is vital if evidence-based preoperative recommendations are to be made to parents considering CIs for their children. This evidence can also be used by governments around the world to make policy decisions on whether to fund bilateral implantation in children. The current evidence, as described earlier in the article, is limited and shows mixed findings. There is therefore a need for further research in this area, particularly with regard to longer-term outcomes, given that most studies to date have involved children of preschool age with limited CI experience.

The present study offers a further comparison of vocabulary and language outcomes in children using unilateral and bilateral CIs. It has the advantages of being prospective, and including older children than in previous studies, thus offering some insight into longer-term outcomes. It includes a moderate sample size of 91 children, with a greater number of children with bilateral CIs than in previous studies. Although the present study has a cross-sectional design, 84% of the country’s population was located in the area from which the study sample was recruited. Of the eligible children in this area, 51.6% were recruited to the study, consistent with reported recruitment rates in the last decade for epidemiological studies (Galea & Tracy 2007). The sample can therefore be considered to be reasonably representative of Australian children with CIs. The present study also expands upon previous research in that the effects of age at CI on language outcomes for both the first and second CI are evaluated. The effects of some parenting practices (family reading habits and child screen time) that have not previously been investigated in any studies of language outcomes in children with CIs and their relationships to language outcomes are also examined. Finally, the proportion of variance in language outcomes accounted for in the present study (up to 69%) is higher than in most previous studies.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Objectives
  1. To compare language abilities of children with unilateral and bilateral CI.
  2. To quantify the rate of any improvement in language attributable to a bilateral CI.
  3. To document other predictors of language development in children with CIs.
Back to Top | Article Outline

PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS

Participants

Ninety-one children 5 to 8 years of age were recruited from three CI clinics and three early-intervention centers in four states of the country, accounting for most of the country’s pediatric CI-related service organizations and major intervention centers. Eighty-four percent of the country’s population is located in the area from which the study sample was recruited. Of the eligible children in this area, 51.6% were recruited to the study. This figure is consistent with reported recruitment rates in the last decade for epidemiological studies (Galea & Tracy 2007). Given the fact that the study cohort was recruited from 84% of the country’s population, with a recruitment rate similar to recently reported recruitment rates for epidemiological studies, these results can be considered to be reasonably representative of Australian children with CIs.

The study cohort consisted of 44 boys and 47 girls. All children were implanted early (first CI by 3.5 years of age and second CI, if bilaterally implanted, by 6 years of age), spoke English as their primary language, and had normal cognitive abilities. The age at CI criteria were chosen based on physiological studies that suggest that in the absence of normal auditory stimulation there is a period of about 3.5 years during which the central auditory system retains its maximum plasticity, and that this can extend in some children up to the age of approximately 6 to 7 years, after which it is significantly reduced (Sharma et al. 2002, 2005). Of the 91 children, 67 used bilateral CIs and 24 used a unilateral CI. All children used a CI from Cochlear Ltd., with Advanced Combined Encoder speech processing strategy. With the exception of 7 participants, for whom information regarding the number of active electrodes was unavailable at the time of writing, all children had between 19 to 22 active electrodes in their arrays. Twelve children had been reimplanted. Mean lengths of device use at the time of language assessment were 5.20 years (SD = 1.79) for the bilateral group and 4.55 years (SD = 2.04) for the unilateral group. Hearing aid use, both before and after, CI1 (if applicable) for all children was documented through parent interviews (see Table 1).

TABLE 1.
TABLE 1.
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Demographic Measures

Table 1 provides demographic information. Of the 91 children, 38 presented with a family history of hearing loss, although only 2 children had deaf parents. Of these 38 children, 25 had a genetic cause of hearing loss, 1 had a viral cause, and the etiology for 12 children was unknown. Thirty-five of the children had a hearing loss of a genetic origin and 42 had a hearing loss of an unknown cause. The remaining 14 children presented with etiologies resulting from viral causes or medical complications at birth. Further information about child development was collected relating to birth order, birth weight, age at which children first walked, and history of concerns with fine motor skill development. Only 3 children had a diagnosed additional disability that may have impacted on their ability to learn language. The communication mode for all families was primarily spoken language, with 2 children using supplementary sign to communicate with immediate family members with a profound hearing loss.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Procedure

The children in this study were part of a wider study examining outcomes for children with CIs. In accordance with the wider project’s protocol, the children were assessed at 5 and 8 years of age. To ensure that children were entered into the analyses only once, the most recently collected data for each child was entered. Of the bilateral group, 41 of the children were assessed at 5 years and 26 were assessed at 8 years. Of the unilateral group, 15 were assessed at 5 years and 9 were assessed at 8 years. For each assessment point, language outcomes were measured using the standardized language tests described in Instruments by speech language pathologists. The cognitive ability of all children was assessed at 5 years of age by an educational psychologist using either the Wechsler Non-Verbal Scale of Ability (86 children; WNV; Wechsler & Naglieri 2006) or the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Third Edition (5 children; WPPSI-III; Wechsler 2002). Normal cognitive ability was defined as a Performance Scale score of 80 or more.

A questionnaire was designed specifically for this study to form a general picture of children’s reading habits (see Questionnaire, Supplemental Digital Content 1, http://links.lww.com/EANDH/A133). Part of the questionnaire was based on items used in the study Growing Up In Australia: The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (Australian Institute of Family Studies 2012). In addition, questions specific to the aims of the present study were developed. The questionnaire was administered using a Web-based form in the first instance. Where no response was received within a predetermined time frame, questionnaires were sent via mail or filled out during a telephone interview with a member of the research team.

Parental involvement in each child’s intervention program was assessed using the Moeller’s Family Rating Scale (MFRS; Moeller 2000). Whether or not children had been slow in fine motor skill development, whether parents were tertiary educated, and whether there was a family history of hearing loss, difficulty learning to read or learning to speak were documented. All of this demographic information was obtained through a telephone interview with the primary caregiver.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Instruments
Back to Top | Article Outline
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition (Australian Standardized Edition)

The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—fourth edition (PPVT-4; Dunn & Dunn 2007) is a norm-referenced (mean = 100, SD = 15), closed-set test of receptive vocabulary that can be used from the ages of 2.6 to 90+ years of age. Children are required to point to one of four pictures that best represents the meaning of a verbally presented stimulus word. All children were assessed using the PPVT-4, and their scores were compared with normative data using standard scores. The average reliability coefficient for this test, based on the normative sample, is 0.89.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Preschool Language Scale—4 (Australian Language Adaptation)

