Skip Navigation LinksHome > February 2013 - Volume 45 - Issue 2 > Effects of Emotional Exposure on State Anxiety after Acute E...
Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise:
doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31826d5ce5
Applied Sciences

Effects of Emotional Exposure on State Anxiety after Acute Exercise

SMITH, J. CARSON

Free Access
Supplemental Author Material
Article Outline
Collapse Box

Author Information

Department of Kinesiology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD

Address for correspondence: J. Carson Smith, Ph.D., FACSM, Department of Kinesiology, University of Maryland, 2351 SPH Building, College Park, MD 20742-2611; E-mail: carson@umd.edu.

Submitted for publication March 2012.

Accepted for publication August 2012.

Supplemental digital content is available for this article. Direct URL citations appear in the printed text and are provided in the HTML and PDF versions of this article on the journal’s Web site (www.acsm-msse.org).

Collapse Box

Abstract

Purpose: Despite the well-known anxiolytic effect of acute exercise, it is unknown if anxiety reductions after acute exercise conditions survive in the face of a subsequently experienced arousing emotional exposure. The purpose of this study was to compare the effects of moderate-intensity cycle ergometer exercise to a seated rest control condition on state anxiety symptoms after exposure to a variety of highly arousing pleasant and unpleasant stimuli.

Methods: Thirty-seven healthy and normally physically active young adults completed two conditions on separate days: 1) 30 min of seated rest and 2) 30 min of moderate-intensity cycle ergometer exercise (RPE = 13; “somewhat hard”). After each condition, participants viewed 90 arousing pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral pictures from the International Affective Picture System for 30 min. State anxiety was measured before and 15 min after each condition, and again after exposure to the affective pictures.

Results: State anxiety significantly decreased from baseline to after the exercise and seated rest conditions (P = 0.003). After the emotional picture-viewing period, state anxiety significantly increased to baseline values after the seated rest condition (P = 0.001) but remained reduced after the exercise condition.

Conclusion: These findings suggest that the anxiolytic effects of acute exercise may be resistant to the potentially detrimental effects on mood after exposure to arousing emotional stimuli.

The effect of a single session of exercise to improve mood and reduce subjective symptoms of anxiety in healthy nonanxious adults has been well established (23,24,26–28). However, in many investigations, anxiety reductions after acute exercise have been shown to be similar to the effect of a “quiet rest” or similar control condition (8,17,30). Although the anxiolytic effects of acute exercise have been shown to persist longer compared with quiet rest conditions (8), this difference has not been observed consistently (17,30).

The efficacy of quiet rest conditions to improve mood suggests there may be a common anxiety-reducing factor present during exercise and rest conditions, such as a time out from stressors or other worries (1,6). Other studies have modified the exercise and rest conditions through manipulations of body temperature (44) or caffeine ingestion (25,45) and have shown that the anxiolytic effects of exercise survive these manipulations, whereas quiet rest conditions do not. This suggests a specific yet undefined effect of acute exercise, not evoked during quiet rest, which promotes durability of its anxiolytic effect. Although adaptations to repeated bouts of exercise stress have been purported to provide protection against other nonexercise stressors (i.e., the cross-stressor adaptation hypothesis [36]), these effects have been shown to be quite heterogeneous and dependent on the type of laboratory stressor used, among other factors (19). This literature has focused on the effects of exercise training or cross-sectional differences in fitness, not acute exercise, in response to laboratory stress tasks (e.g., mental arithmetic, cold-water immersion, and Stroop color-word conflict task) that lack face validity for the types of emotional stressors encountered in daily life. A key unanswered question is whether acute exercise confers short-term protection, after its cessation, against typically experienced psychological stressors or emotional provocation.

As described by Lang and Bradley (21), emotions can be defined as “action dispositions.” As such, the physiological and neural systems that govern emotional responsiveness overlap considerably with the physiological and neural systems that govern muscular activation and motor behavior. Exposure to a variety of affective picture stimuli has been shown to evoke changes in autonomic nervous system activation, including sympathetic activation and cardiac-vagal withdrawal (21), which also occur during acute exercise. Furthermore, Smith et al. (32) have reported that reactivity during repeated exposure to emotional stimuli is sensitive to self-reported state anxiety. This suggests that anxiety-reducing treatments, such as acute exercise or quiet rest, may modify the cumulative effects of exposure to emotionally arousing stimuli (10).