The Preschool Language Scale—4 (PLS-4; Zimmerman et al. 2002) was used to assess the receptive and expressive language skills of 5-year-old children. This assessment is norm-referenced (mean = 100, SD = 15), can be used from age of 0 to 6 years, 11 months, and uses various stimulus materials such as toys, pictures, and verbal prompts to elicit responses. Performance on this test is divided into subscale scores for Auditory Comprehension, Expressive Communication, and a Total Language Score. Each child’s score is then compared with normative data using standard scores. The reliability coefficients for this test, based on the normative sample, range from 0.81 to 0.95.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Fourth Edition (CELF-4; Australian Standardized Edition)

The Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, fourth edition (CELF-4; Semel et al. 2006) was used to assess the receptive, expressive, and global language development of 8-year-old children. This test is designed for use between the ages of 5 to 21 years. For the purpose of this study, the children completed subtests that provided a measure of their skills for Core Language, Receptive Language, and Expressive Language. Each child’s standard score was compared with norm-referenced age-based standard scores (mean = 100, SD = 15). The reliability coefficients, based on the normative sample, are 0.69 to 0.91 for the subtests and 0.87 to 0.95 for the composite scores.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Wechsler Non-Verbal Scale of Ability

The nonverbal cognitive skills of 86 children were assessed using the Wechsler Non-Verbal Scale of Ability (WNV). The WNV is a cognitive assessment that uses pictorial directions rather than language-based instructions, making it a suitable tool for children with hearing loss, perceptual difficulties, or language delay. The test is norm-referenced and provides standard score measures of nonverbal cognitive skills. It has a full-scale score reliability of 0.91.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Wechsler Preschool & Primary Scale of Intelligence—Third Edition

The Performance IQ scale of the WPPSI—III was used to measure the nonverbal intelligence of 5 children. The Performance IQ scale examines nonverbal cognitive skills through the use of block design, matric reasoning, and picture concepts. The test is norm-referenced and provides scaled score measures of nonverbal cognitive skills. The reliability coefficients for the WPPSI-III United States composite scales range from 0.89 to 0.95.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Moeller’s Family Rating Scale

The MFRS was used to determine the quality of parental participation in each child’s intervention and educational programs. Two professionals or interventionists (e.g., teachers of the deaf, teachers, or early-intervention specialists) were asked to rate the family’s participation in the child’s early intervention or specialist programs in the year before the time of assessment. Each family was rated on a scale from 1 to 5 (1 = limited participation through to 5 = ideal participation). Raters were asked to base their ratings on specific descriptions or characteristics that represented each participation category. Raters were also asked to estimate the confidence in their own ratings as questionable, okay, or good. Rater’s scores were averaged to give an overall rating. If the raters specified different confidence levels, a weighted average was calculated, as specified by Moeller (2000). The MFRS was administered at the time of each child’s language assessment.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Statistical Analysis
Back to Top | Article Outline
PPVT Linear Regression Analysis

The sample of PPVT results for 91 children who were 5 and 8 years of age contained considerable variation in the lengths of time the children had used unilateral and bilateral CIs (see second panel of Table 1). The information in this variation was exploited in a regression to estimate the rate at which language ability accumulated over time, and whether this rate was different for children with unilateral and bilateral CIs. The language accumulation regression model had the general form:

(1)
(1)
Image Tools

in which X1,i, X2,i, and X3,i each represent predictors respectively chosen from the parenting style, child characteristics, and family background variables in Table 1. For a bilateral child, the variable “bilat yrs” is the length of time (in years) between activation of bilateral CIs and the date of the PPVT assessment, while “unilat yrs” is the length of time between activations of unilateral and bilateral CIs. Only unilat yrs is relevant for a child with a unilateral CI, and is the length of time between unilateral activation and the PPVT assessment.

Equation (1) relates language ability to years of unilateral CI use and, if applicable, years of bilateral CI use. It also allows the effect of each CI to vary according to its activation age. For example, the predicted marginal change in PPVT score due to an additional year spent with bilateral CIs, holding all other variables constant, is given by α1 + α2 age CI2i. It is hypothesized that α1 > 0, so that each year spent with bilateral CIs is beneficial for language, and α2 < 0, so that bilateral CIs have greater effect when implanted in younger children. The same interpretations apply to α3 and α4 for the unilateral CI. Inference can therefore be carried out to compare the rate of language accumulation with bilateral CIs (α1 + α2 age CI2i) with the rate of language accumulation with a unilateral CI (α3 + α4 age CI1i), controlled for all the other child, parent, and family characteristics.

Given the moderate sample sizes available and the large number of possible predictors that could be included in Eq. (1), a regression specification was chosen to minimize the Akaike information criterion (AIC), which is a bias-corrected estimator of the Kullback-Leibler divergence between the distribution implied by a statistical model and the distribution of the data (Akaike 1974; Claeskens & Hjort 2008). Specifically, each possible specification of the CI years variables (bilat yrsi, age CI2i,unilat yrsi, age CI1i), hearing aid use (X1,i), parenting variables (X1,i), child characteristics (X2,i), and family background variables (X3,i) was estimated, and the specification with minimum AIC selected. The statistical analyses were conducted using Eviews v7.1. Quantitative Micro Software, 2010.

Back to Top | Article Outline
PLS-4 and CELF-4 Linear Regression Analyses

For each PLS-4 and CELF-4 language outcome, a regression was specified for average test scores for children with and without bilateral CIs, controlling for parenting, child, and family characteristics. The differences between predicted test scores with and without the bilateral CI were analyzed to evaluate the effectiveness of the bilateral CI. The language accumulation form of Eq. (1) for the combined sample of 5- and 8-year olds was inapplicable when these age groups were separated. Instead, for a language outcome Yi, the regression model was

(2)
(2)
Image Tools

with X1,i, X2,i, X3,i, and X4,i defined in Eq. (1). The coefficient β1 allowed the average language outcome to differ between children with unilateral and bilateral CIs. In addition, the distinct coefficients β2 and β3 on the interaction terms allowed the language effect of age of first CI to differ between children with unilateral and bilateral CIs. This flexibility is potentially important because age CI1 was, on average, significantly lower for the children with bilateral CIs, and its effect may differ between the two groups. The coefficient β4 allowed the language effect of bilateral CIs to depend on the age of its activation.

To interpret Eq. (2), consider the average effect of a bilateral CI activated at age a2. The structure of Eq. (2) allows for the possibility that this effect depends on the age of first CI, denoted a1. For given parenting, child, and family characteristics x1, x2, x3, the predicted language score with the bilateral CI is


Image Tools

while for a unilateral CI it is


Image Tools

The difference between these predictions


Image Tools

is a measure of the effectiveness of the second CI. This decomposes the predicted difference in language due to bilateral CIs into three components: (1) a constant effect β1, (2) any difference in the effect of age of first implant (β3 − β2) a1, and (3) the effect of the age of the second implant β4a2. These individual components can be tested and interpreted based on the individual coefficients. The overall effect of bilateral CIs, however, may depend on the activation ages, so inference proceeded by computing a “bilateral activation window,” which was the range of ages within which the activation of a bilateral CI produced a significant and positive effect on the language outcome. For a given unilateral activation age a1, the window was defined to be the range [a1, a2], where a2 was the oldest bilateral activation age such that the 95% confidence interval for the overall bilateral effect ybiyuni contained only positive values. The window was empty if bilateral CIs had no significant positive effects.