Very little is known, however, regarding whether acute exercise provides subsequent protection against the potentially stressful effects related to exposure to arousing emotional stimuli. Smith et al. (34) reported that acute exercise did not alter emotional reactivity during the viewing of pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant pictures. In that study, state anxiety was assessed after the exercise and rest conditions before exposure to the emotional pictures, but not after picture viewing. Thus, it is not clear if state anxiety after exercise remains reduced when exposed to arousing emotional stimuli. The aim of this study was to compare the effects of moderate-intensity exercise to a seated rest control condition on state anxiety symptoms after exposure to a variety of highly arousing pleasant and unpleasant stimuli. It was hypothesized that state anxiety would be reduced after both exercise and seated rest. Because acute exercise actively engages the physiological systems involved in emotional responsiveness, it was further hypothesized that anxiety reductions would persist after exposure to emotional stimuli for the exercise but not the seated rest condition.

Back to Top | Article Outline

METHODS

Participants

Thirty-seven healthy college students (22 men and 15 women) volunteered to complete the study. One male participant was excluded from the analysis because of missing data. The institutional review board approved the study, and written informed consent was obtained from each participant. Participants were recruited from undergraduate courses but were not offered extra course credit or payment for participation. The exclusion criteria included left-handedness, current use of antidepressant or antianxiety medication, or contraindications to exercise (e.g., heart disease, high blood pressure, and high cholesterol). No participants were excluded based on these exclusion criteria. The participants were within the normal range on trait anxiety, which measures the proneness to experience anxiety symptoms (mean ± SD, 45.7 ± 3.4; range, 38–54), and reported normal (minimal to mild) levels of depression symptoms (mean ± SD, 4.4 ± 4.7; range, 0–18).

Back to Top | Article Outline
Design

A within-subject experimental design was used. Each participant completed two conditions (seated quiet rest and moderate-intensity cycle ergometer exercise) and afterward viewed emotional pictures from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (22). The primary dependent variable was state anxiety score, measured before and after each condition and after affective picture viewing. Testing occurred on two different days within a 7-d period. The order of condition was counterbalanced across subjects, and two different picture orders were counterbalanced across testing day and condition. A power analysis based on an expected moderate correlation between repeated measures (r = 0.6) and a small effect (d = 0.25) for the interaction between condition (exercise vs seated rest) and time (pre-, post-, and post-picture viewing) detected at an α of 0.05 indicated a sample size of 36 provides statistical power of 0.95 (16).

Back to Top | Article Outline
Procedures

Participants were instructed to arrive each day prepared to exercise. On the first day, participants completed written informed consent, a health history questionnaire, the Beck Depression Inventory II (4), and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (39) in a sound-attenuated and temperature-controlled room (∼24°C ± 1°C). Then participants were taken to a different nearby room where they were informed of the experimental condition that would be performed. For the exercise condition, participants pedaled an electronically braked cycle ergometer (Corival, Lode B.V., Groningen, The Netherlands). After the seat height was adjusted, standardized instructions were provided regarding the use of the RPE scale (5,7). A 5-min warm-up and cool-down was completed at 50 W. During the exercise condition, the participant pedaled for 30 min at an intensity that corresponded with an RPE of 13 (associated with the verbal anchor “somewhat hard”). The participant was free to adjust the resistance to match the perception of “somewhat hard” throughout the 30-min session. Pedal cadence was maintained between 70 and 90 rpm. Heart rate (Polar Electro, Kempele, Finland), RPE, and work rate were recorded every 5 min. During the seated rest condition, the same procedures were followed, but the participant sat on the bike for 40 min and did not pedal.