Back to Top | Article Outline

RESULTS

Bilateral / Unilateral Differences of Means

Table 2 reports the difference of means t tests for each of the seven language outcomes, allowing for unequal variances between the two groups. A significant difference (p = 0.004) was found for the PPVT results, with children with bilateral CIs scoring an average of 9.36 points (10.98%) higher than children with unilateral CIs. Neither the PLS-4 nor CELF-4 mean scores revealed significant differences between children with bilateral and unilateral CIs without controlling for other variables.

TABLE 2.
TABLE 2.
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Vocabulary (PPVT)

Mean scores for both groups of children with unilateral and bilateral CIs were within 1 SD of the mean for typically developing children with normal hearing, although this was the case for the former group only by a narrow margin. As is commonly observed (Blamey et al. 2001; Sarant & Garrard 2013), variability in scores was high, with some children scoring within or above the average range, and others well below it.

Table 3 gives results of the regression to predict bilateral/unilateral vocabulary differences while controlling for the parenting, child, and family characteristics listed in Table 1. The language accumulation specification for the PPVT score revealed that the number of years spent with bilateral CIs was a highly significant (p < 0.001) predictor, with its effect moderated by bilateral activation age (p = 0.033). For example, a bilateral CI activated at 2.93 years of age (the mean activation age for this sample) was estimated to result in a significant (p = 0.004) rate of language improvement of 3.95 – 0.69 × 2.93 = 1.93 points (95% confidence interval = [0.62–3.23], 2.10% of the unilateral average) for each year thereafter. The joint semipartial R2 of the two bilateral variables was 0.09 of a total R2 of 0.51. The variables measuring the unilateral CI years were not found to be significant, being excluded by the AIC from the final specification.

TABLE 3.
TABLE 3.
Image Tools

Two of the parenting predictors were found to be significant. Parental involvement, as measured by the MFRS, was significant (p = 0.013). A 1 SD increase (0.66) in the MFRS score predicted a 0.66 × 5.44 = 3.59 point ([0.78–6.41], 3.90%) increase in the PPVT outcome. Adult reading time was also a significant predictor (p = 0.048), with an extra hour per week predicting an increase of 60 × 0.050 = 3.00 points ([0.03–5.94], 3.26%).

Children’s birth order was found to be a highly significant predictor of vocabulary outcomes (p = 0.001), and contributed 0.07 to the overall R2. Each older sibling predicted a decreased PPVT score by 4.65 points ([1.85–7.45], 5.05%). The education level of the primary caregiver was a highly significant predictor of vocabulary outcomes (p = 0.001), with the child of a tertiary qualified parent predicted to have a score of 9.56 points ([3.83–15.28],10.38%) higher than otherwise.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Language

Tables 4 to 6 give the results of the regressions to predict bilateral/unilateral language differences while controlling for the parenting, child, and family characteristics listed in Table 1.

TABLE 4.
TABLE 4.
Image Tools
TABLE 5.
TABLE 5.
Image Tools
TABLE 6.
TABLE 6.
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline
Language Outcomes for 5-Year-Old Children (PLS-4)

Group mean scores for the unilateral group of children on all three subtests of the PLS-4 were well below the average range for typically developing children, and there was great variability between children in outcomes.

Having bilateral CIs was not an individually significant predictor of outcomes in any of the PLS-4 regressions. The activation age of bilateral CIs, however, was a significant predictor of Total Language and Auditory Comprehension scores (p = 0.002, 0.010). The coefficients were negative, implying that earlier bilateral implantation predicted improved language outcomes; a reduction of 1 year in age at bilateral CI predicted an increase of 5.96 points ([2.31–9.61], 6.25%) on Total Language scores and 4.80 points ([1.20–8.39], 4.60%) on Auditory Comprehension scores. The effect of age at first CI followed a qualitatively similar pattern for the unilateral children for all three PLS subscales (p = 0.000, 0.000, 0.000 for Total Language, Auditory Comprehension, and Expressive Language, respectively). For a child with a unilateral CI, a reduction of 1 year in age at implantation predicted increases of 13.82 points ([8.80–18.84], 19.94%), 11.37 points ([6.44–16.32], 17.10%), and 14.50 points ([9.62–19.39], 19.40%) on the Total Language, Auditory Comprehension, and Expressive Language scores, respectively. The activation age at first CI contributed most (0.22, 0.17, 0.25 for Total Language, Auditory Comprehension, and Expressive Language) to the overall R2 of each regression, while the significant contributions of the activation age at second CI were 0.08 (Total Language) and 0.06 (Auditory Comprehension).

The results for the joint inference based on the bilateral activation windows are given in Table 7. The results for the Total Language and Auditory Comprehension subscales show that bilateral CIs predicted significant language improvements by 5 years of age, provided they were activated early enough. As with the CELF Receptive Language scores, the windows became wider as the age at first CI increased. The windows based on PLS-4 scores were narrower than those based on CELF-4 scores for the older children, implying that bilateral CI activation needs to occur early to give sufficient time to yield significant language improvement by 5 years of age. The absence of significant bilateral variables in the PLS-4 Expressive Language equation translated to an empty bilateral activation window—there was no evidence of improved Expressive Language scores for children with bilateral CIs at any activation age.

TABLE 7.
TABLE 7.
Image Tools

Two of the parenting predictors were found to be significant. The adult reading variable was significant in all three equations (p = 0.012, 0.014, 0.012 for Total Language, Auditory Comprehension, and Expressive Language, respectively). The marginal effect on the Total Language score of an extra hour per week of adult time spent reading to the child was an increase of 60 × 0.096 = 5.76 points ([1.33–10.17], 6.64%). The results of the Auditory Comprehension and Expressive Language equations were similar. Screen time was found to have a significant effect on Total Language and Expressive Language scores (p = 0.039, 0.025), with an extra hour of screen time per day predicting a decrease in Total Language scores of 2.57 points [0.13–5.00] and 2.71 points [0.36–5.06] for the Expressive score. A joint interpretation of these two parenting variables is that the predicted effect of a parenting intervention that reduces a child’s screen time by half an hour every weekday, and substitutes with half an hour of adult reading time (an extra 150 min per week), would be to significantly (p = 0.005) increase the Total Language score by 150 × 0.096 + 0.5 × 2.57 = 15.66 points ([5.01–26.30], 18.08%).