After the exercise or rest condition, the participant was provided water ad libitum and returned to the sound-attenuated room. Fifteen minutes after the completion of the exercise or rest condition, participants completed form Y1 (state anxiety) of the STAI. The STAI-Y1 instrument is widely regarded as a reliable (2,39) (internal consistency α = 0.92; test–retest r = 0.88) and valid measure of state anxiety (39,42) defined as “a temporal cross-section in the emotional stream of life of a person, consisting of subjective feelings of tension, apprehension, nervousness, and worry, and activation or arousal of the autonomic nervous system” (37). The STAI has been used in many different disciplines (more than 3000 publications, translated to 30 languages [38]) and is the most cited anxiety instrument in the context of exercise (26,43). Participants then viewed 90 pictures from the IAPS (22) on a 14-inch color monitor located approximately 1 m away. The IAPS has been used worldwide as a set of visual stimuli to induce emotion in a laboratory setting. Repeated exposure to unpleasant IAPS pictures has been shown to induce a shift in mood and to be sensitive to baseline state anxiety (32). Among the 90 pictures used, 30 were pleasant (15 erotica and 15 babies, families, and cute animals), 30 were neutral (15 neutral people and 15 neutral objects and scenes), and 30 were unpleasant (15 threat and 15 mutilation) based on normative ratings of valence (for normative ratings, see Table 1 and Supplemental Digital Content 1, http://links.lww.com/MSS/A192. Appendix [IAPS stimuli] for IAPS numbers). The 90 pictures were arranged in three blocks of 30; each block contained 10 pictures from each valence category. The order within each block was pseudorandom in that no more than two pictures from the same category could appear consecutively. Two different picture orders were constructed and counterbalanced across testing day and experimental condition. Each picture was shown for 4 s and was followed by a 12-, 14-, or 16-s interpicture interval (mean, 14 s), which consisted of a centrally located fixation cross. The total picture-viewing time, including brief breaks between each picture block, was approximately 30 min. Participants were instructed to look at each picture the entire time it was on the monitor and to subjectively categorize each picture as pleasant, neutral, or unpleasant using (with their right hand) a three-button response pad resting on their lap. The purpose of the picture categorization task was to promote visual attention to the pictures. Immediately after the picture-viewing task (∼50 min after the cessation of the exercise and rest conditions), participants completed form Y1 of the STAI. Upon completion of the study procedures on day 2, participants rated each of the 90 pictures (hard copy, one picture per page in a standard order, self-paced) using the SAM.

Back to Top | Article Outline
Data analysis

State anxiety scores were analyzed using a 2 (condition: exercise and seated rest) × 3 (time: preexercise, postexercise, and postpicture viewing) repeated-measures ANOVA. Follow-up contrasts were computed using a general linear model and paired samples t-tests. There were no violations of the sphericity assumption as indicated by the Mauchly test of sphericity (all P > 0.2). Preliminary analysis indicated no significant effects of sex, so sex was not included as a factor in the analysis.

Back to Top | Article Outline

RESULTS

The characteristics of the sample and subjective valence and arousal ratings of the pictures are shown in Table 1. Physiologic and subjective responses associated with each condition across the different measurement periods are shown in Table 2. As expected, heart rate was significantly greater during and 15 min after the exercise compared with the rest condition. In addition, ratings of perceived exertion, leg muscle pain, and affective arousal were greater during exercise compared with during seated rest. Subjective ratings of pleasantness were greater before and 15 min after exercise compared with seated rest (see Table 2).

Table 1
Table 1
Image Tools
Table 2
Table 2
Image Tools

There were no differences in state anxiety scores before each condition, F(1,35) = 0.266, P= 0.609, hp2 = 0.008. There was a condition × time interaction, F(2, 70) = 3.029, P = 0.055, ηp2 = 0.080, and a main effect for time, F(2,70) = 4.170, P = 0.019, ηp2 = 0.106. Follow-up contrasts indicated state anxiety significantly decreased from before to 15 min after both conditions, F(1,35) = 10.003, P = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.222. However, in comparison to the 15-min post-condition measurement (before exposure to emotion) state anxiety remained decreased after emotional exposure for the exercise condition (P = 0.842) and significantly increased after emotional exposure for the seated rest condition (P = 0.001) (see Fig. 1). Furthermore, the only significant difference in state anxiety between the exercise and seated rest conditions occurred after exposure to emotional pictures when state anxiety was significantly lower after exercise compared with seated rest, F(1,35) = 9.472, P= 0.004, ηp2 = 0.213.

Figure 1
Figure 1
Image Tools
Back to Top | Article Outline

DISCUSSION

There have been few investigations regarding how acute exercise may affect responses to a subsequent exposure to emotional stimuli. The novel finding of the current study was that state anxiety was reduced after 30 min of moderate-intensity exercise and remained reduced after the viewing of arousing emotional pictures. In contrast, the anxiolytic effect of quiet rest did not persist but rather returned to baseline after emotional picture viewing.

This work extends the findings reported by Smith et al. (34) in which they found that neither low-intensity nor moderate-intensity acute exercise modified facial EMG responses during affective picture viewing. Electroencephalographic (EEG) responses during affective picture viewing were also reported to be unaffected by acute exercise or seated rest (10). Although state anxiety was reduced after both the exercise and the seated rest conditions in the Smith et al. (34) study, they did not measure state anxiety after exposure to the emotional stimuli. In the study by Crabbe et al. (10), ratings of picture pleasantness were unaffected by acute exercise; however, ratings of arousal during the viewing of unpleasant pictures were lower after exercise compared with rest, suggesting that acute exercise may have affected subjective responses specific to unpleasant pictures. In the current study, subjective ratings of the picture stimuli occurred only once at the end of the study after the pictures had been viewed twice previously. Thus, it was not possible, in a novel context, to determine whether there were differences between exercise and rest on arousal or pleasantness ratings of the pictures. Another recent study reported that attentional bias toward pleasant and unpleasant IAPS pictures was not changed after acute exercise (3), suggesting that subjective appraisal of specific single instances of emotional stimuli is unaltered after the exercise has ended. The failure of acute exercise to alter psychophysiological responsiveness during the actual viewing of an arousing pleasant or unpleasant stimulus (10,34) suggests that the neural systems that process and respond to specific instances of emotion remain undisturbed. Despite intact emotional responsiveness to briefly presented visual stimuli, the current study suggests that acute exercise may protect one from the cumulative effects of exposure to a variety of arousing emotional stimuli (32).

It is not yet clear, however, how the neural systems that process emotional stimuli are affected during moderate-intensity acute exercise. Low-intensity exercise (40% of maximal capacity) did not alter emotional responsiveness to IAPS pictures (33). However, it has been recently reported that visual attentional bias was altered during moderate-intensity exercise (41). Using the dot-probe task, it was shown that attentional bias shifted (from a neutral bias at rest) toward pleasant faces and away from unpleasant faces during moderate-intensity exercise similar to that used in the current study. This suggests that the engagement of the appetitive motivational system may be enhanced and the engagement of the aversive motivational system may be inhibited during moderate-intensity exercise. However, it is not clear if these effects on the visual attention system may persist into the postexercise period (3,33) or if acute exercise modifies attentional bias among people diagnosed with affective or anxiety disorders (41).

There is evidence to support the hypothesis that exercise may promote a persistence of postexercise anxiety reduction and a resiliency to perturbation by emotional stressors (9). For example, when the time out during exercise was blocked by having high-anxious women study academic material while they exercised, their anxiety scores still decreased by a greater magnitude after exercise compared with when they studied during a quiet rest condition (6). It has also been shown that exercise performed after caffeine ingestion (which leads to increased state anxiety) results in anxiety reduction, an effect not observed after quiet rest conditions (25,45).

There are several studies that have examined the effect of acute exercise on anxiety and panic-like symptoms in response to interoceptive sensations induced during a biological challenge. In two studies, a 35% CO2 mixture with oxygen was inhaled after acute exercise compared with after a control condition in healthy adults (15,35). Panic-like symptoms after the CO2 inhalation were attenuated after exercise compared with after rest (15), and these effects were also shown to be independent of anxiety sensitivity, negative affectivity, and cardiorespiratory fitness (35). Similar results were reported by Ströhle et al. (40), in which panic-like symptoms were reduced when cholecystokinin tetrapeptide (CCK4) was administered to healthy adults after acute exercise compared with after a rest control condition. It has also been shown that the anxiogenic effects of a 35% CO2 challenge are reduced after acute exercise in patients diagnosed with panic disorder (14). There are two important distinctions between this previous work and the current study. First, state anxiety scores have not been reported after exercise or the physiological challenge; rather, panic-like symptoms or fear have been assessed, which are considered to be different from anxiety (11). Second, air enriched with CO2 (or injection of CCK4) is a strong anxiogenic stimulus and when inhaled is a substantial threat to homeostasis. Previous acute exercise did not prevent fear or panic-like symptoms during a CO2 challenge, but it did lessen its effect. Although highly arousing affective pictures do not induce the large-scale interoceptive sensations and biological challenge that occur while breathing 35% CO2, emotional picture viewing has been shown to affect peripheral psychophysiological systems and neural indices of both defensive and appetitive activation (21,32). An emotional picture-viewing paradigm may be more representative of the breadth of repeated emotional challenges people face on a daily basis.