Two of the child characteristics were highly significant predictors of language outcomes on this test, and showed effects of a substantial magnitude. The effect of birth order was highly significant in each equation (p < 0.001), with each older sibling predicting a decrease in Total Language, Auditory Comprehension, and Expressive Language scores of 8.39 points [4.39–12.40], 8.13 points [4.19–12.08] and 7.18 points [3.29–11.06], respectively. Birth order also made a substantial contribution to the R2 of all three equations (0.13, 0.14, and 0.10 to Total Language, Auditory Comprehension, and Expressive Language, respectively). Sex was also significant in each equation (p = 0.010, 0.018, 0.006) with boys predicted to score 10.67 points [2.71–18.63], 9.55 points [1.71–17.38], and 10.92 points [3.24–18.60] lower on the Total Language, Auditory Comprehension, and Expressive Language scores, holding all other characteristics constant.

A family history of hearing loss was the only family history variable to be found significant in any of the equations. It was highly significant for all three subscales (p = 0.003, Total Language; 0.004, Auditory Comprehension; 0.010, Expressive Language) and had a positive effect on scores. For example, having a family history of hearing loss predicted a 14.21-point ([4.98–23.44], 16.39%) higher Total Language score, relative to a child with all the same characteristics but with no family history of hearing loss.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Language Outcomes for 8-Year-Old Children (CELF-4)

Language outcomes on both measures for the children in this study again showed great variability, as has been reported in previous studies using these measures (Spencer et al. 2003; Tobey et al. 2013). Group mean scores for all of the CELF-4 subtests were within 1 SD of the means for typically developing children, but there was great variability in results between children.

Bilateral CIs were a significant predictor for both the Core Language and Expressive language subscales of the PLS-4 (p = 0.004, 0.002, respectively), although not for the Receptive Language subscale. The magnitude of the effect was moderated by the age at which the second CI was activated (p = 0.007, 0.007). The predicted effect of a bilateral CI activation at the mean activation age of 2.94 years was 58.31 – 11.36 × 2.94 = 24.91 points [6.42–43.41] on the CELF Core Language subscale, or 28.06% higher than a unilateral child with all the same other characteristics. The same calculation for the CELF-4 Expressive Language subscale showed a 34% higher result (29.34 points, [11.78–46.89]) for a child with bilateral CIs implanted at the average age. For a child with bilateral CIs, each additional year of delay of implantation of the bilateral CI predicted a decrease in CELF Core Language and Expressive Language scores of 11.36 points ([3.47–19.25], 12.16%) and 10.83 points ([3.34–18.32], 11.40%), respectively.

The CELF-4 regressions accounted for between 59 to 65% of the variability in outcomes on this measure. The contributions of having bilateral CIs and the bilateral activation age to the predictive ability of the regressions are given by the semipartial R2 statistics in Table 3. These two variables were, by definition, highly collinear (correlation coefficient of 0.74), measuring two aspects of bilateral CIs. The overall predictive ability of bilateral CIs was therefore best evaluated using the joint semipartial R2 of the bilateral indicator and CI age, which were 0.20 (Core Language) and 0.22 (Expressive Language). That is, the overall R2 of 0.59 and 0.63 for the Core and Expressive regressions would decrease by 0.20 and 0.22, respectively, if the two bilateral CI variables were omitted.

The bilateral CI indicator was not selected in the CELF Receptive Language equation, but the child’s age at bilateral implantation was selected, and was statistically significant (p = 0.036), indicating that bilateral implantation is predictive of these outcomes, with a unique contribution of 8% of the variance in outcomes. A delay of 1 year in bilateral implantation predicted a 4.11 point ([0.29–7.93], 4.39%) decrease in the CELF Receptive Language score. Child age at first CI was found to be significant (p = 0.004) for children with unilateral CIs, but not for children with bilateral CIs. A delay of 1 year in first CI for a child with a unilateral CI predicted a 13.91 point ([5.07–22.75], 15.10%) decrease in CELF-4 Receptive Language score.

These results suggest that bilateral CIs had a statistically significant effect on CELF-4 language scores of the 8-year-old children in this study, the magnitude of which varied with the ages at which children received their CIs. The above-outlined prediction comparison approach provides a readily interpretable summary of the predicted effectiveness of bilateral CIs.

Table 6 presents results for several first implant activation ages. For the Core Language and Expressive Language subscale results, bilateral CIs were found to have a significant and positive effect, provided they were activated before ages of 3.75 and 4.33 years, respectively.

The results for the child, parent, and family characteristics were similar for the Core and Expressive Language subscales. Birth order was significant in both cases (p = 0.038, 0.010), with each extra older sibling predicting decreases of 8.97 points ([0.56, 17.39], 9.73%) and 10.83 points ([2.84, 18.82], 11.68%), respectively. Parent involvement was also significant for both measures (p = 0.032, 0.031). A 1 SD increase in parent involvement predicted a 7.29 point ([0.70–13.87], 7.94%) and 6.94 point ([0.69–13.19], 7.52%) increase in the Core Language and Expressive Language subscales, respectively. Children’s cognitive ability was significant for all three CELF-4 language subscales (p = 0.008, 0.003, 0.001 for Core Language, Expressive Language, and Receptive Language, respectively), with an extra 10 points of cognitive ability (slightly less than 1 SD), holding all else constant, predicting increases of 8.86 points ([2.57–15.16], 9.61%), 9.63 points ([3.66–15.60], 10.38%), and 9.20 points ([4.82–13.59], 9.66%), respectively. Cognitive ability also made an important contribution to the predictive power of the regressions, especially for the Receptive Language subscale, for which cognitive ability contributed 0.30 to the overall R2 of 0.65. The presence of fine motor problems was significant (p = 0.010, 0.005, 0.003), predicting higher scores of 31.91 points ([8.29–55.52], 34.61%), 33.84 points ([11.43–56.25], 36.48%), and 23.74 points ([9.06–38.41], 25.48%) for the Core, Expressive, and Receptive subscales, respectively.

Back to Top | Article Outline

DISCUSSION

Vocabulary Outcomes

The results of this study showed that children with bilateral CIs achieved significantly better vocabulary outcomes than did comparable children with unilateral CIs. This significant finding contrasts with those of the only other two studies, whose statistical power was likely reduced by reliance on samples containing relatively few children with sufficient bilateral CI experience to develop their listening skills and subsequent language ability.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Language Outcomes

In terms of the comparative performance of children with unilateral and bilateral CIs, the language results were not as unequivocal as the vocabulary outcomes, with no significant differences between the mean scores for unilateral and bilateral CI children for any of the CELF-4 and PLS-4 subtests. However, once other influential factors were controlled for in the regression analysis, bilateral CI use was found to predict significantly faster rates of language development in both of the PLS-4 and CELF-4 tests.