The timing of the anxiety measurements postexercise was based on two factors: 1) the largest effects of anxiety reductions after acute exercise have been observed 15–20 min after the cessation of exercise, not immediately after exercise (26,28); and 2) because physiological arousal is theorized to affect reactions to emotional stimuli (21), the 15-min delay also served to equate subjective arousal between the exercise and the control conditions (as shown in Table 2). The study was designed to provide exposure to emotion at a time when anxiety had been reduced and when arousal was equivalent between conditions. In this regard, any subsequent change in anxiety (or lack thereof) could not be attributable to differences in physiological arousal during emotion exposure, but only to the method by which the anxiety reduction had been realized. The similarity between the exercise and rest condition in this 15-min break preserved the internal validity of the acute exercise manipulation. If the anxiolytic effects of exercise provide a buffer against emotion provocation, as suggested here, and are to be considered useful for the management of anxiety symptoms in the face of ongoing exposure to emotional events in our environment, then one might expect these effects would persist after the exercise has ended. In this case, the anxiety reduction was maintained approximately 1 h after exercise, which is consistent with previous reports and reviews (28,31). It will be important for future studies to examine when these effects may dissipate and if these effects are observed in those diagnosed with anxiety disorders.

The STAI form Y1 was used as the measure of the multidimensional and multisystem construct “state anxiety” because this instrument has been shown repeatedly to demonstrate good reliability, a stable factor structure, and exceptional construct validity evidenced by numerous experimental manipulations and cross-sectional comparisons of clinically diagnosed patient groups (39,42). The recent work by Vautier and Pohl (42) confirmed the original four-factor structure of the STAI (state anxiety present, state anxiety absent, trait anxiety present, and trait anxiety absent). Furthermore, they confirmed that both the state and trait anxiety forms (Y1 and Y2, respectively) measure unified bipolar constructs, not separate constructs such as anxiety and serenity or somatic and cognitive anxiety. This is consistent with the criteria for diagnosis of anxiety disorders, which describes anxiety as an amalgamated multidimensional construct affecting both mind and body (13). Because of the large interindividual variability in state anxiety scores across time, the use of form Y1 to measure state anxiety change were shown by Vautier and Pohl (42) to be highly reliable. Thus, investigators and clinicians should be encouraged to continue the use of the STAI to measure changes in state anxiety in the contexts of exercise and physical activity (29).

There are several limitations of the current study. An intermixed presentation of pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant pictures was used, so it is not clear which emotional content could be more important to the effects observed. The study by Crabbe et al. (10) suggests that subjective arousal during unpleasant picture viewing may be reduced after exercise. The work by Tian and Smith (41), however, suggests visual attention toward pleasant and away from unpleasant pictures may occur during exercise. Future studies should confirm these findings and further examine distinctions between emotion processing during and after exercise as well as the effects of acute exercise on anxiety after a sustained presentation of specific affective content (32). Second, unlike previous studies that have used a “lazy boy” chair, the seated rest condition was conducted on the bike, which provided a control condition that differed from the experimental condition only in the volitional exertion required to pedal at a moderate intensity. As noted in Table 2, other than expected differences in leg muscle pain and affective arousal between the conditions attributable to the manipulation (7,34), the affective experience was very similar between the exercise and the seated rest conditions. Consistent with the affective picture-viewing literature, subjects sat in a comfortable padded chair during picture viewing after both conditions. Thus, it is unlikely that postural or affective differences between the conditions influenced the results. Finally, the sample consisted of healthy young adults in the normal range for trait and state anxiety. This study was focused on the issue of the quality, not quantity, of the anxiety reduction after experimental exercise and rest conditions. The innovation of this work in comparison with the body of literature is a manipulation of exposure to a variety of typical “real-world” emotional stimuli after the anxiolytic effects occurred. This study demonstrated that the anxiolytic effects of acute exercise survive subsequent exposure to emotional stimuli, whereas the anxiolytic effects of quiet rest do not. The demonstration of this effect in normal healthy adults is important and has broad implications for public health, mental health, and the prevention of emotion-related mental disorders in the healthy adult population. However, it is not known if these effects generalize to people diagnosed with anxiety or affective disorders, to less healthy or less physically active individuals, or to older adults.

In summary, both acute moderate-intensity exercise and seated rest were shown to reduce state anxiety scores. However, when faced with a 30-min exposure to a variety of emotional stimuli, state anxiety remained reduced after exercise but increased back to baseline after the seated rest condition. This suggests that acute exercise may enhance resilience to the cumulative effects of exposure to arousing emotional stimuli.