The weaker effect of bilateral CI use on the simple difference of means for the language measures is likely to reflect both the increased complexity of language skills required, the substantial length of time required to master these, and the fact that the sample sizes for both these measures were smaller than for the vocabulary measure, where results for both the 5- and 8-year-old children were included. An illustration of the need for more time to develop higher-level language skills can be seen in the Core Language and Expressive Language CELF-4 regression results, where bilateral CIs were found to have a significant and positive effect on scores, provided they were activated before 3.75 and 4.33 years of age, respectively. Activation of bilateral CIs after these ages may not have allowed sufficient time to produce a significant benefit relative to the unilateral implant for the 8-year-old participants of this study.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Effect of Bilateral Hearing on Vocabulary and Language Outcomes

It was speculated earlier that the perceptual benefits reported for children with bilateral CIs such as improved speech perception in both quiet and noisy listening conditions (Scherf et al. 2007; Zeitler et al. 2008), improved sound localization ability for some children (e.g., Lovett et al. 2010), and reduced listening effort (and therefore reduced tiredness and a greater ability to concentrate; Hughes & Galvin 2013) may facilitate a greater ability to access the spoken language of others and to learn from these increased opportunities. Despite the large variability in language outcomes, a moderate sample size, and multiple factors that influence these outcomes, significantly faster rates of vocabulary and language development were found for the children with bilateral CIs in this study. This finding suggests that the perceptual benefits of bilateral hearing through two CIs conferred a significant advantage, in terms of learning, to these children. It must be emphasized, however, that it is not known whether the improved results for children with bilateral CIs are due simply to having bilateral auditory input, or whether they are the result of true binaural processing. This study was not designed to address this question, but it will be an important area for future investigation.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Predictors of Outcomes
Back to Top | Article Outline
Age at First CI

Age at CI has been documented extensively a significant predictor of child language outcomes (Connor et al. 2006; Schorr et al. 2008; Geers et al. 2009), and marks the beginning of functional auditory input to the auditory cortex and the subsequent development of the auditory processing abilities that facilitate spoken language development. A surprising finding of this study was that neither age at first CI nor duration of unilateral CI use were significant predictors of PPVT scores. This does not imply that a unilateral implant is ineffective, but that there was insufficient information in this sample (with only 24 children with unilateral CIs) to precisely estimate the rate of vocabulary accumulation. It is also worth noting that for many children in this sample, 4 to 7 years had elapsed since their first CI implantation. Although several studies have reported better language outcomes with very early implantation (Dettman et al. 2007; Nicholas & Geers 2007), follow-up in these studies occurred when the children had limited experience with their CIs (Geers 2004). More recent findings have been mixed; one study has since reported that the effect of age at first implant reduces over time (Hay-McCutcheon et al. 2008), while a very recent study has shown that age at first CI is still a strong predictor of outcomes after approximately 8 years of CI use (Geers & Nicholas 2013).

Child age at first CI had expected effects for the PLS-4 and CELF-4 tests. Earlier age at first CI was predictive of a large increase (17 to 19% for each year) in PLS-4 scores across all subscales, and explained the greatest amount of variance in outcomes on this measure (22%). For the 8-year-old children, age at first CI was found to be a significant predictor of outcomes on the CELF-4 Receptive Language subtest only for children with unilateral CIs.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Age at Second CI

As far as the authors are aware, the effect of age at second CI on language outcomes has not been examined in any other studies. Here it was found to have an important effect, with earlier ages of second implantation predicting significant vocabulary and language improvements for both 5- and 8-year olds. This effect was quantified in different ways for the different tests.

For the PPVT test, vocabulary ability was shown to accumulate more quickly for children with bilateral CI compared with unilateral CI, but the magnitude of the difference was reduced with each year of delay before second implantation. Therefore early bilateral implantation would be recommended for maximum vocabulary benefit.

A bilateral CI predicted statistically significant language improvements on all PLS-4 and CELF-4 subtests except the CELF-4 Expressive, and the extent of these improvements was influenced by various interactions between the ages of first and second implant. These results are summarized in the bilateral activation age windows. For each subtest, the bilateral activation age window provides the range of ages within which the activation of a bilateral CI predicts a statistically significant advantage over a unilateral CI. That these windows exist for all but the CELF-4 Expressive subtest demonstrates the diverse benefits of the bilateral CI for both 5- and 8-year olds.

To illustrate, activation windows with fixed upper age limits were found for the CELF-4 Core (3.75 years) and Expressive (4.33 years) subtests, implying that bilateral implantation before these ages was sufficient to produce significant gains by 8 years of age, regardless of age of first CI (at least within the range in this sample). In all cases, the magnitude of these language gains decreased as age of bilateral CI increased. For example, a child receiving a bilateral CI at 2 years of age is predicted to score 35.60 points higher on the CELF-4 Core test relative to a unilateral CI child, but if the activation age were 3.75 years then this gain reduces to 15.72 points. Therefore, early bilateral implantation would be recommended for maximum language benefit.

Some bilateral activation age windows depended on the age of first CI. For example, the upper age limit for the CELF-4 Receptive Language subtest was found to vary with the age of first CI. If a child received their first implant at 1 year of age, then a subsequent bilateral implant received before 2.09 years of age predicted a significant and positive effect on the Receptive Language subscale score. If the first CI were received at 2 years of age, then the bilateral activation window extended to 4.18 years of age. This longer window for children with later age at first CI reflects the considerably larger implant age effect for the first CI relative to the second—a child first implanted at 2 years of age is acquiring hearing and language from a point substantially (predicted 15.10%) behind a child first implanted at 1 year of age; a deficiency that the bilateral implant can significantly reduce. Note, however, that the predicted language scores are always higher for children with lower implant ages, so this longer bilateral activation window does not imply better language scores for children implanted later, rather that a bilateral CI has the potential to help catch up some (not all) of the delay due to a delayed first implant.

Two of the three PLS-4 bilateral age activation windows were nonempty (Total Language and Auditory Comprehension), implying that a bilateral child will already be demonstrating improved language outcomes by 5 years of age, provided they receive their second implant sufficiently early. These activation windows were shorter than those for the CELF-4, implying that, compared with the 8-year olds, earlier bilateral implantation is required for bilateral CI children to show significant language gains relative to unilateral CI children by 5 years of age.

Overall, these age-at-CI results underscore the importance of having the shortest possible time between CIs for children who are sequentially implanted, and support the findings of physiological studies of abnormalities in spatial patterns of cortical brain activity in children implanted sequentially after a long time (Gordon et al. 2010), and theories of reduced central auditory plasticity at older ages (Sharma et al. 2002, 2005).