Morgan Shields, Qu Tian, Jennifer Payton, Timothy Haacker, and Erin Browning provided valuable assistance in the collection of these data.

No external funding supported this study. The author received compensation as a consultant on the Women’s Health Initiative study, from the University of Wisconsin for an invited lecture, and from the National Institutes of Health and the University of Kansas for reviewing grants.

These financial disclosures are unrelated to this article.

The results of this study do not constitute endorsement by the American College of Sports Medicine.

Back to Top | Article Outline

REFERENCES

1. Bahrke MS, Morgan WP. Anxiety reduction following exercise and meditation. Cognit Ther Res . 1978; 2: 322–33.

2. Barnes LLB, Harp D, Jung WS. Reliability generalization of scores on the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Educ Psychol Meas . 2002; 17: 256–66.

3. Barnes RT, Coombes SA, Armstrong NB, Higgins TJ, Janelle CM. Evaluating attentional and affective changes following an acute exercise bout using a modified dot-probe protocol. J Sports Sci . 2010; 28: 1065–76.

4. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II . San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1996. p. 35.

5. Borg G. Borg’s Perceived Exertion and Pain Scales . Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 1998. pp. 1–104.

6. Breus MJ, O’Connor PJ. Exercise-induced anxiolysis: a test of the “time out” hypothesis in high anxious females. Med Sci Sports Exerc . 1998; 30 (7): 1107–12.

7. Cook DB, O’Connor PJ, Eubanks SA, Smith JC, Lee M. Naturally occurring muscle pain during exercise: assessment and experimental evidence. Med Sci Sports Exerc . 1997; 29 (8): 999–1012.

8. Cox RH, Thomas TR, Hinton PS, Donahue OM. Effects of acute 60 and 80% VO2max bouts of aerobic exercise on state anxiety of women of different age groups across time. Res Q Exerc Sport . 2004; 75: 165–75.

9. Crabbe JB, Dishman RK. Brain electrocortical activity during and after exercise: a quantitative synthesis. Psychophysiology . 2004; 41: 563–74.

10. Crabbe JB, Smith JC, Dishman RK. Emotional & electroencephalographic responses during affective picture viewing after exercise. Physiol Behav . 2007; 90: 394–404.

11. Davis M. Are different parts of the extended amygdala involved in fear versus anxiety? Biol Psychiatry . 1998; 44: 1239–47.

12. Dozois DJ, Dobson KS, Ahnberg JL. A psychometric evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychol Assess . 1998; 10: 83–9.

13. DSM-IV-TR. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders . Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. pp. 429–84.

14. Esquivel G, Diaz-Galvis J, Schruers K, Berlanga C, Lara-Munoz C, Griez E. Acute exercise reduces the effects of a 35% CO2 challenge in patients with panic disorder. J Affect Disord . 2008; 107: 217–20.

15. Esquivel G, Schruers K, Kuipers H, Griez E. The effects of acute exercise and high lactate levels on 35% CO2 challenge in healthy volunteers. Acta Psychiatr Scand . 2002; 106: 394–7.

16. 16. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A1. G*Power Software. Version 3.1.3. Available from: http://www.psycho.uni-duesseldorf.de/aap/projects/gpower/.

17. Garvin AW, Koltyn KF, Morgan WP. Influence of acute physical activity and relaxation on state anxiety and blood lactate in untrained college males. Int J Sports Med . 1997; 18: 470–6.

18. Godin G, Shephard RJ. A simple method to assess exercise behavior in the community. Can J Appl Sport Sci . 1985; 10: 141–6.

19. Jackson EM, Dishman RK. Cardiorespiratory fitness and laboratory stress: a meta-regression analysis. Psychophysiology . 2006; 43: 57–72.

20. Lang PJ. Behavioral treatment and assessment, bio-behavioral. computer applications. In: Sidowski JB, Johnson JH, Williams TA, editors. Technology in Mental Health Care Delivery Systems . Norwood, NJ: Albex; 1980. pp. 119–37.

21. Lang PJ, Bradley MM. Emotion and the motivational brain. Biol Psychol . 2010; 84: 437–50.

22. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual. Technical Report A-8 . Gainesville, FL: 2008. pp. 1–61.

23. Morgan WP. Affective beneficence of vigorous physical activity. Med Sci Sports Exerc . 1985; 17 (1): 94–100.

24. Morgan WP, Roberts JA, Feinerman AD. Psychologic effect of acute physical activity. Arch Phys Med Rehabil . 1971; 52: 422–5 passim.

25. Motl RW, Dishman RK. Effects of acute exercise on the soleus H-reflex and self-reported anxiety after caffeine ingestion. Physiol Behav . 2004; 80: 577–85.

26. Petruzzello SJ, Landers DM, Hatfield BD, Kubitz KA, Salazar W. A meta-analysis on the anxiety-reducing effects of acute and chronic exercise. Outcomes and mechanisms. Sports Med . 1991; 11: 143–82.

27. Petruzzello SJ, Snook EM, Gliottoni RC, Motl RW. Anxiety and mood changes associated with acute cycling in persons with multiple sclerosis. Anxiety Stress Coping . 2009; 22: 297–307.

28. Raglin JS. Exercise and health, mental. Beneficial and detrimental effects. Sports Med . 1990; 9: 323–9.

29. Raglin JS. Anxiety and sport performance. Exerc Sport Sci Rev . 1992; 20: 243–74.

30. Raglin JS, Morgan WP. Influence of exercise and quiet rest on state anxiety and blood pressure. Med Sci Sports Exerc . 1987; 19 (5): 456–63.

31. Raglin JS, Turner PE, Eksten F. State anxiety and blood pressure following 30 min of leg ergometry or weight training. Med Sci Sports Exerc . 1993; 25 (9): 1044–8.

32. Smith JC, Bradley MM, Lang PJ. State anxiety and affective physiology: effects of sustained exposure to affective pictures. Biol Psychol . 2005; 69: 247–60.

33. Smith JC, O’Connor PJ. Physical activity does not disturb the measurement of startle and corrugator responses during affective picture viewing. Biol Psychol . 2003; 63: 293–310.

34. Smith JC, O’Connor PJ, Crabbe JB, Dishman RK. Emotional responsiveness after low- and moderate-intensity exercise and seated rest. Med Sci Sports Exerc . 2002; 34 (7): 1158–67.

35. Smits JA, Meuret AE, Zvolensky MJ, Rosenfield D, Seidel A. The effects of acute exercise on CO(2) challenge reactivity. J Psychiatr Res . 2009; 43: 446–54.

36. Sothmann MS, Buckworth J, Claytor RP, Cox RH, White-Welkley JE, Dishman RK. Exercise training and the cross-stressor adaptation hypothesis. Exerc Sport Sci Rev . 1996; 24: 267–87.

37. Spielberger CD. Assessment of state and trait anxiety: conceptual and methodological issues. Southern Psychologist . 1985; 2: 6–16.

38. Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: A comprehensive bibliography . Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1989.

39. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene PR, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory . Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.; 1983. p. 5.

40. Strohle A, Feller C, Onken M, Godemann F, Heinz A, Dimeo F. The acute antipanic activity of aerobic exercise. Am J Psychiatry . 2005; 162: 2376–8.

41. Tian Q, Smith JC. Attentional bias to emotional stimuli is altered during moderate- but not high-intensity exercise. Emotion . 2011; 11: 1415–24.

42. Vautier S, Pohl S. Do balanced scales assess bipolar constructs? The case of the STAI scales. Psychol Assess . 2009; 21: 187–93.

43. Wipfli BM, Rethorst CD, Landers DM. The anxiolytic effects of exercise: a meta-analysis of randomized trials and dose-response analysis. J Sport Exerc Psychol . 2008; 30: 392–410.

44. Youngstedt SD, Dishman RK, Cureton KJ, Peacock LJ. Does body temperature mediate anxiolytic effects of acute exercise? J Appl Physiol . 1993; 74: 825–31.

45. Youngstedt SD, O’Connor PJ, Crabe JB, Dishman RK. Acute exercise reduces caffeine-induced anxiogenesis. Med Sci Sports Exerc . 1998; 30 (5): 740–5.

Keywords:

AFFECT; EMOTION; IAPS; MOOD; PHYSICAL ACTIVITY; QUIET REST

Supplemental Digital Content

Back to Top | Article Outline

©2013The American College of Sports Medicine

Login

Article Tools

Images

Share

Search for Similar Articles
You may search for similar articles that contain these same keywords or you may modify the keyword list to augment your search.

Connect With Us