Back to Top | Article Outline
Parenting Style

The effect of parental involvement on both vocabulary and language development was found to be strongly predictive of better outcomes, as has been documented previously (Moeller 2000; Sarant et al. 2009). The amount of time adults in the family spent reading to their child, and the amount of time children spent watching a screen each week were identified as new factors that significantly affected both vocabulary and language outcomes. An example of a joint interpretation of these two parenting variables is that the predicted effect of a parenting intervention that reduced a child’s screen time by 30 min each weekday and substituted this with 30 min of adult-to-child reading time (150 extra min/week) would be a significant increase (p = 0.005) in the PLS-4 Total Language score by 15.66 points (18.08%; 150 × 0.096 + 0.5 × 2.57). This large predicted increase in scores demonstrates how effective relatively small changes in parenting can be in facilitating significant changes in children’s language development.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Child Characteristics

Birth order was found to have a significant effect on PPVT scores, with each older sibling decreasing scores by approximately 5%. It also exerted a significant negative effect on language scores for five of the six subscales of both language measures. This has been found to be the case in other studies of children with normal hearing, and likely relates to the demands on parents’ time and resources with increasing numbers of children (Hoff Ginsberg 1998; Nelson et al. 2006). Although it has also been found that older siblings can enrich aspects of younger children’s language development, it has also been reported that older children monopolize more parent–child conversations than their younger siblings (Wellen 1985; Oshima-Takane et al.1996). Children with significant hearing loss are likely to miss out on the enriching aspects of “overhearing” conversations due to their auditory limitations, and are also likely to be disadvantaged by the reduced number of opportunities to talk directly with their parents and learn language in this manner.

In common with other reports for both children with normal hearing (Fenson et al. 2000) and children with hearing loss (Moog & Geers 2003;Geers et al. 2009), male sex was significantly predictive of poorer language PLS-4 outcomes for 5-year-old children. However, this effect was no longer evident for the 8-year-old children on the CELF-4 results. This finding fits with reports that the gap in language ability found between boys and girls in the early life closes with increasing age (Gaddes & Crockett 1975; Ely 2005).

Nonverbal cognitive ability also significantly affected language outcomes for the 8-year-old children in this study. Cognitive ability has been identified as a primary predictive factor of language development in children with hearing loss, accounting for large proportions of the variance in outcomes in many studies (e.g., Holt & Kirk 2005; Geers et al. 2009; Sarant et al. 2010).

An unexpected and puzzling finding was that 8-year-old children who were reported by their parents to have had difficulties with fine motor development showed significantly better language development than children whose fine motor development had been viewed by their parents as normal. This finding contradicts those of other large studies that have shown a strong synchrony between fine motor development and language development (Bavin et al. 2008; Taylor 2010). Closer examination of the data showed that despite 14 parent reports of difficulty with fine motor skills, only 4 of these children were receiving assistance with the development of these skills. It is possible that the concern levels of these parents were inconsistent with the degree of delay the children were experiencing. While this explanation could account for the lack of a negative effect of this factor on language development, it does not account for the positive effect found. It could be that this predictor is a proxy for another factor that may be identified with a larger sample size and further research in the future.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Family Background

As has been reported previously (Dollaghan et al. 1999; Sarant et al. 2009; Niparko et al. 2010), there was a significant effect of maternal education level (which can be considered a reasonable proxy for socioeconomic status) on vocabulary scores, with children of tertiary-educated primary caregivers achieving significantly (approximately 10%) higher scores.

Having a family history of hearing loss also had a surprisingly large positive effect on language outcomes for 5-year-old children. This may reflect the value of having a family member with an understanding of the challenges of hearing loss and knowledge of strategies to access assistance in the early years of parenting a child with a hearing loss. While family histories of spoken language or reading difficulties have historically been reported as risk factors for language development (Nelson et al. 2006), the authors are unaware of any evidence regarding family history of hearing loss. It is interesting that this factor did not influence language outcomes for the 8-year-old children, where a high level of parent involvement accounted for 10% of the variance in outcomes. It may be that by the time children are 8 years old, parents have developed sophisticated parenting practices that exceed the effect of the knowledge possessed by parents with a family history of hearing loss, or simply that different predictors were found for these different groups of children.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Study Limitations

As this was a cross-sectional study, its findings may not be representative of other populations of children with CIs. However, given the fact that the study cohort was recruited from the majority of the country’s highly populated areas, and the fact that the recruitment rate was comparable with that of epidemiological studies, these results should be reasonably representative of Australian children with CIs. A further potential limitation of this study was the imbalance in numbers between children with unilateral and bilateral CIs, and the resulting limited representation of children with unilateral CIs. With larger numbers of children in the future, it is hoped that this limitation may be addressed.

Back to Top | Article Outline

CONCLUSIONS

Overall, bilateral CIs contributed to significantly better language outcomes for the children in this study, with improvements moderated by children’s ages at second CI. Earlier implantation, both unilateral and bilateral, was found to be beneficial to language outcomes. With larger sample sizes in the future, possible nonlinearities in implant age effects, such as whether there is a discontinuity between simultaneous and sequential implantation could also be investigated. Examining whether there is an asymptote to the negative effect of delaying implantation, in addition to determining a window of implant ages of maximum effectiveness, would provide valuable clinical information.

The influence of parenting style on language outcomes for children was also extremely important. This study identified parenting practices, not previously investigated in studies of language outcomes in children, that exerted a significant influence on language outcomes. All three parenting variables examined in this study were found (in different regressions) to have statistically and practically significant effects on language development. This could be a particularly important consideration for parents of hearing-impaired children with several older siblings, because the presence of these children may slow language development in a younger child with a CI.

Despite the large reported variability in language outcomes, the present study was able to account for a significant proportion of the variance in language outcomes (up to 69%), which was higher than that in many studies.

The findings of this study add significantly to the body of knowledge regarding outcomes for children with bilateral versus unilateral CIs, and also to that of factors that facilitate improved spoken language outcomes for children with CIs. It is imperative that this knowledge is accessible to, and is used by parents, clinicians, and governments in making evidence-based decisions that are translated into clinical practice and governmental policy development. In this way, it would be possible to most effectively assist the many children around the world for whom bilateral CIs can contribute to closing the gap, relative to those with normal hearing, in terms of language development, and hopefully, subsequent quality of life.

Back to Top | Article Outline

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors express their gratitude to the children and their families for their participation in this study. They also thank and acknowledge the collaboration and work of the following: The Melbourne Cochlear Implant Clinic at the Royal Victorian Eye and Ear Hospital, The Shepherd Centre, The Hear and Say Centre, The Cora Barclay Centre, The Sydney Cochlear Implant Centre, and the Audiology Department at the Adelaide Women’s and Children’s Hospital.

The authors also thank the LOCHI study for sharing cognitive and language data where participants overlapped between studies. They thank Jennifer Holland, Philippa Garrard and Laura Sinclair for their work on data collection.

Back to Top | Article Outline

REFERENCES

Akaike H.. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transact Auto Control. (1974); 19:716–723

Bat-Chava Y., Martin D., Kosciw J. G.. Longitudinal improvements in communication and socialization of deaf children with cochlear implants and hearing aids: Evidence from parental reports. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2005); 46:1287–1296

Bavin E. L., Prior M., Reilly S., et al. The Early Language in Victoria Study: Predicting vocabulary at age one and two years from gesture and object use. J Child Lang. (2008); 35:687–701

Blamey P. J., Sarant J. Z., Paatsch L. E., et al. Relationships among speech perception, production, language, hearing loss, and age in children with impaired hearing. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2001); 44:264–285

Boons T., Brokx J. P., Dhooge I., et al. Predictors of spoken language development following pediatric cochlear implantation. Ear Hear. (2012); 33:617–639

Boons T., Brokx J. P., Frijns J. H., et al. Effect of pediatric bilateral cochlear implantation on language development. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2012); 166:28–34

Claeskens G., Hjort N.L.. Model Selection and Model Averaging. (2008); 1st ed. New York, NY Cambridge University Press

Connor C. M., Craig H. K., Raudenbush S. W., et al. The age at which young deaf children receive cochlear implants and their vocabulary and speech-production growth: Is there an added value for early implantation? Ear Hear. (2006); 27:628–644

Crosson J., Geers A.. Analysis of narrative ability in children with cochlear implants. Ear Hear. (2001); 22:381–394

Dettman S. J., Pinder D., Briggs R. J., et al. Communication development in children who receive the cochlear implant younger than 12 months: Risks versus benefits. Ear Hear. (2007); 28:(2 Suppl)11S–18S

Dollaghan C. A., Campbell T. F., Paradise J. L., et al. Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (1999); 42:1432–1443

Duchesne L., Sutton A., Bergeron F.. Language achievement in children who received cochlear implants between 1 and 2 years of age: group trends and individual patterns. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. (2009); 14:465–485

Dunn L. M., Dunn D. M.. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. (2007); 4th Ed Minneapolis, MN NCS Pearson, Inc

Ely R.. J. B. Gleason. Language and literacy in the school years. The Development of Language. (2005); Boston, MA Allyn & Bacon 395–443

Fenson L., Pethick S., Renda C., et al. Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Appl Psycholinguist. (2000); 21:95–116

Gaddes W. H., Crockett D. J.. The Spreen-Benton aphasia tests, normative data as a measure of normal language development. Brain Lang. (1975); 2:257–280

Galea S., Tracy M.. Participation rates in epidemiologic studies. Ann Epidemiol. (2007); 17:643–653

Galvin K. L., Holland J. F., Hughes K. C.. Longer term functional outcomes and everyday listening performance for young children through to young adults using bilateral implants. Ear Hear.

Galvin K., Holland J. F., Hughes K. C.. Everyday listening performance and functional outcomes in the longer term for bilaterally implanted young children through to young adults: What do parents report after 3.5 years or more? Ear Hear. (2013); 14:(3)121–129

Galvin K. L., Mok M., Dowell R. C.. Perceptual benefit and functional outcomes for children using sequential bilateral cochlear implants. Ear Hear. (2007); 28:470–482

Galvin K. L., Mok M., Dowell R. C., et al. Speech detection and localization results and clinical outcomes for children receiving sequential bilateral cochlear implants before four years of age. Int J Audiol. (2008); 47:636–646

Geers A., Hayes H.. Reading, writing, and phonological processing skills of adolescents with 10 or more years of cochlear implant experience. Ear Hear. (2011); 32:49S–59S

Geers A. E.. Factors affecting the development of speech, language, and literacy in children with early cochlear implantation. Lang, Speech, Hear Serv Schools. (2002); 33:172–183

Geers A. E.. Speech, language, and reading skills after early cochlear implantation. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (2004); 130:634–638

Geers A. E., Moog J. S., Biedenstein J., et al. Spoken language scores of children using cochlear implants compared to hearing age-mates at school entry. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. (2009); 14:371–385

Geers A. E., Nicholas J. G.. Enduring advantages of early cochlear implantation for spoken language development. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2013); 56:643–655

Gordon K. A., Wong D. D., Papsin B. C.. Cortical function in children receiving bilateral cochlear implants simultaneously or after a period of interimplant delay. Otol Neurotol. (2010); 31:1293–1299

Grieco-Calub T. M., Litovsky R. Y.. Sound localization skills in children who use bilateral cochlear implants and in children with normal acoustic hearing. Ear Hear. (2010); 31:645–656

Hay-McCutcheon M. J., Kirk K. I., Henning S. C., et al. Using early language outcomes to predict later language ability in children with cochlear implants. Audiol Neurootol. (2008); 13:370–378

Hintermair M.. Parental resources, parental stress, and socioemotional development of deaf and hard of hearing children. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. (2006); 11:493–513

Hoff Ginsberg E.. The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Appl Psychol. (1998); 19:603

Holt R. F., Kirk K. I.. Speech and language development in cognitively delayed children with cochlear implants. Ear Hear. (2005); 26:132–148

Hughes K. C., Galvin K. L.. Measuring listening effort expended by adolescents and young adults with a unilateral or bilateral cochlear implants or normal hearing. Cochlear Implants Int. (2013); 14:121–129

Johnston J. C., Durieux-Smith A., Angus D., et al. Bilateral paediatric cochlear implants: A critical review. Int J Audiol. (2009); 48:601–617

Lazard D. S., Vincent C., Venail F., et al. Pre-, per- and postoperative factors affecting performance of postlinguistically deaf adults using cochlear implants: A new conceptual model over time. PLoS One. (2012); 7:e48739

Leigh J., Dettman S., Holland J.. Long-term language, speech production, and speech perception outcomes for children who received a cochlear implant at or before 12 months of age. (2008); Presented at Reflecting Connections: A Joint Conference Between New Zealand Speech-Language Therapists Association and Speech Pathology Australia Auckland, New Zealand, May 25–29, 2008.

Litovsky R. Y., Johnstone P. M., Godar S., et al. Bilateral cochlear implants in children: Localization acuity measured with minimum audible angle. Ear Hear. 2006a; 27:43–59

Litovsky R. Y., Johnstone P. M., Godar S. P.. Benefits of bilateral cochlear implants and/or hearing aids in children. Int J Audiol. 2006b; 45 Suppl 1:S78–S91

Lovett R. E., Kitterick P. T., Hewitt C. E., et al. Bilateral or unilateral cochlear implantation for deaf children: An observational study. Arch Dis Child. (2010); 95:107–112

Marschark M., Rhoten C., Fabich M.. Effects of cochlear implants on children’s reading and academic achievement. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. (2007); 12:269–282

Moeller M. P.. Early intervention and language development in children who are deaf and hard of hearing. Pediatrics. (2000); 106:E43

Moeller M. P., Tomblin J. B., Yoshinaga-Itano C., et al. Current state of knowledge: Language and literacy of children with hearing impairment. Ear Hear. (2007); 28:740–753

Moog J. S., Geers A. E.. Epilogue: Major findings, conclusions and implications for deaf education. Ear Hear. (2003); 24:(1 Suppl)121S–125S

Mukari S. Z., Ling L. N., Ghani H. A.. Educational performance of pediatric cochlear implant recipients in mainstream classes. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. (2007); 71:231–240

National Institute on Deafness and other Communication Disorders Cochlear Implants. (2011); Bethesda, MD National Institute on Deafness and other Communication Disorders

Nelson H. D., Nygren P., Walker M., et al. Screening for speech and language delay in preschool children: Systematic evidence review for the US Preventive Services Task Force. Pediatrics. (2006); 117:e298–e319

Nicholas J. G., Geers A. E.. Will they catch up? The role of age at cochlear implantation in the spoken language development of children with severe to profound hearing loss. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2007); 50:1048–1062

Niparko J. K., Tobey E. A., Thal D. J., et al. CDaCI Investigative Team Spoken language development in children following cochlear implantation. JAMA. (2010); 303:1498–1506

Nittrouer S., Caldwell A., Lowenstein J. H., et al. Emergent literacy in kindergartners with cochlear implants. Ear Hear. (2012); 33:683–697

Nittrouer S., Chapman C.. The effects of bilateral electric and bimodal electric–acoustic stimulation on language development. Trends Amplif. (2009); 13:190–205

Oshima-Takane Y., Goodz E., Derevensky J. L.. Birth order effects on early language development: Do secondborn children learn from overheard speech? Child Devel. (1996); 67:621–634

Percy-Smith L., Jensen J. H., Cayé-Thomasen P., et al. Factors that affect the social well-being of children with cochlear implants. Cochlear Implants Int. (2008); 9:199–214

Sarant J., Garrard P.. Parenting stress in children with cochlear implants: Relationships between parent stress, child language and unilateral versus bilateral implants. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. (2014); 19:85–106

Sarant J., Hughes K., Blamey P... The effect of IQ on spoken language and speech perception development in children with impaired hearing. Cochlear Implants Int. (2010); 11 Suppl 1:370–374

Sarant J. Z., Blamey P. J., Dowell R. C., et al. Variation in speech perception scores among children with cochlear implants. Ear Hear. (2001); 22:18–28

Sarant J. Z., Holt C. M., Dowell R. C., et al. Spoken language development in oral preschool children with permanent childhood deafness. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. (2009); 14:205–217

Scherf F., van Deun L., van Wieringen A., et al. Hearing benefits of second-side cochlear implantation in two groups of children. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. (2007); 71:1855–1863

Schorr E. A., Roth F., Fox N.. A comparison of the speech and language skills of children with cochlear implants and children with normal hearing. Commun Disord Quart. (2008); 29:195–210

Semel E., Wiig E., Secord W.. Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—4th Ed.—Australian Standardised Edition. (2006); Marrickville, Australia Harcourt Assessment

Sharma A., Dorman M. F., Kral A.. The influence of a sensitive period on central auditory development in children with unilateral and bilateral cochlear implants. Hear Res. (2005); 203:134–143

Sharma A., Dorman M. F., Spahr A. J.. A sensitive period for the development of the central auditory system in children with cochlear implants: Implications for age of implantation. Ear Hear. (2002); 23:532–539

Sparreboom M., Leeuw A. R., Snik A. F., et al. Sequential bilateral cochlear implantation in children: Parents’ perspective and device use. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. (2012); 76:339–344

Sparreboom M., van Schoonhoven J., van Zanten B. G., et al. The effectiveness of bilateral cochlear implants for severe-to-profound deafness in children: A systematic review. Otol Neurotol. (2010); 31:1062–1071

Spencer L. J., Barker B. A., Tomblin J. B.. Exploring the language and literacy outcomes of pediatric cochlear implant users. Ear Hear. (2003); 24:236–247

Spencer L. J., Gantz B. J., Knutson J. F.. Outcomes and achievement of students who grew up with access to cochlear implants. Laryngoscope. (2004); 114:1576–1581

Spencer P. E., Marschark M., Spencer L. J.. Cochlear Implants: Advances, Issues, and Implications. (2011); Vol. 1:2nd ed New York, NY Oxford University Press

Taylor C. L.. Early motor development is part of the resource mix for language acquisition—A commentary on Iverson’s “Developing language in a developing body: The relationship between motor development and language development.”. J Child Lang. (2010); 37:281–285

Tobey E. A., Geers A. E., Brenner C., et al. Factors associated with development of speech production skills in children implanted by age five. Ear Hear. (2003); 24:(1 Suppl)36S–45S

Tobey E. A., Thal D., Niparko J. K., et al. CDaCI Investigative Team Influence of implantation age on school-age language performance in pediatric cochlear implant users. Int J Audiol. (2013); 52:219–229

Uziel A. S., Sillon M., Vieu A., et al. Ten-year follow-up of a consecutive series of children with multichannel cochlear implants. Otol Neurotol. (2007); 28:615–628

Wechsler D.. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-III. (2002); San Antonio, TX The Psychological Corporation

Wechsler D., Naglieri J. A.. Weschler Nonverbal Scale of Ability. (2006); San Antonio, TX The Psychological Corporation

Wellen C. J.. Effects of older siblings on the language young children hear and produce. J Speech Hear Disord. (1985); 50:84–99

Zeitler D. M., Kessler M. A., Terushkin V., et al. Speech perception benefits of sequential bilateral cochlear implantation in children and adults: A retrospective analysis. Otol Neurotol. (2008); 29:314–325

Zimmerman I., Steiner V., Pond R.. Preschool Language Scale—Fourth Edition (Australian Language Adaptation). (2002); Marrickville, Australia Harcourt Assessment

Keywords:

Bilateral; Children; Cochlear implant; Spoken language; Unilateral

Back to Top | Article Outline

Supplemental Digital Content

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivitives 3.0 License, where it is permissible to download and share the work provided it is properly cited. The work cannot be changed in any way or used commercially.

Copyright © 2014 by Lippincott Williams & Wilkins

Login

Search for Similar Articles
You may search for similar articles that contain these same keywords or you may modify the keyword list to augment your search.